Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Half a Century of Stereotyping Associations Between Gender and Intellectual Ability in Films

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

A Publisher Correction to this article was published on 11 April 2019

This article has been updated

Abstract

A particularly longstanding, prevalent, and well-documented stereotype is the belief that men possess higher-level cognitive abilities than women do. This brilliance = male stereotype has been shown to be endorsed even by children as young as 6-years-old and is believed to be a factor driving the underrepresentation of women in STEM fields. Motivated by the fact that cultural products serve as a source for acquiring individual values and behaviors, we study the presence of this stereotype in a large collection of movie transcripts covering half a century of Western-world film history (n = 11,550). Concretely, we use natural language processing techniques to quantify associations between gender pronouns and high-level cognitive ability-related words. Overall, our estimates suggest that, at an aggregate level, the brilliance = male stereotype is effectively present in films and that movies specifically targeted at children contain this stereotypical association. Moreover, this pattern seems to have been quite persistent for the last 50 years.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5

Similar content being viewed by others

Change history

  • 11 April 2019

    The original version of the article unfortunately contained errors.

References

  • Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1963). Imitation of film-mediated aggressive models. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66(1), 3–11.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1981). Gender schema theory: A cognitive account of sex typing. Psychological Review, 88(4), 354–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bian, L., Leslie, S.-J., & Cimpian, A. (2017). Gender stereotypes about intellectual ability emerge early and influence children’s interests. Science, 355(6323), 389–391 http://science.sciencemag.org/content/355/6323/389. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Broverman, I. K. (1970). Sex-role stereotypes and clinical judgments of mental health. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 34(1), 1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cape, G. S. (2003). Addiction, stigma and movies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 107(3), 163–169. https://doi.org/10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00075.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlana, M. (2018). Implicit stereotypes: Evidence from teachers’ gender bias. HKS Working Papers. No. RWP18–034. https://ssrn.com/abstract=3274717. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Chen, H., Cohen, P., & Chen, S. (2010). How big is a big odds ratio? Interpreting the magnitudes of odds ratios in epidemiological studies. Communications in Statistics: Simulation and Computation, 39(4), 860–864. https://doi.org/10.1080/03610911003650383.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheryan, S., Plaut, V. C., Handron, C., & Hudson, L. (2013). The stereotypical computer scientist: Gendered media representations as a barrier to inclusion for women. Sex Roles, 69(1–2), 58–71 https://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s11199-013-0296-x. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Church, K. W., & Hanks, P. (1990). Word association norms, mutual information, and lexicography. Computational Linguistics, 16(1), 22–29 http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=89086.89095. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Collins, R. L. (2011). Content analysis of gender roles in media: Where are we now and where should we go? Sex Roles, 64(3–4), 290–298. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-010-9929-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crowley, K., Callanan, M. A., Tenenbaum, H. R., & Allen, E. (2001). Parents explain more often to boys than to girls during shared scientific thinking. Psychological Science, 12(3), 258–261. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cvencek, D., Meltzoff, A. N., & Greenwald, A. G. (2011). Math–gender stereotypes in elementary school children. Child Development, 82(3), 766–779. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01529.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dimnik, T., & Felton, S. (2006). Accountant stereotypes in movies distributed in North America in the twentieth century. Accounting, Organizations and Society, 31(2), 129–155. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.aos.2004.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flicker, E. (2003). Between brains and breasts—women scientists in fiction film: On the marginalization and sexualization of scientific competence. Public Understanding of Science, 12(3), 307–318.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Furnham, A., Reeves, E., & Budhani, S. (2002). Parents think their sons are brighter than their daughters: Sex differences in parental self-estimations and estimations of their children’s multiple intelligences. The Journal of Genetic Psychology, 163(1), 24–39. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221320209597966.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gaertner, S. L., & McLaughlin, J. P. (1983). Racial stereotypes: Associations and ascriptions of positive and negative characteristics. Social Psychology Quarterly, 46(1), 23–30 http://www.jstor.org/stable/3033657. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Garg, N., Schiebinger, L., Jurafsky, D., & Zou, J. (2018). Word embeddings quantify 100 years of gender and ethnic stereotypes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(16), E3635–E3644.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geena Davis Institute on Gender in Media. (2015). The reel truth: Women aren’t seen or heard. An automated analysis of gender representation in popular films. Retrieved from https://seejane.org/research-informs-empowers/data/. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Haddock, C. K., Rindskopf, D., & Shadish, W. R. (1998). Using odds ratios as effect sizes for meta-analysis of dichotomous data: A primer on methods and issues. Psychological Methods, 3(3), 339–353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jurafsky, D., & Martin, J. H. (2000). Speech and language processing: An introduction to natural language processing, computational linguistics and speech recognition (3rd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirkcaldy, B., Noack, P., Furnham, A., & Siefen, G. (2007). Parental estimates of their own and their children’s intelligence. European Psychologist, 12(3), 173–180. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040.12.3.173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koivula, N. (1999). Gender stereotyping in televised media sport coverage. Sex Roles, 41(7–8), 589–604. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018899522353.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamoreaux, M., & Morling, B. (2012). Outside the head and outside individualism-collectivism: Further meta-analyses of cultural products. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 43(2), 299–327. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110385234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Larivière, V., Ni, C., Gingras, Y., Cronin, B., & Sugimoto, C. R. (2013). Bibliometrics: Global gender disparities in science. Nature News, 504(7479), 211–213.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lenton, A. P., Sedikides, C., & Bruder, M. (2009). A latent semantic analysis of gender stereotype-consistency and narrowness in American English. Sex Roles, 60(3), 269–278. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9534-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, S.-J., Cimpian, A., Meyer, M., & Freeland, E. (2015). Expectations of brilliance underlie gender distributions across academic disciplines. Science, 347(6219), 262–265. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1261375.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Long, M., Steinke, J., Applegate, B., Knight Lapinski, M., Johnson, M. J., & Ghosh, S. (2010). Portrayals of male and female scientists in television programs popular among middle school-age children. Science Communication, 32(3), 356–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. E., & Wilson, W. C. (1957). Identification and observational learning from films. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 55(1), 76–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K, The Google Books Team, … Aiden, E. L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331(6014), 176–182. http://science.sciencemag.org/content/331/6014/176. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Morling, B., & Lamoreaux, M. (2008). Measuring culture outside the head: A meta-analysis of individualism—collectivism in cultural products. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 12(3), 199–221. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868308318260.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nosek, B. A., Smyth, F. L., Sriram, N., Lindner, N. M., Devos, T., Ayala, A, … Greenwald, A. G. (2009). National differences in gender–science stereotypes predict national sex differences in science and math achievement. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(26), 10593–10597. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0809921106.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olivier, J., & Bell, M. L. (2013). Effect sizes for 2 × 2 contingency tables. PLoS ONE, 8(3), 1–7. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058777.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orthia, L. A., & Morgain, R. (2016). The gendered culture of scientific competence: A study of scientist characters in doctor Who 1963–2013. Sex Roles, 75(3–4), 79–94. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-016-0597-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramakrishna, A., Martìnez, V. R., Malandrakis, N., Singla, K., & Narayanan, S. (2017). Linguistic analysis of differences in portrayal of movie characters. In Proceedings of the 55th annual meeting of the association for computational linguistics (Vol. 1, pp. 1669–1678). Association for Computational Linguistics. Retrieved from http://www.aclweb.org/anthology/P17-1153. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Reuben, E., Sapienza, P., & Zingales, L. (2014). How stereotypes impair women’s careers in science. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(12), 4403–4408. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1314788111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schütze, H., & Pedersen, J. (1993). A vector model for syntagmatic and paradigmatic relatedness. Proceedings of the 9th Annual Conference of the UW Centre for the New OED and Text Research, Vancouver, (pp 104–113).

  • Schwartz, H. A., Eichstaedt, J. C., Kern, M. L., Dziurzynski, L., Ramones, S. M., Agrawal, M., … Ungar, L. H. (2013). Personality, gender, and age in the language of social media: The open-vocabulary approach. PLoS ONE, 8(9), 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0073791.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shumow, L., & Schmidt, J. A. (2013). Enhancing adolescents’ motivation for science. Thousand Oaks: Corwin Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Silverstein, B., Perdue, L., Peterson, B., & Kelly, E. (1986). The role of the mass media in promoting a thin standard of bodily attractiveness for women. Sex Roles, 14(9–10), 519–532. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF0028745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, D. G., Rosenstein, J. E., Nikolov, M. C., & Chaney, D. A. (2018). The power of language: Gender, status, and agency in performance evaluations. Sex Roles, 80, 159–171. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-018-0923-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smyth, F. L., & Nosek, B. A. (2015). On the gender–science stereotypes held by scientists: Explicit accord with gender-ratios, implicit accord with scientific identity. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1–19 https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00415. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Steele, C. (1997). A threat in the air. How stereotypes shape intellectual identity and performance. The American Psychologist, 52(6), 613–629. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066X.52.6.613.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Steinke, J. (2005). Cultural representations of gender and science: Portrayals of female scientists and engineers in popular films. Science Communication, 27(1), 27–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547005278610.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinke, J., & Tavarez, P. M. P. (2018). Cultural representations of gender and stem: Portrayals of female stem characters in popular films 2002-2014. International Journal of Gender, Science and Technology, 9(3), 244–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Steinke, J., Lapinski, M. K., Crocker, N., Zietsman-Thomas, A., Williams, Y., Evergreen, S. H., … Kuchibhotla, S. (2007). Assessing media influences on middle school–aged children’s perceptions of women in science using the draw-a-scientist test (dast). Science Communication, 29(1), 35–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinke, J., Applegate, B., Lapinski, M., Ryan, L., & Long, M. (2012). Gender differences in adolescents’ wishful identification with scientist characters on television. Science Communication, 34(2), 163–199. https://doi.org/10.1177/1075547011410250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stoet, G., & Geary, D. C. (2018). The gender-equality paradox in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics education. Psychological Science, 29(4), 581–593. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797617741719.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Storage, D., Horne, Z., Cimpian, A., & Leslie, S.-J. (2016). The frequency of “brilliant” and “genius” in teaching evaluations predicts the representation of women and African Americans across fields. PLoS ONE, 11(3), 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0150194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tan, A.-L., Jocz, J. A., & Zhai, J. (2017). Spiderman and science: How students’ perceptions of scientists are shaped by popular media. Public Understanding of Science, 26(5), 520–530.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2003). Parent-child conversations about science: The socialization of gender inequities? Developmental Psychology, 39(1), 34–47. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.1.34.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Twenge, J. M., Campbell, W. K., & Gentile, B. (2012). Male and female pronoun use in U.S. books reflects women’s status, 1900–2008. Sex Roles, 67(9), 488–493. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-012-0194-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Upson, S., & Friedman, L. F. (2012). Where are all the female geniuses? Scientific American Mind, 23(5), 63–65 http://www.jstor.org/stable/24942297. Accessed 1 Feb 2019.

  • Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1982). Measuring sex stereotypes: A thirty-nation study. Beverly Hills: Sage Publications, Inc..

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank OpenSubtitles.org administrators (www.opensubtitles.org) for their invaluable help in gathering subtitle data. We also thank Luciana Ferrer, Carlos Diuk, Diego Fernández Slezak, Julieta Schiro and Agustín Gravano for fruitful discussions and useful comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ramiro H. Gálvez.

Ethics declarations

Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no potential conflicts of interest.

Research Involving Human Participants and/or Animals

The research did not involved human participants and/or animal.

Informed Consent

The research did not require and informed consent.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

The original version of this article was revised: In Fig. 3, panel b, the gray shades were captured on top of the lines instead as a background.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Gálvez, R.H., Tiffenberg, V. & Altszyler, E. Half a Century of Stereotyping Associations Between Gender and Intellectual Ability in Films. Sex Roles 81, 643–654 (2019). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01019-x

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-019-01019-x

Keywords

Navigation