Skip to main content
Log in

A Latent Semantic Analysis of Gender Stereotype-Consistency and Narrowness in American English

  • Original Article
  • Published:
Sex Roles Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Using latent semantic analysis, we examined gender stereotypes in American English by submitting over 100 masculine, neutral, and feminine role-words and trait-words to pair-wise semantic similarity comparisons with masculine (man, he, him) and feminine (woman, she, her) referents separately. We expected to find: (a) Stereotyping—roles and traits would be more semantically similar to the ostensible ‘matching’ than ‘mismatching’ gender category referent; (b) Categorical narrowness—both categories would be less semantically similar to counterstereotypical than to neutral or stereotypical characteristics; but this would be especially so for the male category, indicating its relatively greater narrowness. Results supported these hypotheses, but only among role-words. American English reflects and reinforces gender stereotypes regarding gender roles at a level beyond that recognized previously.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American National Corpus. (2007). Retrieved May 19, 2008, from http://americannationalcorpus.org/SecondRelease/frequency2.html.

  • Anderson, C. A., & Sedikides, C. (1991). Contributions of a typological approach to associationistic and dimensional views of person perception. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60, 203–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bem, S. L. (1974). The measurement of psychological androgyny. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 42, 155–162.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berry, M. W., Dumais, S. T., & Letsche, T. A. (1995, December). Computational methods for intelligent information access. In Paper presented at the Proceedings of Supercomputing. San Diego, CA. Retrieved February 23, 2007 from http://www.cs.utk.edu/∼berry/sc95/sc95.html.

  • Beyer, S. (1999). The accuracy of academic gender stereotypes. Sex Roles, 40, 787–813.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Biernat, M. (1991). Gender stereotypes and the relationship between masculinity and femininity: A developmental analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 61, 351–365.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, I. V., & Banaji, M. R. (1996). Automatic and controlled processes in stereotype priming. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 1142–1163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blair, I. V., Ma, J. E., & Lenton, A. P. (2001). Imagining stereotypes away: The moderation of implicit stereotypes through mental imagery. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81, 828–841.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bosson, J. K., Prewitt-Freilino, J. L., & Taylor, J. N. (2005). Role rigidity: A problem of identity misclassification? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 552–565.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • British National Corpus (2001). The British National Corpus. version 2 (BNC World). Distributed by Oxford University Computing Services on behalf of the BNC Consortium. Retrieved February 21, 2007, from http://view.byu.edu/.

  • Burn, S. M. (2000). Heterosexuals’ use of “fag” and “queer” to deride one another: A contributor to heterosexism and stigma. Journal of Homosexuality, 40, 1–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Campbell, R. S., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2003). The secret life of pronouns: Flexibility in writing style and physical health. Psychological Science, 14, 60–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. B. (1956). Introduction. In J. B. Carroll (Ed.), B. L. Whorf, Language, thought and reality: Selected writings of Benjamin Lee Whorf. Cambridge, MA: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deerwester, S. C., Dumais, S. T., Landauer, T. K., Furnas, G. W., & Harshman, R. A. (1990). Indexing by latent semantic analysis. Journal of the American Society for Information Science American Society for Information Science, 41, 391–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dennis, S., Landauer, T., Kintsch, W., & Quesada, J. (2003). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Slides from the tutorial given at the 25th Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Boston. Retrieved February 27, 2007 from http://lsa.colorado.edu/%7Esimon/LSATutorial.pdf.

  • Diekman, A. B., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Stereotypes as dynamic constructs: Women and men of the past, present, and future. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 26, 1171–1181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., & Steffen, V. J. (1984). Gender stereotypes stem from the distribution of women and men into social roles. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 735–754.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eagly, A. H., Wood, W., & Diekman, A. B. (2000). Social role theory of sex differences and similarities: A current appraisal. . In T. Eckes & H. M. Trautner (Eds.), The developmental social psychology of gender (pp. 123–174). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ernst, S. B. (1995). Gender issues in books for children and young adults. In S. Lehr (Ed.), Battling dragons: Issues and controversy in children’s literature (pp. 66–78). Portsmouth, NH: Heinemann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gastil, J. (1990). Generic pronouns and sexist language: The oxymoronic character of masculine generics. Sex Roles, 23, 629–643.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 1–55.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Glenberg, A. M., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Symbol grounding and meaning: A comparison of high-dimensional and embodied theories of meaning. Journal of Memory and Language, 43, 379–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Global Reach (2004). Global Internet statistics: Sources and references. Retrieved July 22, 2008 from http://www.glreach.com/globstats/refs.php3.

  • Graddol, D. (2000). The future of English? A guide to forecasting the popularity of the English language in the 21st century. London, England: The British Council. Retrieved July 22, 2008 from http://www.britishcouncil.org/de/learning-elt-future.pdf.

  • Helgeson, V. S. (1994). Prototypes and dimensions of masculinity and femininity. Sex Roles, 31, 653–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hense, R. L., Penner, L. A., & Nelson, D. A. (1995). Implicit memory for age stereotypes. Social Cognition, 13, 399–415.

    Google Scholar 

  • Judd, C. M., & Park, B. (1993). Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes. Psychological Review, 100, 109–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. T. (1986). Norm theory: Comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kennison, S. M., & Trofe, J. L. (2003). Comprehending pronouns: A role for word-specific gender stereotype information. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 32, 355–378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kontostathis, A., & Pottenger, W. M. (2002). A mathematical view of latent semantic indexing: Tracing term co-occurrences. Technical Report, LU-CSE-02-006, Department of Computer Science and Engineering, Lehigh University. Retrieved 16 September, 2007 from http://www3.lehigh.edu/engineering/cse/research/reports/2002.asp.

  • Laham, D. (1997). Latent semantic analysis approaches to categorization. In M. G. Shafto & P. Langley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (p. 979). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum Retrieved March 1, 2007 from: http://lsa.colorado.edu/.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K. (2002). On the computational basis of learning and cognition: Arguments from LSA. In N. Ross (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation, 41 (pp. 43–84). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., & Dumais, S. T. (1997). A solution to Plato’s problem: The Latent semantic analysis theory of the acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge. Psychological Review, 104, 211–240.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., Foltz, P. W., & Laham, D. (1998a). Introduction to latent semantic analysis. Discourse Processes, 25, 259–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., & Derr, M. (2004). From paragraph to graph: Latent semantic analysis for information visualization. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 101, 5214–5219.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., & Foltz, P. W. (1998b). Learning human-like knowledge by singular value decomposition: A progress report. In M. I. Jordan, M. J. Kearns, & S. A. Solla (Eds.), Advances in neural information processing systems, 10 (pp. 45–51). Cambridge: MIT.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landauer, T. K., Laham, D., Rehder, B., & Schreiner, M. E. (1997). How well can passage meaning be derived without using word order? A comparison of latent semantic analysis and humans. In M. G. Shafto & P. Langley (Eds.), Proceedings of the 19th annual meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 412–417). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lemaire, B., & Denhière, G. (2006). Effects of high-order co-occurrences on word semantic similarity. Current Psychology Letters: Behaviour, Brain & Cognition, 18.

  • Lenton, A. P., & Webber, L. (2006). Cross-sex friendships: Who has more? Sex Roles, 54, 809–820.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lippa, R., & Connelly, S. (1990). Gender diagnosticity: A new Bayesian approach to gender-related individual differences. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1051–1065.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maass, A., Salvi, D., Arcuri, L., & Semin, G. (1989). Language use in intergroup contexts: The linguistic intergroup bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57, 981–993.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Maccoby, E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prentice, D. A., & Carranza, E. (2002). What women and men should be, shouldn’t be, are allowed to be, and don’t have to be: The contents of prescriptive gender stereotypes. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 26, 269–281.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Queller, S. (2002). Stereotype change in a recurrent network. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 6, 295–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Queller, S., & Smith, E. R. (2002). Subtyping versus bookkeeping in stereotype learning and change: Connectionist simulations and empirical findings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 300–313.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rodler, C., Kirchler, E., & Holzl, E. (2001). Gender stereotypes of leaders: An analysis of the contents of obituaries from 1974 to 1998. Sex Roles, 45, 827–844.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schiffman, S. S., Reynolds, M. L., & Young, F. W. (1981). Introduction to multidimensional scaling: Theory, methods, and applications. San Diego, CA: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & Conrey, F. R. (2007). Mental representations as states not things: Implications for implicit and explicit measurement. In B. Wittenbrink, & N. Schwarz (Eds.), Implicit measures of attitudes (pp. 247–264). New York, NY: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Smith, E. R., & DeCoster, J. (1998). Knowledge acquisition, accessibility, and use in person perception and stereotyping: Simulation with a recurrent connectionist network. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 21–35.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Touchstone Applied Science & Associates (TASA) Inc. (2006). Word frequency guide. Retrieved January 22, 2008, from http://www.tasaliteracy.com/wfg/wfg-main.html.

  • Watt, N., & Gow, D. (March 25, 2006). Chirac vows to fight the growing use of English. Guardian News and Media Limited 2008. Retrieved July 22, 2008 from http://www.guardian.co.uk/world/2006/mar/25/france.eu.

  • Wolfe, M. B. W., & Goldman, S. R. (2003). Use of latent semantic analysis for predicting psychological phenomena: Two issues and proposed solutions. Behavior Research Methods, 35, 22–31.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alison P. Lenton.

Additional information

The research reported herein was supported by Economic and Social Research Council grant #RES-000-22-0253.

Appendix

Appendix

The feminine, neutral, and masculine role- and trait-words submitted to latent semantic analysis

 

Roles

Traits

Feminine

Beautician

Affectionate

Caregiver

Caring

Cheerleader

Cheerful

Dancer

Compassionate

Decorator

Delicate

Designer

Emotional

Dietician

Flatterable

Florist

Gentle

Hairdresser

Gossipy

Homemaker

Humble

Housekeeper

Loyal

Model

Moody

Nanny

Nagging

Nurse

Polite

Receptionist

Sensitive

Stylist

Shy

Typist

Sympathetic

Tender

Understanding

Warm

Neutral

Assistant

Adaptable

Cashier

Candid

Clerk

Childlike

Doctor

Conceited

Editor

Conscientious

Lawyer

Conventional

Poet

Earnest

Reporter

Forward

Servant

Friendly

Worker

Gullible

Happy

Helpful

Inefficient

Irrational

Jealous

Likable

Outspoken

Reliable

Ridiculous

Secretive

Sincere

Solemn

Stubborn

Tactful

Theatrical

Truthful

Unpredictable

Unsystematic

Yielding

Masculine

Architect

Aggressive

Carpenter

Ambitious

Coach

Analytical

Contractor

Arrogant

Detective

Assertive

Electrician

Athletic

Engineer

Authoritative

Farmer

Bold

Firefighter

Capable

Gambler

Charismatic

Inventor

Competitive

Machinist

Confident

Mechanic

Crude

Officer

Daring

Physicist

Decisive

Pilot

Dominant

Programmer

Forceful

Rancher

Independent

Sheriff

Individualistic

Soldier

Reckless

Unyielding

Vulgar

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lenton, A.P., Sedikides, C. & Bruder, M. A Latent Semantic Analysis of Gender Stereotype-Consistency and Narrowness in American English. Sex Roles 60, 269–278 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9534-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11199-008-9534-z

Keywords

Navigation