Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Dimensions of discourse level oral language skills and their relation to reading comprehension and written composition: an exploratory study

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We examined the relations of discourse-level oral language skills [i.e., listening comprehension, and oral retell and production of narrative texts (oral retell and production hereafter)] to reading comprehension and written composition. Korean-speaking first grade students (N = 97) were assessed on listening comprehension, oral retell and production, word reading, spelling, handwriting fluency as well as reading comprehension and written composition. Listening comprehension, and oral retell and production tasks were best described as having a bi-factor structure, capturing a general discourse-level oral language construct as well as unique listening comprehension and oral retell constructs which are not explained by the general discourse-level oral language skill. The general discourse-level oral language skill was related to reading comprehension whereas listening comprehension and oral retell were not. Although positive in direction, the general discourse-level oral language skill did not reach the conventional statistical significance in relation to writing quality. These findings suggest that the general discourse-level oral language skill underlying listening comprehension, and oral retell and production tasks is important for reading comprehension, and unique listening comprehension and oral retell skills that are not subsumed to the general discourse-level oral language skill do not independently contribute to reading comprehension.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. A post hoc statistical power analysis suggests that the sample size of 97 allows detecting effect size of .21 at the power of .80 and p value of .05 level for the given number of observed variables and latent variables used in the present study.

  2. We expected children to be familiar with the word, goblin, because of its high frequency in Korean folk tales.

  3. Prior to examining the four alternative models, confirmatory factor analysis was conducted to examine whether oral retell and production tasks are best considered as separable skills. Both single factor model and two factor model had excellent fit to the data. In addition, the Chi square different test was on the cut point (p = .05) and differences in AIC (2.11) and BIC (3.04) values between two alternative models were small. Therefore, for parsimony, oral retell and production were considered as a single construct in confirmatory factor models of four alternative models.

References

  • Adlof, S., Catts, H., & Little, T. (2006). Should the simple view of reading include a fluency component? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 933–958.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apel, K., & Apel, L. (2011). Identifying intraindividual differences in students’ written language abilities. Topics in Language Disorders, 31, 54–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes, A., Kim, Y.-S., & Phillips, B. (2014). The relations of proper character introduction to narrative quality and listening comprehension for young children from high poverty schools. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 1189–1205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2010). Listening comprehension, oral expression, reading comprehension, and written expression: Related yet unique language systems in grades 1, 3, 5, and 7. Journal of Educational Psychology, 102, 635–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., Graham, S., & Richards, T. (2002). Writing and reading: Connections between language by hand and language by eye. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 39–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Amtmann, D. (2003). Preventing written expression disabilities through early and continuing assessment and intervention for handwriting and/or spelling problems: Research into practice. In H. L. Swanson, K. R. Harris, & S. Graham (Eds.), Handbook of learning disabilities (pp. 345–363). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., & Swanson, H. L. (1994). Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of the development of skilled writing. In E. Butterfield (Ed.), Children’s writing: Toward a process theory of development of skilled writing (pp. 57–81). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press [reproduced in The Learning and Teaching of Reading and Writing (by R. Stainthorp). Wiley, 2006].

  • Berninger, V. W., Vaughan, K., Abbott, R., Abbott, S., Brooks, A., Rogan, L., … Graham, S. (1997). Treatment of handwriting fluency problems in beginning writing: Transfer from handwriting to composition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 652–666.

  • Berninger, V. W., & Winn, W. D. (2006). Implications of advancements in brain research and technology for writing development, writing instruction, and educational evolution. In C. MacArthur, S. Graham, & J. Fitzgerald (Eds.), Handbook of writing research (pp. 96–114). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K. (2007). Syntactic awareness and reading ability: Is there any evidence for a special relationship? Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 679–694.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal ability, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1995). Oral and written language scales. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carrow-Woolfolk, E. (1999). Comprehensive assessment of spoken language. Bloomington, MN: Pearson Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catts, H., Adlof, S., & Ellis Weismer, S. (2006). Language deficits in poor comprehenders: A case for the simple view of reading. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 278–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, F. F., West, S. G., & Sousa, K. H. (2006). A comparison of bifactor and second-order models of the quality of life. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 41, 189–225.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curenton, S. M., Craig, M. J., & Flanigan, N. (2008). Use of decontextualized talk across story contexts: How oral storyretelling and emergent reading can scaffold children’s development. Early Education and Development, 19, 161–187.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dickinson, D. K., & Tabors, P. O. (1991). Early literacy: Linkages between home, school and literacy achievement at age five. Journal of Research in Childhood Education, 6, 30–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., Altoè, G., & Levorato, M. C. (2009). Listening comprehension in preschoolers: The role of memory. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 27, 935–951.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gillam, R. B., & Pearson, N. A. (2004). Test of narrative language. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., Hoover, W., & Peterson, C. (1996). Some observations on a simple view of reading. In C. Cornoldi & J. Oakhill (Eds.), Reading comprehension difficulties: Processes and intervention. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P., & Tunmer, W. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 6–10.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Abbott, S. P., & Whitaker, D. (1997). Role of mechanics in composing of elementary school students: A new methodological approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 170–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graham, S., Harris, K., & Hebert, M. A. (2011). Informing writing: The benefits of formative assessment. A Carnegie Corporation Time to Act report. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.

  • Hagtvet, B. E. (2003). Listening comprehension and reading comprehension in poor decoders: Evidence for the importance of syntactic and semantic skills as well as phonological skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 505–539.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indices in covariance structure analysis. Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, R. M., & Aaron, P. G. (2000). The component model of reading: Simple view of reading made a little more complex. Reading Psychology, 21, 85–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, R. M., Tao, S., Aaron, P. G., & Quiroz, G. (2012). Cognitive component of componential model of reading applied to different orthographies. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 5, 480–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juel, C., Griffith, P. L., & Gough, P. B. (1986). Acquisition of literacy: A longitudinal study of children in first and second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 78, 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., Bowles, R., Pence, K., & Gosse, C. (2010). A scalable tool for assessing children’s language abilities within a narrative context: The NAP (narrative assessment protocol). Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 25, 218–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Ukrainetz, T. A., Eisenberg, S. L., & Gillam, R. B. (2006). The index of narrative microstructure: A clinical tool for analyzing school-age children’s narrative performances. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 15, 177–191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kang, J. Y. S., Kim, Y.-S., & Pan, B. A. (2009). Five-year-olds’ book talk and story retelling: Contributions of mother–child joint bookreading. First Language, 29, 243–265.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. M., Betjemann, R. S., & Olson, R. K. (2008). Reading comprehension tests vary in the skills they assess: Differential dependence on decoding and oral comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 281–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kendeou, P., van den Broek, P., White, M. J., & Lynch, J. S. (2009). Predicting reading comprehension in early elementary school: The independent contributions of oral language and decoding skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 765–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2009). The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: The relationship between letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness and their relative contribution to literacy skills. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 907–931.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2010). Componential skills in early spelling development in Korean. Scientific Studies of Reading, 14, 137–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2011a). Proximal and distal predictors of reading comprehension: Evidence from young Korean readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 167–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2011b). Considering linguistic and orthographic features in early literacy acquisition: Evidence from Korean. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36, 177–189.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (in press). Language and cognitive predictors of text comprehension: Evidence from multivariate analysis. Child Development.

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Puranik, C., Sidler, J. F., Greulich, L., & Wagner, R. K. (2011). Componential skills of beginning writing: An exploratory study. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 517–525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Sidler, J. F., & Greulich, L. (2013a). Language, literacy, attentional behaviors, and instructional quality predictors of written composition for first graders. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28, 461–469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Sidler, J. F., Greulich, L., & Puranik, C. (2014a). Evaluating the dimensionality of first grade written composition. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 57, 199–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Al Otaiba, S., Wanzek, J., & Gatlin, B. (in press). Towards an understanding of dimension, predictors, and gender gaps in written composition. Journal of Educational Psychology.

  • Kim, Y.-S., Park, C., & Park, Y. (2013b). Is academic language use a separate dimension in beginning writing? Evidence from Korean children. Learning and Individual Differences, 27, 8–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Park, C., & Wagner, R. K. (2014b). Is oral/text reading fluency a “bridge” to reading comprehension? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 79–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., & Petscher, Y. (2013). Considering word characteristics for spelling accuracy: Evidence from Korean-speaking children. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 80–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., & Phillips, B. (2014). Cognitive correlates of listening comprehension. Reading Research Quarterly, 49, 269–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S., Wagner, R., & Lopez, D. (2012). Developmental relations between reading fluency and reading comprehension: A longitudinal study from grade 1 to grade 2. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 113, 93–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kline, B. (2005). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kozlow, M., & Bellamy, P. (2004). Experimental study on the impact of the 6 + 1 trait writing model on student achievement in writing. http://educationnorthwest.org/webfm_send/134

  • Lepola, J., Lynch, J., Laakkonen, E., Silvén, M., & Niemi, P. (2012). The role of inference making and other language skills in the development of narrative listening comprehension in 4- to 6-year old children. Reading Research Quarterly, 47, 259–282.

  • McCabe, A., & Peterson, C. (Eds.). (1991). Developing narrative structure. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCabe, A., & Rollins, P. R. (1994). Assessment of preschool narrative skills. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology: A Journal of Clinical Practice, 3, 45–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGee, L. M., & Richgels, D. J. (1996). Literacy’s beginnings: Supporting young readers and writers (2nd ed.). Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Moats, L. (2000). Speech to print: Language essentials for teachers. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (2012). Mplus. Los Angeles: Muthén and Muthén.

    Google Scholar 

  • Northwest Regional Educational Laboratory (2011). 6 + 1 Trait ® writing. Retrieved from http://educationnorthwest.org/traits

  • Olinghouse, N. G., & Graham, S. (2009). The relationship between discourse knowledge and the writing performance of elementary-grade students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 37–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pankratz, M. E., Plante, E., Vance, R., & Insalaco, D. M. (2007). The diagnostic and predictive validity of the Renfrew bus story. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 38, 390–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Mousaki, A., Sideridis, G. D., Kotsolakou, A., & Simos, P. G. (2013). The role of vocabulary in the context of the simple view of reading. Reading & Writing Quarterly: Overcoming Learning Difficulties, 29, 168–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. K., & Vaughn, S. (2012). Retell as an indicator of reading comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 16, 187–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. K., Vaughn, S., & Petscher, Y. (2012). The validity of a holistically scored retell protocol for determining the reading comprehension of middle school students. Learning Disability Quarterly, 35, 76–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scott, C., & Windsor, J. (2000). General language performance measures in spoken and written narrative and expository discourse in school-age children with language learning disabilities. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 43, 324–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shapiro, L. R., & Hudson, J. A. (1991). Tell me a make-believe story: Coherence and cohesion in young children’s picture-elicited narratives. Developmental Psychology, 27, 960–974.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Porche, M. V., Tabors, P. O., & Harris, S. R. (2007). Is literacy enough? Pathways to academic success for adolescents. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, H.-M. (1999). The Korean language. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I. (1980). The Korean writing system: An alphabet? A syllabary? A logography? In P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad, & H. Bouma (Eds.), Processing of visible language (pp. 67–82). New York: Plenum Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Tomblin, J. B., & Zhang, X. Y. (2006). The dimensionality of language ability in school-age children. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research, 49, 1193–1208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tompkins, V., Guo, Y., & Justice, L. M. (2013). Inference generation, story comprehension, and language in the preschool years. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 26, 403–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunmer, W. E., & Chapman, J. W. (2012). The simple view of reading redux: Vocabulary knowledge and the independent components hypothesis. Learning of Learning Disabilities, 45, 453–466.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ukrainetz, T. A., Justice, L. M., Kaderavek, J. N., Eisenberg, S. L., Gillam, R. B., & Harm, H. M. (2005). The development of expressive elaboration in fictional narratives. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 1363–1377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Tunmer, W. E., Jaccard, J., & Chen, S. (2007). Components of reading ability: Multivariate evidence for a convergent skills model of reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 3–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Puranik, C. S., Foorman, B., Foster, E., Tschinkel, E., & Kantor, P. T. (2011). Modeling the development of written language. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal., 24, 203–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W., McGrew, K. S., & Mather, N. (2001). Woodcock–Johnson III tests of achievement. Itasca, IL: Riverside.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by Grant NRF-2010-330-B00299 from the National Research Foundation of Korea. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Research Foundation of Korea. We would like to thank participating children and teachers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Suk Grace Kim.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kim, YS.G., Park, C. & Park, Y. Dimensions of discourse level oral language skills and their relation to reading comprehension and written composition: an exploratory study. Read Writ 28, 633–654 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9542-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9542-7

Keywords

Navigation