Skip to main content
Log in

The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: the relationship between letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness and their relative contribution to literacy skills

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relative contribution of letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness to literacy skills and the relationship between letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness, using data from Korean-speaking preschoolers. The results revealed that although both letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness made unique contributions to literacy skills (i.e., word reading, pseudoword reading, and spelling), letter-name knowledge played a more important role than phonological awareness in literacy acquisition in Korean. Letter-name knowledge explained appreciably greater amount of variance and had larger effect sizes in literacy skills. Furthermore, children with greater syllable, body (e.g., segmenting cat into ca-t), and phoneme awareness had higher levels of letter-name knowledge. In particular, children’s syllable awareness and body awareness were positively associated with their letter-name knowledge, even after controlling for children’s phoneme awareness. These results suggest that Korean children’s awareness of larger phonological units (i.e., syllable and body) in addition to phoneme awareness may mediate the relationship between letter-name knowledge and literacy acquisition in Korean, in contrast with previous findings in English that have demonstrated a positive relationship only between phoneme awareness and letter-name knowledge, and the hypothesis that phoneme awareness mediates the relationship between letter-name knowledge and literacy acquisition.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In English 24 of the 26 letters contain a speech phoneme of the sound value (Treiman & Kessler, 2003) while in Korean 35 of the 40 letters do. In Korean five double consonants do not contain the corresponding phoneme and they are named literally ‘double’-single consonant.

  2. Two exceptions are letters, /kɪ.jʌk/ and /tɪ.gɨt/, which have a CV.CVC structure.

  3. While the majority of letter names for consonants have /ɨ/ in the second syllable, two exceptions exist. One letter name has /jʌ/ (kɪjʌk) and the other has /o/ (∫ɪot).

  4. Phonemic contrasts of 19 consonants in the onset are neutralized to seven in the coda.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aidinis, A., & Nunes, T. (2001). The role of different levels of PA in the development of reading and spelling in Greek. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 145–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Azen, R., & Budescu, D. V. (2003). The dominance analysis for comparing predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Methods, 8, 129–148. doi:10.1037/1082-989X.8.2.129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blatchford, P., & Plewis, L. (1990). Pre-school reading-related skills and later reading achievement: Further evidence. British Educational Research Journal, 16, 425–428. doi:10.1080/0141192900160409.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A. (1994). Phonological sensitivity in novice readers and nonreaders. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58, 134–159. doi:10.1006/jecp.1994.1029.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J., & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction. Applied Psycholinguistics, 12, 91–121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1985). Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.26.3.429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Budescu, D. V. (1993). Dominance analysis: A new approach to the problem of relative importance of predictors in multiple regression. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 542–551. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.114.3.542.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burgess, S. R., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Bidirectional relations of phonological sensitivity and prereading abilities: Evidence from a preschool sample. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 70, 117–141. doi:10.1006/jecp.1998.2450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1990). Acquiring the alphabetic principle: A case for teaching recognition of phoneme identity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 82, 805–812. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.82.4.805.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluation of a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 104–111. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.85.1.104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaney, C. (1994). Language development, metalinguistic awareness, and emergent literacy skills of 3-year-old children in relation to social class. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 371–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cho, J.-R., & McBride-Chang, C. (2005). Correlates of Korean Hangul Acquisition among kindergartners and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 3–16. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.91.3.450.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dixon, M., Stuart, M., & Masterson, J. (2002). The relationship between phonological awareness and the development of orthographic representations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 295–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1983). A critique of five studies related to letter-name knowledge and learning to read. In L. Gentile, M. Kamil, & J. Blanchard (Eds.), Reading research revisited (pp. 143–153). Columbus, OH: C.E. Merrill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Evans, M. A., Shaw, D., & Bell, M. (2000). Home literacy activities and their influence on early literacy skills. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54, 65–75. doi:10.1037/h0087330.

    Google Scholar 

  • Foulin, J. N. (2005). Why is letter-name knowledge such a good predictor of learning to read? Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 129–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading. Child Development, 62, 1110–1123. doi:10.2307/1131156.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Mead, F. (1992). Onset and rime awareness and analogies in reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 153–162. doi:10.2307/747684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Ziegler, J. C., & Richardson, U. (2005). The effects of spelling consistency on phonological awareness: A comparison of English and German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 345–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P. J., & Hulme, C. (1999). Phonemes, rhymes and intelligence as predictors of children’s responsiveness to remedial reading instruction: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72, 130–153. doi:10.1006/jecp. 1998.2480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoien, T., Lundberg, I., Stanovich, K., & Bjaalid, I.-K. (1995). Components of phonological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 171–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C., Hatcher, P. J., Nation, K., Brown, A., Adams, J., & Stuart, G. (2002). Phoneme awareness is a better predictor of early reading skill than onset-rime awareness. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82, 2–28. doi:10.1006/jecp.2002.2670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, R. S., Anderson, M., & Holligan, C. (1996). Knowledge of the alphabet and explicit awareness of phonemes in pre-readers: The nature of the relationship. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 217–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Katzir, T., Kim, Y.-S., Wolf, M., O’Brien, B., Kennedy, B., Lovett, M., et al. (2006). Reading fluency: The whole is more than the parts. Annals of Dyslexia, 56, 51–82. doi:10.1007/s11881-006-0003-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Phonological awareness and literacy skills in Korean: an examination of the unique role of body-coda units. Applied Psycholinguistics, 1, 69–94. doi:10.1017/S014271640707004X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (in press). Cat in a hat or cat in a cap? An investigation of developmental trajectories of phonological awareness for Korean children. Journal of Research in Reading.

  • Levin, I., Patel, S., Margalit, T., & Barad, N. (2002). Letter names: Effect on letter saying, spelling, and word recognition in Hebrew. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 269–300. doi:10.1017/S0142716402002060.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Shatil-Carmon, S., & Asif-Rave, O. (2006). Learning of letter names and sounds and their contribution to word recognition. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 93, 139–165. doi:10.1016/j.jecp. 2005.08.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lonigan, C. J., Burgess, S. R., & Anthony, J. L. (2000). Development of emergent literacy and early reading skills in preschool children: Evidence from a latent-variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 36, 596–613. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.596.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLean, M., Bryant, P., & Bradley, L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes, and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255–281.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1999). The ABCs of the ABCs: The development of letter-name and letter-sound knowledge. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 45, 285–308. 3–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Taylor, S. (1998). Segmentation, not rhyming, predicts early progress in learning to read. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 71, 3–27.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., Hulme, C., Snowling, M., & Stevenson, J. (2004). Phonemes, rimes, vocabulary, and grammatical skills as foundations of early reading development: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology. 40, 665–681.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K., & Hulme, C. (1997). Phonemic segmentation, not onset-rime segmentation, predicts early reading and spelling skills. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 154–167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Na̎slund J. C., & Schneider, W. (1996). Kindergarten letter knowledge, phonological skills, and memory processes: Relative effects on early literacy. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 62, 30–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Oney, B., & Durgunoglu, A. Y. (1997). Beginning to read in Turkish: A phonologically transparent orthography. Applied Psycholinguistics, 18, 1–15.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Francis, D. J., Foorman, B. R., Fletcher, J. M., & Mehta, P. (1999). The dimensionality of phonological awareness: An application of item response theory. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 439–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2004). Knowing letter names and learning letter sounds: A causal connection. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 88, 213–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting in literate and preliterate Hebrew speakers: Onsets and rimes or bodies or codas? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., Jorm, A. F., Maclean, R., & Matthews, R. (1984). Sources of individual differences in reading acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 1309–1324.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snow, C. E., Burns, M. S., & Griffin, P. (Eds.). (1998). Preventing reading difficulties in young children. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprugevica, I., & Hoien, T. (2003). Enabling skills in early reading acquisition: A study of children in Latvian kindergartens. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 16, 159–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M., & Coltheart, M. (1988). Does reading develop in a sequence of stages? Cognition, 30, 139–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, I. (1980). The Korean writing system: An alphabet? a syllabary? a logography? In P. A. Kolers, M. E. Wrolstad, & H. Bouma (Eds.), Processing of visible language (pp. 67–82). New York: Plenum Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1983). The structure of spoken syllables: Evidence from novel word games. Cognition, 15, 49–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1985). Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 161–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1992). The role of intrasyllabic units in leaning to read and spell. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Treiman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 65–106). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Kessler, B. (2003). The role of letter names in the acquisition of literacy. In R. Kail (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (Vol. 31). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Tincoff, R., Rodriguez, K., Mouzaki, A., & Francis, D. J. (1998). The foundations of literacy: Learning the sounds of letters. Child Development, 69, 1524–1540.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Zukowski, A. (1991). Level of phonological awareness. In S. A. Brady & D. P. Shankweiler (Eds.), Phonological processes in literacy (pp. 67–84). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., & Scanlon, D. M. (1987). Phonological coding, phonological awareness, and reading ability: Evidence from a longitudinal and experimental study. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 321–363.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bidirectional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing. Austin, TX: PRO-ED.

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Lonigan, C. J. (1998). Child development and emergent literacy. Child Development, 69, 848–872.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, Y.-B., & Derwing, B. (2001). A language without a rhyme: Syllable structure experiments in Korean. Canadian Journal of Linguistics, 46, 187–237.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

Part of this study was supported by a National Science Foundation Dissertation Grant (#0545205) and Harvard Korea Institute’s Min Young-Chul Memorial Summer Travel Fellowship. The author wishes to thank all the children, teachers, and preschool directors who participated in this study. Special thanks are due to Jaesik Kim and Heesook Kim. In addition, the author wishes to thank Catherine Snow, John Willett, Andrew Nevins, and anonymous reviewers for their feedback on earlier version of the article.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Young-Suk Kim.

Appendices

Appendix A

Pilot sample means, standard deviations, and t statistics for testing differences in children’s performances on body awareness tasks with varied linguistic manipulations in the target word in the oddity tasks (n = 75)

Body awareness

Mean (SD)

Rime change

2.81 (1.22)

Onset coda change

2.37 (1.18)

Syllable change

2.40 (1.25)

In this study, syllable change condition was used in the body awareness task. In the pilot test, the linguistic characteristics of the target odd word (or distractors) were varied in the body awareness task in order to investigate whether its phonological characteristics influence children’s performance. The distractors varied in the following: (1) those requiring a rime change (i.e., the distractor shared the same onset as the other two words; e.g., puk, pul, pam), (2) those requiring an onset-coda change (i.e., the distractor shared the same vowel as the other two words; sam, p h an, p h al), and (3) those requiring a syllable change (i.e., the distractor did not share any sounds with the other two words; e.g., mal, puk, man). A total of 24 items (8 items in each linguistic structure for distractors) was used in the body awareness task. The order of items for each linguistic manipulation condition was randomly arranged.

As seen in the table above, in the body awareness tasks, children’s performance on the rime change distractor items was statistically significantly higher than on the onset-coda change distractor items (t = 3.65, = .000) as well as on the syllable change distractor items (t = 3.32, p = .001), but there was no difference in children’s performance between onset-coda change distractor items and syllable change items. Thus, in this study, syllable change condition was used.

In addition, an attempt was made to examine the potential effect of global sound similarity in the distractors. In another study, a subsample of children (n = 63) were given another body awareness task in addition to the body awareness task with syllable change condition used in this study. In this task, the odd word (distractor) shared the same consonant sounds as one of the other two words and only differed in the vowel (e.g., kaŋ, kam, kim). In this case, the global sound similarity was controlled because two phonemes are shared in the two words that share the same sounds in the body unit (CV, /k/ and /a/ in the example) as well as in one of the two words and the distractor (CC, /k/ and /m/ in the example). Children’s performance on this task (m = 10.11) was not statistically different from their performance on the body awareness task in which the odd word had an entirely different syllable (m = 10.44) (F (1, 62) = .84, p = .36).

Appendix B

Korean alphabet letters and their names

Consonants

Vowels

Letter

Letter name

Letter

Letter name

kɪj∧k

a

nɪɨn

ja

tɪgɨt

rɪɨl

j∧

mɪɨm

o

pɪɨp

jo

∫ɪot

u

ɪɨŋ

ju

cɪɨt

ɨ

t∫ɪ ɨt

ɪ

khɪɨk

æ

thɪɨt

æ

phɪɨp

wa

hɪɨt

ssaŋkɪj∧k

ɨɪ

ssaŋtɪgɨt

w∧

ssaŋpɪɨp

ssaŋ∫ɪot

ssaŋcɪɨt

  

  

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kim, YS. The foundation of literacy skills in Korean: the relationship between letter-name knowledge and phonological awareness and their relative contribution to literacy skills. Read Writ 22, 907–931 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9131-0

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-008-9131-0

Keywords

Navigation