Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Comparing vocabulary development in Spanish- and Chinese-speaking ELLs: the effects of metalinguistic and sociocultural factors

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study evaluated the impact of two metalinguistic factors, English derivational awareness and English–Spanish cognate awareness, and the impact of two sociocultural factors, maternal education and children’s length of residence in Canada, on English Language Learners (ELLs)’ vocabulary knowledge. The participants of the study were 89 Spanish-speaking ELLs, 77 Chinese-speaking ELLs, and a comparison group of 78 monolingual English-speaking children in Grades 4 and 7. The sample included both first-generation (born outside of Canada) and second generation (born in Canada) immigrant children. The study yielded several important findings. First, it confirmed the strong link between derivational awareness and vocabulary knowledge observed in the previous research, and extended this relationship to two groups of ELLs from different first language backgrounds. Second, this study unveiled differences in vocabulary learning between Spanish-speaking and Chinese-speaking ELLs. While Spanish-speaking children were able to utilize the cognate strategy to learn English words, this strategy was not available for Chinese-speaking ELLs. With respect to the sociocultural factors, length of residence in Canada was significantly related to ELLs’ vocabulary development. Interestingly, length of residence in Canada only influenced the development of noncognate vocabulary, but not cognate vocabulary, in Spanish-speaking ELLs, which provides additional evidence for these children’s use of the cognate strategy. Finally, maternal education was not related to English vocabulary development. The theoretical and educational implications of these findings were discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. According to Packard (2000), there are at least two criteria that can be used to distinguish between bound roots and derivational suffixes in Chinese: (1) derivational affixes are more general in meaning than bound roots, and (2) derivational suffixes are more productive than bound roots.

References

  • Anglin, J. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, 1–166. doi:10.2307/1166112.

  • August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary development for English language learners. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 20, 50–57. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2005.00120.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bellomo, T. S. (1999, November). Etymology and vocabulary development for the L2 college student. TESL-EJ, 4(2), 1–2. Retrieved March, 2010, from http://www.cc.kyoto-su.ac.jp/information/tesl-ej/ej14/a2.html.

  • Biemiller, A. (1999). Language and reading success. Cambridge, MA: Brookline.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A., & Slonim, N. (2001). Estimating root word vocabulary growth in normative and advantaged populations: Evidence for a common sequence of vocabulary acquisition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 498–520. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.93.3.498.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bravo, M., Hiebert, E., & Pearson, D. (2007). Tapping the linguistic resources of Spanish/English bilinguals: The role of cognates in science. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 140–156). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bybee, J. L. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language and Cognitive Processes, 10, 425–455. doi:10.1080/01690969508407111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (1995). Morphological awareness and early reading achievement. England: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc, Hillsdale, NJ.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190. doi:10.1023/A:1008131926604.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2007). Fostering morphological processing, vocabulary development, and reading comprehension. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 78–103). NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Fleming, J. (2003). Lexical processing of morphologically complex words in the elementary years. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 239–253. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Nomanbhoy, D. M. (1993). Phonological and morphological awareness in first graders. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 177–195. doi:10.1017/S0142716400009541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlo, M., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C., Dressler, C., Lippman, D., et al. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215. doi:10.1598/RRQ.39.2.3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 1–8. doi:10.1007/s11881-004-0006-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. (1983). Stages of reading development. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chen, X., Pasquarella, A., & Deacon, H. (in preparation). The role of cognate awareness in word reading and reading comprehension in first grade French immersion children.

  • Chow, B. W. Y., McBride-Chang, C., Cheung, H., & Chow, C. S. L. (2008). Dialogic reading and morphology training in chinese children: Effects on language and literacy. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 233–244. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobo-Lewis, A., Pearson, B. Z., Eilers, R. E., & Umbel, V. C. (2002). Effects of bilingualism and bilingual education on oral and written English skills: A multifactor study of standardized test outcomes. In D. K. Oller & R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children. Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Collier, V. P. (1987). Age and rate of acquisition of a second language for academic purposes. TESOL Quarterly, 21, 617–641. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/stable/3586986.

  • Collier, V. P. (1989). How long? A synthesis of research on academic achievement in a second language. TESOL Quarterly, 23, 509–531. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/3586923.

  • Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (1988). Acquisition of cognitive-academic second language proficiency: A six-year study. Paper presented at the American Education Research Association conference, New Orleans.

  • Collier, V. P., & Thomas, W. P. (1989). How quickly can immigrants become proficient in school English? Educational Issues of Language Minority Students, 5, 27–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conger, R. D., McCarthy, J., Yang, R., Lahey, B., & Kropp, J. (1984). Perception of childrearing values, and emotional distress as mediating links between environmental stressors and observed maternal behavior. Child Development, 54, 2234–2247. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/1129795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (1994). The Acquisition of English as a second language. In K. Spangenberg-Urbschat & R. Pritchard (Eds.), Reading Instruction for ESL students. Delaware: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummins, J. (2000). Language, power and pedagogy: Bilingual children in the crossfire. Clevedon: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, P., & Allington, R. (2009). Classrooms that work: They all can read and write. Toronto: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, T. H., & Graham, C. R. (2000). Increasing native English vocabulary recognition through Spanish immersion: Cognate transfer from foreign to first language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 37–49. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.92.1.37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223. doi:10.1017/S0142716404001110.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38(1), 78–103. doi:10.1598/RRQ.38.1.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L., & Dunn, L. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Duyck, W., Van Assche, E., Drieghe, D., & Hartsuiker, R. J. (2007). Visual word recognition by bilinguals in a sentence context: Evidence for non-selective access. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 33, 663–679. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.33.4.663.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fowler, A. E., & Liberman, I. Y. (1995). The role of phonology and orthography in morphological awareness. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, P., & Baron, J. (1982). Individual differences in acquisition of derivational morphology. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 282–295. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(82)90615-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, G. E. (1988). Factors influencing the English reading test performance of SpanishEnglish bilingual children. University of Illinois, Urbana-Champaign, unpublished doctoral dissertation.

  • Garcia, G. E. (1991). Factors influencing the English reading test performance of Spanish-speaking Hispanic students. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 371–392. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/pss/747895.

  • Garcia, G. E., & Nagy, W. E. (1993). Latino students’ concept of cognates. In D. Leu & C. K. Kinzer (Eds.), Examining central issues in literacy research, theory, and practice (pp. 367–374). Chicago: National Reading Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. (2008). Facets of metalinguistic awareness related to reading acquisition in Hebrew: Evidence from monolingual and bilingual children. In K. Koda & A. Zehler (Eds.), Learning to read across languages (pp. 154–187). New York, NY: Routledge.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Farnia, F. (2009). Language skills, reading fluency, and reading comprehension in immigrant children. Paper presented at Language and Reading Comprehension for Immigrant Children Conference, Toronto, Canada.

  • Golberg, H., Paradis, J., & Crago, M. (2008). Lexical acquisition over time in minority first language children learning English as a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 1–25. doi:10.1017/S014271640808003X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldenberg, C., Rueda, R., & August, D. (2006). Sociocultural influences on the literacy attainment of language-minority children and youth. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 269–347). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hammond, J., & Simmons, C. (1987) The cognate language teacher. In Proceedings of a conference on the use of computers in the teaching of language and languages, pp. 100–106, CTISS, Bath.

  • Hancin-Bhatt, B., & Nagy, W. (1994). Lexical transfer and second language morphological development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 289–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hao, M. L., Chen, X., Dronjic, V., Shu, H., & Anderson, R. C. (in press). The development of young Chinese children’s morphological awareness: The role of semantic relatedness and morpheme type. Applied Psycholinguistics.

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jean, M., & Geva, E. (2009). The development of vocabulary in English as a second language children and its role in predicting word recognition ability. Applied Psycholinguistics, 30, 153–185. doi:10.1017/S0142716408090073.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jia, G. (1998). Beyond brain maturation: The critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition revisited. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. New York: New York University.

  • Jiménez, R. T., Garcia, G. E., & Pearson, P. D. (1996). The reading strategies of bilingual Latino/a students who are successful English readers: Opportunities and obstacles. Reading Research Quarterly, 31, 283–301. doi:10.1598/RRQ.31.1.5.

  • Kieffer, M., & Lesaux, N. (2008). The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Reading and Writing, 21, 783–804. doi:10.1007/s11145-007-9092-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lam, K., Chen, X., Geva, E., Luo, Y., & Li, H. (accepted). The effects of morphological awareness development on reading achievement in young English language learners (ELLs): A longitudinal study. Reading and Writing.

  • Lesaux, N., & Geva, E. (2006). Synthesis: Development of literacy in language-minority students. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 53–74). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N., & Siegel, L. S. (2003). The development of reading in children who speak English as a Second Language (ESL). Developmental Psychology, 39, 1005–1019. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.6.1005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, C. N., & Thompson, S. A. (1981). Mandarin Chinese. A functional reference grammar. Berkeley: University of California Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Libben, M., & Titone, D. (2009). Bilingual lexical access in context: Evidence from eye movements during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 381–390. doi:10.1037/a0014875.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2007). The development of reading skills in children with English as a second language. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 105–131. doi:10.1080/10888430709336555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Louie, V. (2004). Compelled to excel: Immigration, education and opportunity among Chinese Americans. Stanford: Stanford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucchese, F., & Tamis-LeMonda, C. S. (2007). Fostering language development in children from disadvantaged backgrounds. Encyclopedia of language and literacy development (pp. 1–11). London, ON: Canadian Language and Literacy Research Network.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahony, D. L. (1994). Using sensitivity to word structure to explain variance in high-school and college level reading-ability. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 6, 19–44. doi:10.1007/BF01027276.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malabonga, V., Kenyon, D. M., Carlo, M., August, D., & Louguit, M. (2008). Development of a cognate awareness measure for spanish-speaking english language learners. Language Testing, 25(4), 495–519. doi:10.1177/0265532208094274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Tardif, T., Cho, J.-R., Shu, H., Fletcher, P., Stokes, S. F., et al. (2008). What’s in a word? Morphological awareness and vocabulary knowledge in three languages. Applied Psycholinguistics, 29, 437–462. doi:10.1017/S014271640808020X.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLoyd, V. C. (1990). The impact of economic hardship on Black families and children: Psychological distress, parenting, and socioemotional development. Child Development, 61, 311–346. doi:10.2307/1131096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moss, G. (1992). Cognate recognition: Its implications in the teaching of ESP reading courses to Spanish speakers. English for Specific Purposes, 11, 141–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muter, V., & Diethelm, K. (2001). The contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. Language Learning, 51, 187–219. doi:10.1111/1467-9922.00153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., & Anderson, R. C. (1984). How many words are there in printed school English. Reading Research Quarterly, 19, 304–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006). Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.98.1.134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.730.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Garcia, G. E., Durgunoglu, A. Y., & Hancin-Bhatt, B. (1993). Spanish–English bilingual students’ use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 241–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. A. (2000). Vocabulary Processes. In M. Kamil, P. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. III, pp. 269–284). Muhwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nash, R. (1997). NTC’s dictionary of Spanish cognates: Thematically organized. Lincolnwood, IL: NTC Publishing Group.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicoladis, E., Palmer, A., & Marentette, P. (2007). The role of type and token frequency in using past tense morphemes correctly. Developmental Science, 10(2), 237–254. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00582.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nunes, T., Bryant, P., & Olsson, J. (2003). Learning morphological and phonological spelling rules: An intervention study. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7(3), 289–307. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0703_6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OCA and The Asian American Studies Program. (2008). A Snapshot of “A Portrait of Chinese Americans”. Retrieved on May 25, 2010 from http://www.aast.umd.edu/snapshotofportrait.pdf.

  • Packard, J. (2000). The morphology of Chinese: A linguistic and cognitive approach. UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Paradis, J. (2007). Second language acquisition in childhood. In E. Hoff & M. Shatz (Eds.), Language development (pp. 387–405). Malden, Oxford, Carlton: Blackwell Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Penno, J. F., Wilkinson, I. A. G., & Moore, D. W. (2002). Vocabulary acquisition from teacher explanation and repeated listening to stories: Do they overcome the Matthew effect? Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(1), 23–33. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.94.1.23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Phythian-Sence, C., & Wagner, R. K. (2007). Vocabulary acquisition: A primer. In R. K. Wagner, A. E. Muse, & K. R. Tannenbaum (Eds.), Vocabulary acquisition: Implications for reading comprehension (pp. 1–14). NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Towards a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 246–256. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.97.2.246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., & Mo, E. (2009). The relationship between cognate awareness and English comprehension among Spanish–English bilingual fourth grade students. TESOL Quarterly, 43, 126–136.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ramirez, G., Chen, X., Geva, E., & Luo, Y. C. (2011). Morphological awareness and word reading in ELLs: Evidence from Spanish- and Chinese-speaking children, Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 601–618.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (1958). Standard progressive matrices. London, UK: H.K. Lewis and Co. Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., Raven, J. C., & Court, J. H. (2000). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Section 3: The standard progressive matrices. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reed, D. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and effects on reading outcomes for students in grades K-12. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 36–49. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00261.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, A. I., & Kroll, J. F. (2006). Bilingual lexical activation in sentence context. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 197–212. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2006.03.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Crain, S., Katz, L., Fowler, A. E., Liberman, A. M., Brady, S. A., et al. (1995). Cognitive profiles of reading-disabled children: Comparison of language skills in phonology, morphology, and syntax. Phonological Science, 6, 149–156. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00324.x.

    Google Scholar 

  • Singson, M., Mahony, D., & Mann, V. (2000). The relation between reading ability and morphological skills: Evidence from derivational suffixes. Reading and Writing, 12, 219–252. doi:10.1023/A:1008196330239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S. A., & Nagy, W. (2006). Teaching word meanings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Statistics Canada. (2008). Profile of languages, immigration, citizenship, mobility, and migration for urban areas, 2006 Census. Retrieved March, 2010, from http://www.statcan.ca/bsolc/english/bsolc?catno=94-577-X2006009.

  • Tomasello, M. (2003). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649–667. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90002-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Umbel, V. M., Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, M. C., & Oller, D. K. (1992). Measuring bilingual children’s receptive vocabularies. Child Development, 63, 1012–1020. doi:10.2307/1131250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • U.S. Department of Education, National Center for Education Statistics. (2001). The condition of education. Washington, DC: U.S. Government Printing Office.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Hell, J. G. (1998). Cross-language processing and bilingual memory organization. Unpublished doctoral Dissertation, University of Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

  • Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1993). Lexical knowledge of monolingual and bilingual children. Applied Linguistics, 14, 344–363. Retrieved from http://resolver.scholarsportal.info.myaccess.library.utoronto.ca/resolve/01426001/v14i0004/344_lkomabc.

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J., & Rashotte, C. (1999). Comprehensive test of phonological processing (CTOPP). Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wang, S., & Lo, L. (2004). Chinese immigrants in Canada: Their changing composition and economic performance. Toronto: Joint Centre of Excellence for Research on Immigration and Settlement.

    Google Scholar 

  • White, T., Power, M., & White, S. (1989). Morphological analysis: Implications for teaching for understanding vocabulary growth. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 283–304. Retrieved from http://www.jstor.org/stable/747771.

  • Whitley, M. S. (2002). Spanish/English contrasts: A course in Spanish linguistics (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: Georgetown University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1984). Woodcock language proficiency battery. Allen, TX: DLM.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22(1), 66–81. doi:10.2307/747721.

    Google Scholar 

  • Xing, J. Z. (2006). Teaching and learning Chinese as a foreign language: A pedagogical grammar. Hong Kong: Hong Kong University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zeno, S. M., Ivens, S. H., Millard, R. T., & Duvvuri, R. (1995). The educator’s word frequency guide. Brewster, NY: Touchstone Applied Science Associates.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Xi Chen.

Appendix

Appendix

  List of cognate and non-cognate items

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Chen, X., Ramirez, G., Luo, Y.C. et al. Comparing vocabulary development in Spanish- and Chinese-speaking ELLs: the effects of metalinguistic and sociocultural factors. Read Writ 25, 1991–2020 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9318-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9318-7

Keywords

Navigation