Skip to main content
Log in

The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the relationship between morphological awareness and reading comprehension in English among Spanish-speaking English language learners (ELLs) followed from fourth through fifth grade. Students’ ability to decompose derived words while reading was assessed using an experimental task. Multiple regression analyses were used to investigate the contribution of performance on this task to reading comprehension above and beyond word reading skills, phonological awareness, and breadth of vocabulary knowledge. The relationship between morphological awareness and reading comprehension was found to strengthen between fourth and fifth grade, and in fifth grade, morphological awareness was found to be a significant predictor of reading comprehension. The findings were robust across two measures of reading comprehension and two methods of scoring the experimental task of morphological awareness, and thus support the inclusion of derivational morphology in a model of the English reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking ELLs.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. In the correlational analyses reported here and multiple regression analyses that follow, developmental standard scores were used when available (for the WLPB-R measures and Gates-MacGinitie Reading Comprehension test) and raw scores were used when not (for the PPVT, CTOPP Elision, TOWRE Sight Word Efficiency, and test of morphological awareness); using these scores rather than standard scores allows for better longitudinal comparisons in that they are only interval scales that also do not constrain variation to be the same across time.

References

  • Aarts, R., & Verhoeven, L. (1999). Literacy attainment in a second language submersion context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, 377–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1998a). Attitudes and culture in second language learning among Israeli-Arab students. Curriculum and Teaching, 13, 13–30.

    Google Scholar 

  • Abu-Rabia, S. (1998b). Social and cognitive factors influencing the reading comprehension of Arab students learning Hebrew as a second language in Israel. Journal of Research in Reading, 21, 201–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1981). Vocabulary knowledge. In J. T. Guthrie (Ed.), Comprehension and teaching: Research reviews (pp. 77–117). Newark, DE: International Reading Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, R. C., & Freebody, P. (1983). Reading comprehension and the assessment and acquisition of word knowledge. In B. Hutson (Ed.), Advances in reading/language research (Vol. 2). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  • Anglin, J. M. (1993). Vocabulary development: A morphological analysis. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 58, Serial #238.

  • August, D., Carlo, M., Dressler, C., & Snow, C. E. (2005). The critical role of vocabulary development for English language learners. Learning Disabilities: Research and Practice, 20, 50–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • August, D., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2002). Bringing words to life: Robust vocabulary instruction. New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capps, R., Fix, M., Murray, J., Ost, J., Passel, J., & Herwantoro, S. (2005). The new demography of America’s schools: Immigration and the no child left behind act. Washington, DC: The Urban Institute.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2000). Awareness of the structure and meaning of morphologically complex words: Impact on reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 169–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F. (2003). Morphology matters in learning to read: A commentary. Reading Psychology, 24, 291–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Katz, L. A. (2006). Effects of word and morpheme familiarity on reading of derived words. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 669–693.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 428–449.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlo, M. S., August, D., McLaughlin, B., Snow, C. E., Dressler, C., Lippman, D. N., Lively, R. J., & White, C. E. (2004). Closing the gap: Addressing the vocabulary needs of English-language learners in bilingual and mainstream classrooms. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 188–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). Early reading acquisition and its relation to reading experience and ability ten years later. Developmental Psychology, 33, 934–945.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Deacon, S. H., & Kirby, J. R. (2004). Morphological awareness: Just “more phonological”? The roles of morphological and phonological awareness in reading development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 223–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 78–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody picture vocabulary test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Durkin, D. (1978/1979). What classroom observations reveal about comprehension instruction. Reading Research Quarterly, 14, 481–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Francis, D. J., Snow, C. E., August, D., Carlson, C. D., Miller, J., & Iglesias, A. (2006). Measures of reading comprehension: A latent variable analysis of the Diagnostic Assessment of Reading Comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 301–322.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Freyd, P., & Baron, J. (1982). Individual differences in acquisition of derivational morphology. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 21, 282–295.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garcia, G. E. (1991). Factors influencing the English reading test performance of Spanish-speaking Hispanic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 26, 371–392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graves, M. F. (2006). The vocabulary book: Learning and instruction. New York: Teachers College Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hacquebord, H. (1994). L2-reading in the content areas: Text comprehension in secondary education in the Netherlands. Journal of Research in Reading, 17, 83–98.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hancin-Bhatt, B., & Nagy, W. (1994). Lexical transfer and second language morphological development. Applied Psycholinguistics, 15, 289–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutchinson, J. M., Whiteley, H. E., Smith, C. D., & Connors, L. (2003). The developmental progression of comprehension-related skills in children learning EAL. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 19–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Keenan, J. (2006). How comparable are reading comprehension tests? Presented at the meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading. Vancouver, BC, Canada.

  • Kemp, N. (2006). Children’s spelling of base, inflected, and derived words: Links with morphological awareness. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 737–765.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (2000). Rapid processing of base and derived forms of words and grades 4, 5, and 6 children’s spelling. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 277–302.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., with Koda, K., Siegel, L. S., & Shanahan, T. (2006). Development of literacy. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (pp. 75–122). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Lesaux, N. K., & Crosson, A. C. (2005). Addressing variability and vulnerability: Promoting the academic achievement of English learners in San Diego. In R. Hess (Ed.). Urban reform: Lessons from San Diego (pp. 263–281). Cambridge, MA: Harvard Education Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MacGinitie, W., MacGinitie, R., Maria, K., & Dreyer, L. G. (2000). Gates-MacGinitie reading test (4th ed.). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mahony, D., Singson, D., & Mann, V. (2000). Reading ability and sensitivity to morphological relations. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 12, 191–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mumtaz, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2001). The effects of bilingualism on learning to read English: Evidence from the contrast between Urdu-English bilingual and English monolingual children. Journal of Research in Reading, 24, 113–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W., Berninger, V., Abbott, R., Vaughan, K., & Vermeulen, K. (2003). Relationship of morphology and other language skills to literacy skills in at-risk second-grade readers and at-risk fourth-grade writers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 730–742.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., Berninger, V. W., & Abbott, R. D. (2006) Contributions of morphology beyond phonology to literacy outcomes of upper elementary and middle-school students. Journal of Educational Psychology, 98, 134–147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Garcia, G. E. (1993). Spanish-English bilingual students’ use of cognates in English reading. Journal of Reading Behavior, 25, 241–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti C. A., & Hart, L. (2001). The lexical basis of comprehension skill. In D. S. Gorfein (Ed.), On the consequences of meaning selection: Perspectives on resolving lexical ambiguity (pp. 67–86). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Proctor, C. P., Carlo, M., August, D., & Snow, C. E. (2005). Native Spanish-speaking children reading in English: Toward a model of comprehension. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 246–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raudenbush, S. W., & Bryk, A. S. (2002). Hierarchical linear models: Applications and data analysis methods (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • RAND Reading Study Group. (2002). Reading for understanding: Toward an R&D program in reading comprehension. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation.

  • Roser, N., & Juel, C. (1982). Effects of vocabulary instruction on reading comprehension. In J. A. Niles & L. A. Harris (Eds.), Yearbook of the National Reading Conference: Vol. 31: New inquiries in reading research and instruction (pp. 110–118). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.

    Google Scholar 

  • Saville-Troike, M. (1984). What really matters in second language learning for academic achievement? TESOL Quarterly, 18, 199–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scott, J. A., Jamieson-Noel, D., & Asselin, M. (2003). Vocabulary instruction throughout the day in twenty-three Canadian upper-elementary classrooms. Elementary School Journal, 103, 269–283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shankweiler, D., Lundquist, E., Dreyer, L. G., & Dickinson, C. C. (1996). Reading and spelling difficulties in high school students: Causes and consequences. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 267–294.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Singer, J., & Willett, J. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S., & Fairbanks, M. (1986). The effects of vocabulary instruction: A model based meta-analysis. Review of Educational Research, 56, 72–110.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S., & Nagy, W. (2006). Teaching word meanings. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tannenbaum, K., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (2006). Relationships between word knowledge and reading comprehension in third-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 381–398.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tyler, A., & Nagy, W. (1989). The acquisition of English derivational morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 649–667.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhallen, M., & Schoonen, R. (1998). Lexical knowledge in L1 and L2 third and fifth graders. Applied Linguistics, 19, 452–470.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & Carlisle, J. F. (2006). Introduction to the special issue: Morphology in word identification and word spelling. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 643–650.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verhoeven, L., & Perfetti, C. (2003). The role of morphology in learning to read. Scientific Studies of Reading, 7, 209–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watts, S. M. (1995). Vocabulary instruction during reading lessons in six classrooms. Journal of Reading Behavior, 27, 399–424.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1991). Woodcock Language Proficiency Battery-revised (WLPB-R). Itasca, IL: Riverside Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wysocki, K., & Jenkins, J. R. (1987). Deriving word meanings through morphological generalization. Reading Research Quarterly, 22, 66–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This research was supported by National Institute for Child Health and Human Development Grant 1 R03 HD049674-01 awarded to Nonie K. Lesaux, as well as a Spencer Postdoctoral Fellowship, and in part by a Harvard Graduate School of Education Dean’s Summer Fellowship awarded to Michael J. Kieffer. Some of these findings were presented at the 2006 annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association (San Francisco, CA) as well as at the 2006 annual meeting of the Society for the Scientific Studies in Reading (Vancouver, BC). The authors would like to thank Amy C. Crosson for major contributions to this research, William E. Nagy for his thoughtful insights into an earlier draft of this paper, and Carol Barry, Debbie Beldock, and Carol Osborne for their support of the project. Finally, special thanks to the principals, teachers, and students who participated in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael J. Kieffer.

Appendix

Appendix

Derivational Morphological Awareness Task (adapted from Carlisle, 2000)

Practice:

  • a. Driver. Children are too young to ____________________________________.

  • b. Improvement. My teacher wants my spelling to _________________________.

  • 1. growth. She wanted her plant to _____________________________________.

  • 2. dryer. Put the wash out to __________________________________________.

  • 3. width. The mouth of the river is very _________________________________.

  • 4. density. The smoke in the room was very ______________________________.

  • 5. discussion. The friends have a lot to __________________________________.

  • 6. famous. The actor would achieve much _______________________________.

  • 7. fifth. The boy counted from one to ___________________________________.

  • 8. strength. The girl was very __________________________________________.

  • 9. decision. The boy found it hard to ____________________________________.

  • 10. popularity. The girl wants to be _____________________________________.

  • 11. runner. How fast can she __________________________________________?

  • 12. availability. The video will soon be __________________________________.

  • 13. glorious. The hero achieved great ___________________________________.

  • 14. originality. That painting is very ____________________________________.

  • 15. courageous. The man showed great __________________________________.

  • 16. admission. How many people will they _______________________________?

  • 17. baker. She put the bread in to _______________________________________.

  • 18. division. The cake is hard to ________________________________________.

  • 19. fourth. Seven minus three equals ____________________________________.

  • 20. continuous. How long will the storm _________________________________?

  • 21. swimmer. The girl loves to _________________________________________.

  • 22. furious. The man was full of ________________________________________.

  • 23. teacher. Mr. Gonzalez likes to _______________________________________.

  • 24. activity. The children are very _______________________________________.

  • 25. possession. How many toys do you ___________________________________?

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Kieffer, M.J., Lesaux, N.K. The role of derivational morphology in the reading comprehension of Spanish-speaking English language learners. Read Writ 21, 783–804 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9092-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9092-8

Keywords

Navigation