Skip to main content
Log in

The Integration of Speaker and Listener Responses: A Theory of Verbal Development

  • Theoretical Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We provide an empirically updated Skinnerian-based account of verbal behavior development, describing how the speaker-as-own-listener capability in children (the capability of children to behave as speaker and listener within their own skin) accrues and how it is pivotal to becoming verbal. The theory grew from (a) findings in experiments with children with and without language delays and (b) findings from research devoted to the identification of derived and emergent behavior (i.e., novel, creative, and spontaneous behavior). Experiments identified preverbal instructional histories leading to separate listener and speaker capabilities and experiences that joined the listener and the speaker. Once this learned intercept is present, children engage in conversational self-talk, engage in say-do correspondence, and acquire new vocabulary without direct instruction. These developmental capabilities make it possible for most complex behavior to be learned, including reading, writing, emission of novel tenses and suffixes, and the following of and construction of complex algorithms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • ALBERS, A.E., & GREER, R. D. (1991). Is the three-term contingency trial a predictor of effective instruction? Journal of Behavioral Education, 1, 337–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ARNTZEN, E. A., & ALMAS, A. K. (2002). Effects of mand-tact versus tact only training on the acquisition of tacts. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 35, 419–422.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BAER, D. M., PETERSON, R. F., & SHERMAN, J. A. (1967). The development of imitation in young children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 10, 405–416.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., & CULLINAN, V. (2001). Relational frame theory and Skinner’s Verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 69–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., STAUNTON, C., WHELAN, R., BARNES-HOLMES, Y., COMMINS, S., WALSH, D., Et al. (2005). Derived relations, semantic priming, and event related potentials. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84, 417–451.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, D., HEALEY, O., & HAYES, S. C. (2000). Relational frame theory and the relational evaluational procedure: Approaching language as derived relational responding. In J. C. Leslie & D. Blackman (Eds.), Experimental and applied behavior analysis (pp. 149–180). Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • BARNES-HOLMES, Y., BARNES-HOLMES, D., ROCHE, B., HEALEY, O., LYDDY, F., CULLINAN, V., Et al. (2000). Psychological development. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 157–180). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (2007). Learning, interim 4th edition. Cornwall-on-Hudson, NY: Sloan Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (2001). Three types of selection and three centuries. Revista Internacional de psicologia y therapia psicologia, I(1), 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • CATANIA, A. C. (2007). Learning, interim (4th edition). Cornwall-on-Hudson, NY: Sloan.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHASE, P. N., JOHNSON, T. R., & SULZER-AZAROFF, B. (1985). Verbal relations within instruction: Are there subclasses of the intraverbal? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 301–313.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • CHAVEZ-BROWN, M, & GREER, R. D. (in press). The effects of the acquisition of a generalized auditory word match-to-sample selection repertoire on the emergence or improvement of the echoic repertoire. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior.

  • CHOMSKY, N. (1959). A review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CHU, H. C. (1998). A comparison of verbal-behavior and social-skills approaches for development of social interaction skills and concurrent reduction of aberrant behaviors of children with developmental disabilities in the context of matching theory (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 1998). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [online]. Dissertation Abstracts Item: AAT 9838900.

    Google Scholar 

  • CRYSTAL, D. (2006). How language works: How babies babble, words change meaning, and language lives or dies. Woodstock, NY: Overlook Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • CULOTTA, E., & HANSON, B. (2004). First words. Science, 303, 1315.

  • DAVIDSON, R. J. (1978, May). Specificity and patterning in biobehavioral systems: Implications for behavior change. American Psychologist, pp. 430–436.

    Google Scholar 

  • DECASPER, A. J., & SPENCE, M. J. (1987). Prenatal maternal speech influences newborns’ perception of speech sounds. Infant Behavior and Development, 9, 133–150.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DICKINS, D. W. (2005). On aims and methods in the neouroimaging of derived relations. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 84, 453–484.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • DINSMOOR, J. A. (1983). Observing and conditioned reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 6, 693–728.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DONAHOE, J. W., & PALMER, D. C. (2004). Learning and complex behavior. Richmond, VA: Ledgetop.

    Google Scholar 

  • DONLEY, C. R., & GREER, R. D. (1993). Setting events controlling social verbal exchanges between students with developmental delays. Journal of Behavioral Education, 3(4), 387–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EMURIAN, H. H. (2004). A programmed instruction tutoring system for JAVA™: Consideration of learning performance and software selfefficacy. Computers in Human Behavior, 20, 423–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • EMURIAN, H. H., HU, X., WANG, J., & DURHAM, D. (2000). Learning JAVA: A programmed instruction approach using applets. Computers in Human Behavior, 16, 395–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ENGELMANN, S., & CARNINE, D. (1991). Theory of instruction: Principles and applications. Eugene, OR: ADI Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • EPSTEIN, R., LANZA, P., & SKINNER, B. F. (1980). Symbolic communication between two pigeons (Columba livia domestica). Science, 207, 543–545.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • EPTING, L. K., & CRITCHFIELD, T. S. (2006). Self-editing: On the relation between behavioral and psycholinguistic approaches. The Behavior Analyst, 29, 211–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ESCH, B. E., CARR, J. E., & MICHAEL, J. (2005). Evaluating stimulus-stimulus pairing and direct reinforcement in the establishment of an echoic repertoire of children diagnosed with autism. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 43–58.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • FELICIANO, G. (2006). Multiple exemplar instruction and the listener half of Naming in children with limited speaker abilities. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Columbia University, 2006. Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstract Item: AAT 3213505.

    Google Scholar 

  • FIORILE, C. A., & GREER, R. D. (2007). The induction of Naming in children with no echoic-to-tact responses as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 23, 71–88.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GEWIRTZ, J. L. (1969). Mechanisms of social learning: Some roles of stimulation and behavior in early human development. In D. A. Goslin, Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 57–72). Chicago: Rand-McNally.

    Google Scholar 

  • GILIC, L. (2005). Development of Naming in two-year-old children (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 2005). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstract Item: AAT 3188740.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D. (1994). The measure of a teacher. In R. Gardner III, et al. (Eds.), Behavior analysis in education: Focus on measurably superior instruction (pp. 161–171). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., CHAVEZ-BROWN, M., NIRGUDKAR, A. S., STOLFI, L., & RIVERA-VALDES, C. L. (2005). Acquisition of fluent listener responses and the educational advancement of young children with autism and severe language delays. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 6(2), 88–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & KEOHANE, D. D. (2005). The evolution of verbal behavior in children. Behavioral Development Bulletin, 1, 31–47. Reprinted in 2006 in the Journal of Speech and Language Pathology: Applied Behavior Analysis, Volume 1(2). http://www.slp-aba.net

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & MCDONOUGH, S. (1999). Is the learn unit the fundamental measure of pedagogy? The Behavior Analyst, 20, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., NIRGUDKAR, A., & PARK, H. (2003, June). The effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transformation of mand and tact functions. Paper presented at the annual international meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & O’SULLIVAN, D. (2007, May). Preliminary report of Naming for 2-dimensional stimuli in typically developing first graders. Paper presented at the annual international meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Diego.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & ROSS, D. E. (2004). Verbal behavior analysis: A program of Research in the induction and expansion of complex verbal behavior. Journal of Early Intensive Behavioral Intervention, 1(2). 141–165. Retrieved February 20, 2005, from http://www.jeibi.com/JEIBI-1-2.pdf

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & ROSS, D. E. (2008). Verbal behavior analysis: Inducing and expanding complex communication in children severe language delays. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & SINGER-DUDEK, J. (2008). The emergence of conditioned reinforcement from observation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89, 15–39.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., SINGER-DUDEK, J., & GAUTREAUX, G. (2006). Observational learning. International Journal of Psychology, 42(6), 486–489.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., SINGER-DUDEK, J., LONGANO, J., & ZRINZO, M. (2008). The emergence of praise as conditioned reinforcement as a function of observation in preschool and school age children. Revista Psychologie Mexico, 25(1), 5–26.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., STOLFI, L., CHAVEZ-BROWN, M., & RIVERA-VALDEZ, C. (2005). The emergence of the listener to speaker component of Naming in children as a function of multiple exemplar instruction. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 123–134.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., STOLFI, L., & PISTOLJEVIC, N. (2007). Emergence of Naming in preschoolers: A comparison of multiple and single exemplar instruction. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 8, 119–131.

    Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., & YUAN, L. (2008). How kids learn to say the darnedest things: The effects of multiple exemplar instruction on novel verb usage. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 103–121.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GREER, R. D., YUAN, L., & GAUTREAUX, G. (2005). Novel dictation and intraverbal responses as a function of a multiple exemplar instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HART B. M., & RISLEY, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday life of America’s children. Baltimore: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & ROCHE, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post Skinnerian theory of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer/Academic Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., & HAYES, L. J. (1989). The verbal action of the listener as a basis for rule-governance. In S. C. Hayes, Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies, and instructional control (pp. 153–190). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • HEALY, O., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (2000). Derived relationalresponding as generalized operant behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 74, 207–227.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HELOU-CARE, Y. (2008). The effects of the acquisition of naming on reading comprehension with academically delayed middle school students with behavioral disorders. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • HEYES, C. M. & Galef, Jr., B. G. (Eds.). (1996). Social learning in animals: The roots of culture. San Diego: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HORNE, P. J., & LOWE, C. F. (1996). On the origins of Naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HORNE, J., LOWE, C. F., & RANDLE, V. R. L. (2004). Naming and categorization in young children: II. Listener behavior training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 81, 267–288.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HUGH, A. K. (2006). Effects of auditory stimulation on echolalia and palilalia. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • INGHAM, P., & GREER, R. D. (1992). Changes in student and teacher responses in observed and generalized settings as function of supervisor observations of teachers. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 25, 153–164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JADLOWSKI, S. M. (2000). The effects of a teacher editor, peer editing, and serving as a peer editor on elementary students’ self-editing behavior (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 2000). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 9970212.

    Google Scholar 

  • KANGAS, B. D., & BRANCH, M. N. (2008). Empirical validation of a procedure to correct position and stimulus biases in matching-to-sample. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 103–112.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KARCHMER, M. A., & MITCHELL, R. E. (2003). Demographic and achievement characteristics of deaf and hard-of-hearing students. In M. Marschark & P. E. Spencer (Eds.), Deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 146–192). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • KEOHANE, D. D., & GREER, R. D. (2005). Teachers’ use of verbally governed algorithm and student learning. Journal of Behavioral and Consultation Therapy, 1(3), 249–259. Available online at www.behavior_analyst_today.com

    Google Scholar 

  • KEOHANE, D. D., GREER, R. D., & ACKERMAN, S. A. (2006a, May). Effects of teaching sameness across the senses on acquisition of instructional objectives by pre-listeners and listeners and emergent-speakers. Paper presented as part of a symposium at the annual international Association for Behavior Analysis, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • KEOHANE, D. D., GREER, R. D., & ACKERMAN, S. A. (2006b, May). The effect of conditioning visual tracking on the acquisition of instructional objectives by pre-listeners and pre-speakers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • KEOHANE, D. D., GREER, R. D., & ACKERMAN, S. A. (2006c, May). The effects of conditioning voices as reinforcement for listener observation on acquisition of instructional objectives by pre-listeners and listeners and emergent-speakers. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Association for Behavior Analysis, Atlanta, GA.

    Google Scholar 

  • KEOHANE, D., PEREIRA-DELGADO, J., & GREER, R. D. (2009). Observing responses: Foundations of higher order verbal operants. In Y. Barnes-Holmes & R. A. Rehfeldt (Eds.), Applications of relational frame theory (pp. 41–62). Los Angeles: Harbinger Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • LAMARRE, J., & HOLLAND, J. (1985). The functional independence of mands and tacts. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 5–19.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LEADER, G., BARNES-HOLMES, D., & SMEETS, P. M. (2000). Establishing equivalence relations using a respondent-type procedure III. Psychological Record, 50, 63–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LEE, V. L. (1981) Prepositional phrases spoken and heard. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 35, 227–242.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LEE-PARK, H. S. (2005). Multiple exemplar instruction and transformation of stimulus function from auditory-visual matching to visual-visual matching (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 3174834.

    Google Scholar 

  • LODHI, S., & GREER, R. D. (1989). The speaker as listener. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 353–360.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LONGANO, J. M. (2008). An investigation of the source of reinforcement for Naming as a function of a second order classical conditioning procedure. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • LONGANO, J., & GREER, R. D. (2006). The effects of a stimulus-stimulus pairing procedure on the acquisition of conditioned reinforcement for observing and manipulating stimuli by young children with autism. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Interventions, 3, 135N-150. Retrieved February 22 from http://www.behavior-analyst-online.org

  • LOVAAS, O. I. (1977). The autistic child: Language development through behavior modification. New York: Irvington.

    Google Scholar 

  • LOWE, C. F., HORNE, P. J., HARRIS, D. S., & RANDLE, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LOWE, C. F., HORNE, P. J., & HUGHES, J. C. (2005). Naming and categorization in young children: III. Vocal tact training and transfer of function. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 83(1), 47–65.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LOWENKRON, B. (1991). Joint control and the generalization of selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 9, 121–126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LOWENKRON, B. (1998). Some logical functions of joint control. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69, 327–354.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LUCIANO, M. C., HERRUZO, J., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (2001). Generalization of say-do correspondence. The Psychological Record, 51, 111–130.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LUCIANO, M. C., & POLAINO-LORENTE, A. (1986). The effects of the acquisition of prerequisite behavior of non-vocal children and vocal imitation in children with severe retardation. The Psychological Record, 36, 315–332.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LYDON, H., HEALY, O., LEADER, G., & KEOHANE, D. D. (2008). The effects of intensive tact instruction on three verbal operants in non-instructional settings for two children with autism. Paper submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • MADHO, V. (1997). The effects of the responses of a reader on the writing effectiveness of children with developmental delays (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT9809740.

    Google Scholar 

  • MARION, C., VAUSE, T., HARAPIAK, S., MARTIN, G. L., YU, D. C., SAKKO, G., & WALTERS, K. L. (2003). The hierarchical relationship between several visual and auditory discriminations and three verbal operants among individuals with developmental disabilities. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 19, 91–106.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MARSICO, M. J. (1998). Textual stimulus control of independent math performance and generalization to reading (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 9822227.

    Google Scholar 

  • MCGUINNESS, D. (2004). Early reading instruction: What science really tells us about how to teach reading. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • MEINCKE-MATTHEWS, K. (2005). Induction of metaphorical responses in middle school students as a function of multiple exemplar instruction (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, 2005). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 3174851.

    Google Scholar 

  • MELTZOFF, A. N. (1996). The human infant as an imitative generalist: A 20-year progress report on infant imitation with implications for comparative psychology. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals (pp. 347–370). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • MELTZOFF, A. N., & MOORE, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Development, 54, 702–709.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MICHAEL, J. (1984). Verbal behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 42, 363–376.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MIGUEL, C. F., PETURSDOTTIR, A. I., CARR, J. E., & MICHAEL, J. (2008). The role of naming in stimulus categorization by preschool children. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 89(2), 343–407.

    Google Scholar 

  • NIRGUDKAR, A. S. (2005). The relative effects of the acquisition of naming and the multiple exemplar establishing operation experience on the acquisition of the transformation of establishing operations across mands and tacts (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [on-line]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 3159751.

    Google Scholar 

  • NOVAK, G. (1996). Developmental psychology: Dynamical systems and behavior analysis. Reno, NV: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • NOVAK, G., & PEL ÁEZ, M. (2004). Child and adolescent development: A behavioral systems approach. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • NUZZOLO-GOMEZ, R., & GREER, R. D. (2004). Emergence of untaught mands or tacts with novel adjective-object pairs as a function of instructional history. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 30–47.

    Google Scholar 

  • PELÁEZ-NOGU ERAS, M., FIELD, T., CIGALES, M., GEWIRTZ, J., GONZALES, A., & CLASKY, C. (1997). The effects of systematic stroking versus tickling and poking on infant attention and affect. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 18, 169-177.

  • PANIAGUA, F. A., & BAER, D. M. (1982). The analysis of correspondence training as a chain reinforceable at any point. Child Development, 53, 786–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PEIRCE, C. S. (1935). In C. Hartsthorne & P. Weiss (Eds.). Collected papers of Charles Saunders Peirce. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

  • PEREIRA-DELGADO, J. A., & OBLAK, M. (2007). The effects of daily intensive tact instruction on the emission of pure mands and tacts in noninstructional settings by three preschool children with developmental delays, Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4(2), 392–411. Retrieved May 1, 2007, from http://www.behavior-analyst-online.org

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • PETURSDOTTIR, A. I., CARR, J. E., & MICHAELS, J. (2005). Emergence of mands and tacts among preschool children. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 59–74.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • PINKER, S. (1999). Words and rules. New York: Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • PISTOLJEVIC, N. (2008). The effects of multiple exemplar training and intensive tact instructional histories on the acquisition of naming in preschoolers. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University, New York.

    Google Scholar 

  • PISTOLJEVIC, N., & GREER, R. D. (2006). The effects of daily intensive tact instruction on preschool students’ emission of pure tacts and mands in non-instructional setting. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 103–120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • POULSON, C. L., KYMISS, E., REEVE, K. F., ANDREATOS, M., & REEVE, L. (1991). Generalized vocal imitation in infant vocal conditioning. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 267–279.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (1976). Intelligence in ape and man. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • PREMACK, D. (2004, January 16). Is language key to human intelligence? Science, 303, 318–320.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • REILLY-LAWSON, T. (2008). Phonemic control as the source of derived relations between naming and reading and writing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • REILLY-LAWSON, T., & GREER, R. D. (2006). Teaching the function of writing to middle school students with academic delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 3, 151–169.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • REILLY-LAWSON, T., & WALSH, D. (2007). The effects of observational training on the acquisition of reinforcement for listening. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 430–452. Retrieved May 1, 2007, from http://www.behavior-analyst-online.org

    Google Scholar 

  • ROBINSON, A. (1995). The story of writing: Alphabets, hieroglyphs, and pictograms. London: Thames & Hudson Ltd.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROCHE, B., & BARNES-HOLMES, D. (1997). A transformation of respondently conditioned functions in accordance with arbitrarily applicable relations, Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 67, 275–301.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ROGERS-WARREN, A. R., & BAER, D. M. (1976). Correspondence between saying and doing: Teaching children to share and praise. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 9, 335–354.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ROSALES-RUIZ, J., & BAER, D. M. (1996). A behavior-analytic view of development. In S. Bijou & E. Ribes (Eds.), New directions in behavior development. Nevada: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • ROSS, D. E., & GREER, R. D. (2003). Generalized imitation and the mand: Inducing first instances of speech in young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 24, 58–74.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SAVAGE-RUMBAUGH, E. S. (1984). Verbal behavior at a procedural level in the chimpanzee. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43, 5–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • SCHAUFFLER, G., & GREER, R. D. (2006). The effects of intensive tact instruction on audience-accurate tacts and conversational units. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 120–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • SELINSKI, J., GREER, R. D., & LODHI, S. (1991). A functional analysis of the comprehensive application of behavior analysis to schooling. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 24, 108–118.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1986). Functional analysis of emergent classes. In T. Thompson & M. Zeiler (Eds.), Analysis and integration of behavioral units (pp. 213–245). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • SIDMAN, M. (1994). Stimulus equivalence: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • SINGER-DUDEK, J. GREER, R. D., & SCHMELZKOPF, J. (2008). The effects of an observational intervention on the acquisition of reinforcing properties of a previously neutral stimulus. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 5, 57–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group and the B. F. Skinner Foundation.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1968). A science and technology of teaching. New York: Appleton Century Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • SKINNER, B. F. (1986). The evolution of verbal behavior, Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 115–122.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SPECKMAN, J., & GREER, R. D. (2006). Multiple exemplar instruction and the emergence of generative production of suffixes as autoclitic frames. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • SPECKMAN-COLLINS, J., PARK, H. S., & GREER, R. D. (2007). Generalized selection-based auditory matching and the emergence of the listener component of Naming. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavior Intervention, 4, 412–429.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • STAATS, A. W. (1968). Learning, language, and cognition: Theory, research, and method for the study of human behavior and its development. New York: Holt, Rinehart, and Winston.

    Google Scholar 

  • STEMMER, N. (1992). The behavior of the listener, generic extension, and the communicative adequacy of verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 10, 69–80.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SUNDBERG, C. T., & SUNDBERG, M. L. (1990). Comparing topography-based verbal behavior with stimulus selection-based verbal behavior. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 8, 31–41.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SUNDBERG, M. L., MICHAEL, J., PARTINGTON, J. W., & SUNDBERG, C. A. (1996). The role of automatic reinforcement in early language acquisition. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 21–37.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • TSIOURI, I., & GREER, R. D. (2007). Different social reinforcement contingencies in inducing echoics to tacts through motor imitation responding in children with severe language delays. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3.

  • TSIOURI, I., & GREER, R. D. (2003). Inducing vocal verbal behavior through rapid motor imitation training in young children with language delays. Journal of Behavioral Education, 12, 185–206.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TSAI, H., & GREER, R. D. (2006). Conditioned preference for books and faster acquisition of textual responses by preschool children. Journal of Early and Intensive Behavioral Interventions, 3, 35–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TWYMAN, J. S. (1996). The functional independence of impure mands and tacts of abstract stimulus properties. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 13, 1–19.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • UTTAL, W. R. (2001). The new phrenology: The limit of localizing cognitive processes in the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • VARGAS, E. A. (1982). Intraverbal behavior: The codic, duplic, and sequelic subtypes. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 1, 5–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WILLIAMS, G., & GREER, R. D. (1993). A comparison of verbal-behavior and linguistic communication curricula for training developmentally delayed adolescents to acquire and maintain vocal speech. Behaviorology, 1, 31–46.

    Google Scholar 

  • YOON, S. Y. (1998). Effects of an adult’s vocal sound paired with a reinforcing event on the subsequent acquisition of mand functions (Doctoral dissertation, Columbia University). Abstract from: UMI Proquest Digital Dissertations [online]. Dissertations Abstracts Item: AAT 9839031.

    Google Scholar 

  • ZENTALL, T. R. (1996). An analysis of social learning in animals. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef, Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of culture (pp. 221–243). San Diego: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. Douglas Greer.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Greer, R.D., Speckman, J. The Integration of Speaker and Listener Responses: A Theory of Verbal Development. Psychol Rec 59, 449–488 (2009). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395674

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395674

Navigation