Skip to main content
Log in

The Emergence of the Listener to Speaker Component of Naming in Children as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction

  • Published:
The Analysis of Verbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We tested the effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transfer of stimulus function for unfamiliar pictures across listener responses (i.e., matching and pointing) and speaker responses (i.e., pure tacts and impure tacts). Three preschool students, who were 3- and 4-year-old males and did not have the listener to speaker component of the naming repertoire, participated in the experiment. The dependent variable was numbers of correct responses to probe trials of both untaught listener responses (“point to__”) and speaker responses (tact and impure tacts) following mastery of matching responses for two sets of five unfamiliar pictures (Set 1 and Set 3). After each participant mastered matching (e.g., “match Labrador”) for Set 1 pictures they were probed on the three untaught responses to Set 1 words. That is, they were asked to point to Labrador, tact the picture of Labrador, and respond to the picture of a Labrador and the question “What is this?” Next, the participants were taught mastery of all four types of responses using MEI for a second set of five pictures (Set 2) and probed again on the 3 untaught Set 1 responses. Finally, matching responses were taught to mastery for a novel set of pictures (Set 3) and then probed on the three untaught responses. The results showed that untaught speaker responses emerged at 60% to 85% for two participants, and 40%- 70% for one participant. We discuss the role of instructional history in the development of the listener to speaker component of naming.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bahadorian, AJ. (2000). The effect of the learn unit on student’s performance in two university courses. Unpublished Ph.D. dissertation, Columbia University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Cullinan, V (2001). Relational frame theory and Skinner’s Verbal behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 69–84.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, W. (1992). Direct instruction: A twenty-year review. In R. West & L. Hamerlynck, Design for educational excellence: The legacy ofB. F. Skinner (pp. 71–112). Longmont, CO: Sopris West.

    Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (1998). Learning (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Donley, C. R., & Greer, R. D. (1993). Setting events controlling social verbal exchanges between students with developmental delays. Journal of Behavioral Education, 3 (4), 387–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Emurian, H. H., Hu, X., Wang, J., & Durham, D. (2000). Learning JAVA: A programmed instruction approach using applets. Computers in Human Behavior, 16, 395–422

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fields, L., Reeve, K. F., Mateneja, P., Varelas, A., Belanich, J., Fitzer, A., & Shannon, K. (2003). The formation of a generalized categorization repertoire: Effects of training with multiple domains, samples, and comparisons. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 291–313

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D. (2003). Designing teaching strategies: An applied behavior analysis systems approach. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Chavez-Brown, M, Nirgudkar, A., Steffi, L., & Rivera-Valdes, (July 2003). The effects of a procedure to teach fluent listener responses to students with autism and changes in the numbers of learn units students required to meet instructional objectives. Paper presented at the First Congress of the European Association for Behavior Analysis, Parma, Italy.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & Keohane, D. (2004). The evolution of verbal behavior. Manuscript submitted for publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Keohane, D. D., & Healey, O. (2002). Quality and applied behavior analysis. The Behavior Analyst Today, 3 (1), 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D. & McCorkle, N. (2003). CABAS® international curriculum and inventory of repertoires from preschool through kinder garten. Yonkers, NY: CABAS® and the Fred S. Keller School.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., & McDonough, S. (1999). Is the learn-unit the fundamental measure of pedagogy? The Behavior Analyst, 20, 5–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Nirgudkar, A., & Park, H. (2003b). The effect of multiple exemplar instruction on the transformation of mand and tact functions. Paper Presented at the International Conference of the Association for Behavior Analysis, San Francisco, CA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Greer, R. D., Yaun L., & Gautreaux, G (2005). Novel Dictation and Intraverbal Responses as a Function of a Multiple Exemplar Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 21, 99–116.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C, Gifford, E. V, Wilson, K. G, Barnes-Holmes, D., & Healy, O. (2001). Derived relational responding as learned behavior. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relationalframe theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 21–49). New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Home, P. J., & Lowe, C. F. (1996). On the origins of naming and other symbolic behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 65, 185–241.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Horner, R. D., & Baer, D. M. (1978). Multiple-probe technique: Avariation on the multiple baseline. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 189–196.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lodhi, S. & Greer, R.D. (1989). The speaker as listener. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 51, 353–360.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Lowe, C. F., Home, P. J., Harris, D. S., & Rändle, V. R. L. (2002). Naming and categorization in young children: Vocal tact training. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 78, 527–549.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nuzzolo-Gomez, R. & Greer, R. D. (2004). Emergence of Untaught Mands or Tacts with Novel Adjective-Object Pairs as a Function of Instructional History. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 24, 30–47

    Google Scholar 

  • Pinker, S. (1999). Words and rules. New York: Perennial.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1989). The behavior of the listener. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies and instructional control (85–96). New York: Plenum.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B.F. (1957,1992). Verbal behavior. Acton, MA: Copley Publishing Group.

    Book  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to R. Douglas Greer.

Additional information

We thank the staff, students, and families of The Fred S. Keller School for their participation and continued support.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Greer, R.D., Stolfi, L., Chavez-Brown, M. et al. The Emergence of the Listener to Speaker Component of Naming in Children as a Function of Multiple Exemplar Instruction. Analysis Verbal Behav 21, 123–134 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393014

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03393014

Key words

Navigation