Skip to main content
Log in

Universal and language-specific constraints on phonemic awareness: evidence from Russian–Hebrew bilingual children

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study tested phonemic awareness in the two languages of Russian (L1)–Hebrew (L2) sequential bilingual children (N = 20) using phoneme deletion tasks where the phoneme to be deleted occurred word initial, word final, as a singleton, or part of a cluster, in long and short words and stressed and unstressed syllables. The experiments were designed to test the effect of four linguistic factors on children’s phoneme deletion: phoneme position (initial, final), linguistic context (singleton, cluster), word length and stress. The results indicated that word length and stress confirmed previous findings in other languages demonstrating the universal validity of these factors. However, phoneme position and linguistic context gave rise to novel findings in the languages studied and provided evidence for language-specific effects on phonemic awareness reflecting onset-rime versus body-coda syllable structure differences. The results are discussed within the framework of universal versus language-specific constraints on phonemic awareness performance in different languages.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The number of stimulus items in the two languages was different because final clustered phonemes, which hardly exist in Hebrew but are frequent in Russian, were only targeted in Russian but not in Hebrew.

  2. This condition was tested in Russian only.

  3. Cluster versus singleton conditions were manipulated in monosyllabic words only. This is because high familiarity bi-syllabic words with consonantal clusters are rare in the Hebrew lexicon of preschool children.

  4. This condition was tested in Russian only.

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. London: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aitchison, J., & Straf, M. (1981). Lexical storage and retrieval: A developing skill? Linguistics, 19, 751–795.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14, 575–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bat-El, O. (1993). Parasitic metrification in the modern Hebrew stress system. The Linguistic Review, 10, 189–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, M. (2003). Hebrew stress: Can’t you hear those trochees? In Kaiser & Arunachalam (Eds.) Proceedings of PLC 26 (Vol. 9.1, pp. 45–58).

  • Ben-Dror, I., Frost, R., & Bentin, S. (1995). Orthographic representation and phonemic segmentation in skilled readers: A cross-language comparison. Psychological Science, 6, 176–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bolozky, S. (2006). A note on initial consonant clusters in Israeli Hebrew. Hebrew Studies, XLVII, 227–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bondarko, L. V. (1969). The syllable structure of speech and distinctive features of phonemes. Phonetica, 20, 1–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booij, G. E. (1983). Principles and parameters in prosodic phonology. Linguistics, 21, 249–280.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. R., & Perfetti, C. A. (2002). Onset and rime structure influences naming but not early word identification in children and adults. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 1–23.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 313, 419–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., Genesee, F., & Caravolas, M. (1997). A cross-linguistic study of early literacy acquisition. In B. Blachman (Ed.), Foundations of reading acquisition and dyslexia: Implications for early intervention (pp. 145–162). London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M., & Treiman, R. (1990). Phonological awareness and spelling in normal children and dyslexics: The case of initial consonant clusters. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 156–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M., Bradley, L. L., & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phonemic detection, and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1989). Phonemic awareness and letter knowledge in the child’s acquisition of the alphabetic principle. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 313–321.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1990). Acquiring the alphabetic principle: A case for teaching recognition of phoneme identity. Journal of Educational Psychology, 28, 805–812.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Byrne, B., & Fielding-Barnsley, R. (1993). Evaluating a program to teach phonemic awareness to young children: A 1-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 104–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Caravolas, M., & Bruck, M. (1993). The effect of oral and written language input on children’s phonological awareness: A cross-linguistic study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 55, 1–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carroll, J. M., & Snowling, M. J. (2001). The effects of global similarity between stimuli on children’s judgment of rime and alliteration. Applied Psycholinguistics, 22, 327–342.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiat, S. (1983). Why Mikey’s right and my key’s is wrong: The significance of stress and word boundaries in a child output system. Cognition, 14, 275–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen-Gross, D. (2003). Hamivne hahavarti shel ha_ivrit hexadasha [Syllable structure in Modern Hebrew. In O. Schwarzwald, S. Blum-Kulka, & E. Olshtein (Eds.), A tribute to Raphael Nir: Studies in communication, linguistics, and language teaching (pp. 359–369). Jerusalem, Israel: Carmel (in Hebrew).

  • De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2002). Statistical analysis of similarity relations among spoken words: Evidence for the special status of rimes in English. Behavioural Research Methods and Instrumentation, 34, 416–423.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Cara, B., & Goswami, U. (2003). Phonological neighbourhood density effects in a rhyme awareness task in 5-year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 30, 695–710.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, L. G., Seymour, P. H. K., & Hill, S. (1997). How important are rhymes and analogy in beginning reading? Cognition, 63, 171–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Pallier, C., Sebastián, N., & Mehler, J. (1997). A destressing “deafness” in French? Journal of Memory and Language, 36, 406–421.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dupoux, E., Peperkamp, S., & Sebastián-Gallés, N. (2001). A robust method to study stress ‘deafness’. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 110, 1606–1618.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoglu, A. Y. (2002). Cross-linguistic transfer in literacy development and implications for language learners. Annals of Dyslexia, 52, 189–204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Durgunoğlu, A. Y., & Öney, B. (1999). A cross-linguistic comparison of phonological awareness and word recognition. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 11, 281–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eviatar, Z., Leikin, M., & Ibrahim, R. (1999). Phonological processing in second language phonemes: A selective deficit in bilingual aphasic. Language Learning, 49, 121–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fudge, E. C. (1969). Syllables. Journal of Linguistics, 5, 193–320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fudge, E. C. (1987). Branching structure within the syllable. Journal of Linguistics, 23, 359–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Genesee, F., & Geva, E. (2006). Cross-linguistic relationships in working memory, phonological processes, and oral language. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second language learners: A report of the national literacy panel on language minority children and youth (chapter 7, pp. 175–184). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geudens, A., & Sandra, D. (2003). Beyond implicit phonological knowledge: No support for an onset-rime structure in children’s explicit phonological awareness. Journal of Memory and Language, 49, 157–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geudens, A., Sandra, D., & Martensen, (2005). Rhyming words and onset-rime constituents: An inquiry into structural breaking points and emergent boundaries in the syllable. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 366–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geudens, A., Sandra, D., & van den Broek, W. (2004). Segmenting two-phoneme syllables: Developmental differences in relation with early reading skills. Brain and Language, 90, 338–352.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E. (2006). Second-language oral proficiency and second-language literacy. In D. August & T. Shanahan (Eds.), Developing literacy in second-language learners: A report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth (chapter 5, pp. 123–140). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goetry, V., Wade-Woolley, L., Kolinsky, R., & Mousty, P. (2006). The role of stress processing abilities in the development of bilingual reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 349–362.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldsmith, J. (1990). Autosegmental and metrical phonology. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1986). Children’s use of analogy in learning to read: A developmental study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 73–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1988). Orthographic analogies and reading development. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 40A, 239–268.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological skills and learning to read. East Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., & East, M. (2000). Rhyme and analogy in beginning reading: Conceptual and methodological issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 63–93.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Thomson, J., Richardson, U., Stainthorp, R., Hughes, D., Rosen, S., et al. (2002). Amplitude envelope onsets and developmental dyslexia: A new hypothesis. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science, 99, 10911–10916.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Graf, D. (2000). Stress assignment in the nominal system of Modern Hebrew (MH). In A. Z. Wyner (Ed.), The proceedings of the 15th annual conference of IATL, Israel (Vol. 7).

  • Graf, D. (2003). Prosodic control: Consonants clusters in Modern Hebrew. In The Proceedings of the 11th Manchester Phonology Meeting, Manchester, England.

  • Halle, M. (1971). The sound pattern of Russian. The Hague: Mouton & Co.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jiménez González, J. E., & Haro García, C. R. (1995). Effects of word linguistic properties on phonological awareness in Spanish children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 193–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jusczyk, P., Cutler, A., & Redanz, N. (1993). Infants’ preference for the predominant stress patterns of English words. Child Development, 64, 675–687.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, Y.-S. (2007). Phonological awareness and literacy skills in Korean: An examination of the unique role of body-coda units. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 69–94.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirtley, C., Bryant, P. E., MacLean, M. J., & Bradley, L. L. (1989). Rhyme, rime and the onset of reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 48, 224–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Knyazev, S. V. (1999). O kriteriyah slogodeleniya v sovremennom russkom yazike: Teoriya volni, sonornosti i teoriya optimalnosti. Voprosi Yazikoznaniya (The criteria of syllabifications in Modern Russian language: Wave Theory, Sonority Theory, and Optimality Theory). Issues on Linguistics, 1, 84–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koda, K. (2007). Reading and language learning: Crosslinguistic constraints on second language reading development. Language Learning, 57, 1–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levin, I., Saiegh-Haddad, E., Hindi, N., & Ziv, M. (2008). Early literacy in Arabic: An intervention study among Israeli Palestinian kindergartners. Applied Psycholinguistics., 29, 413–436.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lewkowics, N., & Low, L. (1979). Effects of visual aids and word structure on phonemic segmentation. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 4, 238–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Fischer, F. W., & Carter, B. (1974). Explicit syllable and phoneme segmentation in the young child. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 18, 201–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 1–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C. (1995). What is phonological awareness? Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 179–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L. (1997). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken word recognition. Memory and Cognition, 25, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mixdorff, H., & Ami, N. (2002). The prosody of Modern Hebrew—a quantitative study. In Proceedings of SP-2002 conference (pp. 511–514).

  • Muneaux, M., Ziegler, J. C., Truc, C., Thomson, J., & Goswami, U. (2004). Deficits in beat perception and dyslexia: Evidence from French. NeuroReport, 15, 1255–1259.

    Google Scholar 

  • National Reading Panel. (2000). Report of the national reading panel teaching children to read: An evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Washington, DC: National Institute of Child Health and Human Development.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., Bertoncini, J., & Mehler, J. (1998). Language discrimination by newborns: Towards an understanding of the role of rhythm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 24, 756–766.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nazzi, T., & Ramus, F. (2003). Perception and acquisition of linguistic rhythm by infants. Speech Communication, 41, 233–243.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., & Liu, Y. (2005). Orthography to phonology and meaning: Comparisons across and within writing systems. Reading and writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 193–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, U., Thomson, J. M., Scott, S. K., & Goswami, U. (2004). Auditory processing skills and phonological representation in dyslexic children. Dyslexia, 10, 215–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2003). Linguistic distance and initial reading acquisition: The case of Arabic diglossia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 115–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2004). The impact of phonemic and lexical distance on the phonological analysis of words and pseudo words in a diglossic context. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 495–512.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2005). Correlates of reading fluency in Arabic: Diglossic and orthographic factors. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 18, 559–582.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007a). Linguistic constraints on children’s ability to isolate phonemes in Arabic. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 607–625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Saiegh-Haddad, E. (2007b). Epilinguistic and metalinguistic awareness may be subject to different constraints. First Language, 27, 385–405.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., Blair, R., & Rvachew, S. (2006). Rimes are not necessarily favoured by pre-readers: Evidence from meta- and epi-linguistic phonological tasks. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 183–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schreuder, R., & van Bon, W. H. J. (1989). Phonemic analysis: Effects of word properties. Journal of Research in Reading, 12, 59–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., Geva, E., Share, D., & Leikin, M. (2007). Learning to read in English as a third language: The cross-linguistic transfer of phonological processing skills. Written Language and Literacy, 10, 25–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, M., Leikin, M., & Share, D. L. (2005). Bi-literate bilingualism versus mono-literate bilingualism: A longitudinal study of reading acquisition in Hebrew (L2) among Russian-speaking (L1) children. Written Language and Literacy, 8, 179–207.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy skills in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L., & Blum, P. (2005). Syllable splitting in literate and preliterate Hebrew speakers: Onsets and rimes or bodies and codas? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 182–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2000). Dyslexia (2nd ed.). Oxford: UK, Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, A. (1998). Phonology. UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stahl, S. A., & Murray, B. A. (1994). Defining phonological awareness and its relationship to early reading. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 221–234.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanback, M. L. (1992). Syllable and rime patterns for teaching reading: Analysis of a frequency-based vocabulary of 17, 602 words. Annals of Dyslexia, 42, 196–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E., Cunningham, A. E., & Cramer, B. B. (1984). Assessing phonological awareness in kindergarten children: Issues of task comparability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 175–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, M. (2005). Phonemic analysis and reading development: Some current issues. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 39–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Swan, D., & Goswami, U. (1997). Phonological awareness deficits in developmental dyslexia and the phonological representations hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 66, 18–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thomas, E., & Senechal, M. (2004). Long-term association between articulation quality and phoneme sensitivity: A study from age 3 to age 8. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 513–541.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1983). The structure of spoken syllables: Evidence from novel word games. Cognition, 15, 49–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1985). Onsets and rimes as units of spoken syllables: Evidence from children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 39, 161–181.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1988). The internal structure of the syllable. In G. Carlson & M. Tanenhaus (Eds.), Linguistic structure in language processing (pp. 27–52). Norwell, MA: Kluwer Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (1991). Children’s spelling errors on syllable-initial consonant clusters. Journal of Educational Psychology, 83, 346–360.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Berch, D., & Weatherson, S. (1993). Children’s use of phoneme-grapheme correspondences in spelling: Roles of position and stress. Journal of Educational Psychology, 85, 466–477.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995a). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., & Weatherson, S. (1992). Effects of linguistic structure on children’s ability to isolate initial consonants. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 174–201.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R., Zukowski, A., & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995b). What happened to the “n” of sink? Children’s spellings of final consonant clusters. Cognition, 55, 1–38.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vinarskaya E. N., Lepskaya N. I., & Bogomazov, G. M. (1977). Pravila slogodeleniya i slogovye modeli (ne materiale detskoy rechi). Problemi teoreticheskoy i eskperemental’noj fonetiki. Publikacii otdeleniya strukturnoy i prikladnoj lingvistiki MGU. The rules for syllabification and syllable models (the analysis of children’s speech). The problems of theoretical and experimental phonetics. Publications of the department of structural and applied linguistics of Moscow State University. Issue 8. Moscow.

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ward, D. (1965). The Russian language today: System and anomaly. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C. (2004). Speech rhythm processing in young children and its relation to phonological awareness. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the society for the scientific studies of reading, Amsterdam.

  • Wood, C. (2006a). Sensitivity to speech rhythm and the development of reading. Trends in cognitive psychology research. New York: Nova Science.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C. (2006b). Metrical stress sensitivity in young children and its relationship to phonological awareness and reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 29, 270–287.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wood, C., & Terrell, C. (1998). Poor readers’ ability to detect speech rhythm and perceive rapid speech. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 16, 397–413.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, H. K., Bolger, D. J., Kwon, O. S., & Perfetti, C. A. (2002). Subsyllabic units in reading. A difference between Korean and English. In L. Verhoeven, C. Elbo, & P. Reitsma (Eds.), Precursors of functional literacy. Amsterdam/Philadelphia: John Benjamins Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yopp, H. K. (1988). The validity and reliability of phonemic awareness tests. Reading Research Quarterly, 23, 159–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zaretsky, E. (2002). Effects of oral language on sound segmentation skills: Cross linguistic evidence. In W. Fay & K. M. Louise (Eds.), Investigations in clinical phonetics and linguistics (pp. 201–212). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Elinor Saiegh-Haddad.

Appendices

Appendix A

Phonological awareness tasks

Russian

Stimulus words for initial phoneme deletion

Practice trials: лoкoть, мopoз, pyкaв, кycoк, дecять, лaдoнь, кaмeнь, кoвёp, нoмep

Experimental trials: xaлaт, caxap, лoшaдь, внyк, вeчep, бaнт, мocт, cтyл, кycт, двepь, гocть, дивaн, нocoк, кoнeц, лиcт, дpyг, нoжик, цвeт, мaлыш, выxoд

Stimulus words for final phoneme deletion

Practice trials: coceд, вoceмь, ceвep, гopox, пaмять, любoвь, cиpoп, пecoк, зaпax

Experimental trials: тopт, cмex, вeтep, дoждь, cвeт, мecяц, глaз, пeтyx, живoт, дeвять, билeт, гopoд, xлeб, pocт, зoнт, дypaк, caлaт, вxoд, пapк, гoлoc

Hebrew

Stimulus words for initial phoneme deletion

Practice trials: ורד, סדר, סכ’ן, גבר, כסף, סד’ן, ר’קוד, סרט, פנס

Experimental trials: חדר, צמח, מלון, פקק, צחוק, לשון, חתן, זמן, גשר, פרס, חודש, חלב, מלך, ס’פור, חלון, קצת

Stimulus words for final phoneme deletion

Practice trials: דלת, בצל, מרק, גזר, סוכר, גשר, ספר, בגד, בשר, חלום, מפ’ת

Experimental trials: כרוב, סתו, כב’ש, ת’נוק, לחם, כפר, כדור, בוקר, דבשה דרך, בטן, כר’ת, כלב, בלו

Appendix B

Russian letter discrimination

Practice trials

1

Г

Г

Same

2

H

И

Dif

Experimental trials

1

З

Э

Dif

2

Ш

C

Dif

3

Щ

Ч

Dif

4

З

З

Same

5

П

Ц

Dif

6

Г

Г

Same

7

Э

E

Dif

8

Ж

К

Dif

9

Б

Г

Dif

10

Я

Я

Same

11

Д

T

Dif

12

И

У

Dif

13

O

У

Dif

14

A

Л

Dif

15

Ш

Щ

Dif

16

A

Я

Dif

17

Д

Д

Same

18

P

B

Dif

19

B

З

Dif

20

Б

Б

Same

Russian letter identification/naming

Practice trials

1

B

2

Л

Experimental trials

1

Ф

2

Ë

3

P

4

Ц

5

Г

6

H

7

Ж

8

C

9

З

10

Ч

11

У

12

A

13

К

14

П

15

X

16

Э

17

Ш

18

Щ

19

Ы

20

Б

Hebrew letter discrimination

Practice trials

1

ג

ז

2

ן

ן

Experimental trials

1

ר

ד

2

ק

ק

3

ב

ב

4

ה

ח

5

ס

ם

6

ק

ח

7

ז

ב

8

ג

ג

9

נ

ג

10

ז

ו

11

ב

ת

12

ס

ס

13

ן

ך

14

פ

כ

15

 

 

16

ס

 

17

ך

ך

18

ת

ח

19

 

 

20

ט

ד

Hebrew letter identification/naming

Practice trials

1

ם

2

 

Experimental trials

1

 

2

כ

3

י

4

ר

5

ה

6

א

7

ז

8

ך

9

ק

10

ס

11

 

12

ד

13

ןו

14

 

15

ג

16

ח

17

ט

18

נ

19

ם

20

פ

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Saiegh-Haddad, E., Kogan, N. & Walters, J. Universal and language-specific constraints on phonemic awareness: evidence from Russian–Hebrew bilingual children. Read Writ 23, 359–384 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9204-8

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-009-9204-8

Keywords

Navigation