The amount of visual information available at any moment is far too sizable for our nervous system to handle. This requires some prioritization of information so as not to overwhelm the system. The enhancement of relevant stimuli and the suppression of irrelevant stimuli are, thus, crucial for survival. Attention serves to prioritize important stimuli by increasing the neural responses to attended ones and decreasing the neural responses to unattended ones (see Kanwisher & Wojciulik, 2000, for review). In certain circumstances, attention is able to select information on the basis of objectness (see Chen, 2012, for a review). Attending to an object enhances the properties of that object, such that (after reporting one object feature) it becomes easier to report a second feature of the attended object, versus reporting a feature of an unattended object (Duncan, 1984). The focus of the present study was to broaden our knowledge of the conditions under which attention is object-based.

In a seminal study, Egly, Driver, and Rafal (1994) investigated the effects of both space-based and object-based attention within a single paradigm. Two rectangles were shown on each trial. One end of a rectangle was cued, followed by a target at one of three locations: (1) the cued end of the rectangle (valid location), (2) the uncued end of the cued rectangle (same-object location), or (3) the equidistant end of the uncued rectangle (different-object location). Their results showed that less time was required to detect the target at the cued location than any other location—evidence of spatial cueing. Critically, target detection was also faster at the same-object than at the different-object location. This performance difference, often referred to as the “same-object advantage,” cannot be explained by spatial attention, because the two invalid locations were equidistant from the cue. Hence, the same-object advantage reflects the attentional priority assigned to the cued object—that is, object-based attention. Further studies using the double-rectangle paradigm have shown that both detection (Atchley & Kramer, 2001; He, Fan, Zhou, & Chen, 2004; Lamy & Egeth, 2002) and discrimination (Drummond & Shomstein, 2010; Macquistan, 1997; Moore, Yantis, & Vaughan, 1998; Shomstein & Behrmann, 2008; Shomstein & Yantis, 2004) performance exhibit a same-object advantage.

Nearly all object-based attention studies have investigated covert attention—that is, attention in the absence of eye movements. In everyday life, however, people tend to fixate objects of interest via saccades (overt attention). Thus, in the interest of ecological validity, it is important to know whether object-based selection occurs for overt attention—in other words, whether saccades are affected by the deployment of attention to the whole object once a part of the object has been selected.

The relationship between saccades and space-based covert attention has been of great interest to researchers. In particular, many studies have investigated how space-based attention affects the latency to initiate saccades, and have found that attention can have a small, but significant, effect on saccade latency. For example, Crawford and Müller (1992) presented exogenous cues followed by simple visual targets in a detection task and instructed participants to either execute an eye movement to the target or a keypress. They found that both saccade latency and keypress reaction time (RT) to targets at the cued location were faster than to other locations. These results suggest that exogenously driven space-based attention accelerates saccade preparation. Further studies have also shown that endogenous space-based attention, as manipulated by reward and target predictability, influences saccade latency. For example, differentially rewarding saccades to a specific location led to faster latencies to higher-rewarded locations than to other target locations (Kawagoe, Takikawa, & Hikosaka, 1998; Rothkirch, Ostendorf, Sax, & Sterzer, 2013; Takikawa, Kawagoe, Itoh, Nakahara, & Hikosaka, 2002). Likewise, as the probability of target appearance increases at a location, the saccade latency to that location becomes faster (Abrams & Jonides, 1988; Dorris & Munoz, 1998; Murray & Giggey, 2006). These studies established that saccade latency is a useful metric to index the deployment of attention.

The abovementioned studies on saccade latencies are consistent with the general notion that covert and overt attention share largely similar mechanisms, which is supported by findings showing coupling between saccades and attention (Deubel & Schneider, 1996; Godijn & Pratt, 2002; Hoffman & Subramaniam, 1995; Kowler, Anderson, Dosher, & Blaser, 1995; Shepherd, Findlay, & Hockey, 1986). Indeed, the premotor theory of attention suggests that overt and covert attention are the same phenomena (see Rizzolatti, Riggio, Dascola, & Umiltà, 1987; Sheliga, Riggio, & Rizzolatti, 1994). The similarity between covert and overt attention was further supported by physiological findings showing that saccade preparation and covert attention are processed in the same brain regions (for a review, see Awh, Armstrong, & Moore, 2006). Although the coupling between covert and overt attention is generally accepted, some counterevidence has also challenged the strong coupling between covert and overt spatial attention. For example, attention is not always deployed to the saccade-prepared location (Hunt & Kingstone, 2003; Klein, 1980), and attentional deployment does not necessarily engender saccade preparation (Juan, Shorter-Jacobi, & Schall, 2004). In addition, saccade preparation deploys attention to the saccade-prepared location much faster than deployment of covert attention (Rolfs & Carrasco, 2012). Lastly, physiological studies suggest that different sub-populations of neurons process attention and saccades (Juan et al., 2004; Sato & Schall, 2003; Thompson, Bichot, & Schall, 1997; Thompson, Biscoe, & Sato, 2005). Thus, although covert and overt spatial attention are usually correlated, they do not have identical properties (see also Smith & Schenk, 2012). Given the observed dissociations between overt and covert attention, one might not predict that saccades will follow the same pattern as covert attention in observing object-based effects as measured in the two-rectangle paradigm.

These considerations thus led us to investigate whether object-based attention also influences saccade latency. Few studies have investigated the effect of object-based attention on eye movements. McCarley, Kramer, and Peterson (2002) and Theeuwes, Mathôt, and Kingstone (2010) used the double-rectangle cueing paradigm to investigate how object-based attention affects eye movements. In their experiments, after the initial spatial cue disappeared, participants first made a saccade to the cued location, which was followed by the presentation of three distractor letters and one target, each located at one end of one rectangle. The task was to report the identity/orientation of a small target letter that required foveating. Participants, thus, made a second saccade to the target, which became the focus of the analyses. Both studies revealed that the second saccade tended to stay within the cued object. Furthermore, McCarley et al., (2002) found that latency for the second saccade to the same-object location was faster than to the different-object location, although such a difference was not observed in the Theeuwes et al., (2010)’s study. A more recent experiment with a similar design using real-world pictures also reported that the second saccade latency to the same-object location was faster than the different-object location (Malcolm & Shomstein, 2015).

These results imply a relationship between object-based attention and eye movements, but they are inconclusive for several reasons. First, an effect of object-based attention on second saccade latencies was not consistently observed. Second, there was a potential low-level confound in the results for the second saccade, which in these studies always began at the cued location. However, once the eyes had landed on the cued location, there were also several retinal-level differences between the same-object and different-object locations. One of these was the presence of obstructing object edges in the path to the different-object location (which could impede saccades to this location), but an absence of such edges for the same-object location. More importantly, the saccade path to the same-object location was delineated by elongated lines (those of the rectangle edges) that were absent on the saccade path to the different-object location. “Following elongated edges” can produce the same effect as “obstructing object edges”—they both lead to faster RTs at the same-object location than the different-object location. Importantly, however, there is empirical evidence for the “following elongated edges” idea, such that the presence of contour lines on the saccade path biases eye movements along the direction of the contours (Wismeijer & Gegenfurtner, 2012). Thus, the same-object advantage of eye movements in these studies might have been caused by the presence of guiding lines to the same-object location rather than by the effects of object-based attention.

Notably, in these studies, the researchers did not focus on the first saccade, which is arguably a better measure of the initial distribution of attention to the entire display. Furthermore, at the time of the first saccade, the visual scene is symmetric, so low-level retinal features are equated (which serves to avoid the confound of guiding contours and obstructing edges detailed above). Our aim here was to investigate the effect of object-based attention on first saccade latencies in the double-rectangle paradigm. We also varied the nature of the initial cue, using exogenous, endogenous, and hybrid spatial cues, to examine the generality of the coupling between object-based attention and saccade preparation. In three experiments, we first measured the effect of object-based attention via conventional keypress RTs, which provided a baseline measure. The saccade experiments used identical visual stimuli, but participants made a rapid eye movement to the target. We measured the latency of the first saccade to the target as an index of the initial distribution of attention. To preview our results, we found strong evidence that object-based attention facilitated both keypress RTs and saccade latencies.

Experiment 1: Hybrid cue

The double-rectangle cueing paradigm first introduced by Egly et al. (1994) used a hybrid cue—that is, a peripheral cue that is also predictive of the target location. Previous research using the double-rectangle paradigm with hybrid cues has yielded results supporting Egly et al., (1994)’s original findings (Atchley & Kramer, 2001; Moore et al., 1998; Shomstein & Behrmann, 2008). Here, we also performed a replication of Egly et al.’s original finding under our experimental setup, which serves as a point of departure to explore object-based effects in a novel saccade task. In our first experiment with keypress responses (Exp. 1a), we predicted both space-based effects and object-based effects in keypress RTs. We then conducted an experiment to investigate whether the effect of object-based attention can be detected using saccade latency (Exp. 1b). If object-based attention affects eye movement preparation, we should find analogous effects in saccade latency: faster first saccade latencies for targets appearing at the invalid same-object location than for those at the invalid different-object location.

Experiment 1a

Method

Participants

Sixteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment: all were students from Michigan State University. One participants’ data were excluded due to excessive premature responses (61.25 % of trials). Participants had normal, or corrected-to-normal, vision and did not have any neurological or psychiatric disorders. They were compensated with course extra credit or $10/h, with a bonus of $5 after the completion of the experiment. The experimental protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board at Michigan State University.

Apparatus and visual stimuli

The experiment was programmed in MATLAB (The Mathworks, Natick, MA) with MGL extensions (gru.stanford.edu/doku.php/mgl/overview). The stimuli were displayed on a 19–in. CRT monitor (resolution: 1,024 × 768 pixels) at a vertical refresh rate of 100 Hz. The viewing distance was 68 cm ensured via the use of a chinrest.

The background of the display remained black (luminance: 0.051 cd/m2) throughout the experiment. The fixation cross (line length: 0.05° × 0.05°, line width: 0.03° × 0.03°) was a gray (luminance: 16.19 cd/m2) “plus sign” located at the center of the screen. Following Egly et al. (1994), we used two rectangle outlines as objects. The inner areas of the rectangles matched the screen background and the rectangle edges were delineated by gray lines (thickness: 0.2°, luminance: 16.19 cd/m2). The distance from the centroid of the each rectangle to the fixation cross was 4.8°. In the vertical orientation, each object’s centroid fell on the screen’s horizontal meridian, whereas in the horizontal orientation, each object’s centroid fell on the screen’s vertical meridian.

The spatial cue was a white thickening (thickness = 0.4°, luminance = 64.78 cd/m2) at one end of a rectangle. All three sides of the cue were equal to the length of the short edge of the rectangle. The target was a light gray circle (size = 1° × 1°, luminance = 35.71 cd/m2) located at the end of the rectangle, at an eccentricity of 6.79° (see Fig. 1 for example stimuli).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Sequence of a trial in Experiment 1. The rectangles appeared in horizontal or vertical orientation (vertical is shown here). A peripheral cue flashed for 100 ms. After a 200-ms interstimulus interval (ISI), the target appeared at the cued location (60 %), the invalid same-object location (10 %), the invalid different-object location (10 %), or it did not appear (20 %). ITI = intertrial interval

Design and procedure

The experiments took place in a dimly lit room. A fixation cross and two objects appeared on the screen at the beginning of each trial. The orientation of the objects (vertical vs. horizontal) was randomized across trials. One second after the onset of the initial display, the cue appeared randomly at one of the four corners of the two objects for 100 ms. After a 200-ms interval, the target appeared at the cued location on 60 % of trials, at the uncued end of the cued object (same-object) on 10 % of trials, and at the equidistant corner of the uncued object on 10 % of trials (different-object). The remaining trials (20 %) were “catch” trials in which the target did not appear. There were ten blocks, each consisting of 80 trials.

Participants were instructed to keep their eyes on the fixation cross throughout the experiment and to quickly press the spacebar with their dominant hand as soon as the target appeared. In the absence of a correct keypress response (and on target-absent trials), the trial was terminated 2 s after the target had appeared. The subsequent trial began 500 ms after the termination of the previous trial (Fig. 1). Eye position was not recorded.

Results

For all statistical tests, we report the Greenhouse–Geisser-corrected values when the correction is necessitated by Mauchly’s test of sphericity.

Eligible trials

We excluded all trials on which the RT was shorter than 200 ms or longer than 1,500 ms. The proportion of eligible trials in each condition was submitted to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued location, same-object location, different-object location) repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA). None of the effects reached significance (all ps > .1). On average, .96 of trials were eligible for the analysis.

Reaction time

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same object, different object) repeated measures ANOVA was performed on the mean keypress RTs (Fig. 2). The main effect of target location was significant [F(2, 28) = 8.38, MSE = 3,651.12, p = .01, η p 2 = .37]. Keypress RTs were faster for the cued location (M = 368.89, SE = 18.83) than for both the same-object (M = 404.14, SE = 27.94, Cohen’s d = 0.69) and the different-object (M = 413.77, SE = 29.65, Cohen’s d = 0.8) locations (ps = .010 and .008, respectively). Importantly, RTs were significantly faster for the same-object than for the different-object location (p = .021, Cohen’s d = 0.67). The effect of orientation [F(1, 14) = 0.51, MSE = 148.69, p = .488, η p 2 = .04] and the interaction [F(2, 28) = 0.94, MSE = 285.12, p = .404, η p 2 = .06] were not significant.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Mean reaction times (RTs, in milliseconds) as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same-object, different-object). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

As predicted, our results replicated Egly et al.’s (1994) findings using the double-rectangle cueing paradigm. First, target detection was fastest at the cued location, indicative of enhanced processing due to space-based attention. Furthermore, target detection was also faster at the same-object location than at the different-object location. Given that these two locations were equidistant from the cued location, the observed performance difference cannot be attributed to space-based attention, but must be due to attention enhancing the representation of the cued object—an object-based attention effect (sometimes termed a “same-object advantage”). Given our successful replication of the manual RT results, we next used the double-rectangle cueing paradigm in an experiment with eye movement responses as the dependent measure.

Experiment 1b

Method

Participants

Fifteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment: all were students from Michigan State University. One author (G.S.) participated in the experiment; none of the participants had taken part in Experiment 1a. The participant recruitment criteria were the same as in Experiment 1a.

Apparatus and visual stimuli

Both the display and stimuli were identical to those in Experiment 1a. Eye movements were recorded by an EyeLink 1000 eyetracker (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) at a sampling rate of 1 kHz.

Design and procedure

We included a training session in this experiment to familiarize participants with the use of saccades to indicate their responses. The training session and the experiment took place on the same day in a dimly lit room. Participants who had participated in a previous eye movement study in our lab were not required to complete the training session. The equipment and stimulus characteristics were identical in the training session and the experiment.

The experimenter was in the room during the training session. Before the beginning of the training session, a nine-grid calibration and validation was performed. A fixation cross appeared first for 800 ms, followed by a target at one of four possible locations, corresponding to the corners of the rectangles in the main experiment (the rectangles were not displayed). The participants were instructed to keep their gaze at the fixation cross and make a quick eye movement to the target when it appeared. The target disappeared when the gaze stayed within a 2° radius of the target for 200 ms. In the absence of a correct eye movement, the target was removed from the screen after 2 s. The next trial started after a 500-ms interval. The training session consisted of three blocks, with 40 trials per block.

The experimenter was also present in the room during the main experiment and during the calibration and validation of the eyetracker. The main experiment began after a successful calibration and validation procedure. The main experiment was run as explained in the Design and Procedure section of Experiment 1a, with the following exception: Participants moved their eyes to fixate the target instead of using keypresses to indicate target detection. They were instructed to (a) fixate the central cross until the target appeared, (b) make a quick eye movement to the target, (c) keep their gaze on the target until it disappeared, and (d) look back at the fixation cross afterward. The target disappeared if a participant’s gaze stayed within 1.5° of the target center for at least 200 ms, which was defined as a correct response. In the absence of a correct eye movement (and on target-absent trials), the trial was terminated 2 s after the target appeared. The subsequent trial began 500 ms after the termination of the previous trial.

Data preprocessing

We used SR Research’s eye movement parser, set at the cognitive orientation, to detect saccades. The velocity threshold was 30°/s, the acceleration threshold was 8000°/s2, and the motion threshold was 0.1°.

We performed offline drift correction of the eye movement data. The mean eye position during the initial 100 ms of a trial (i.e., the initial fixation period) was subtracted from the eye position values on that trial. The saccade latency was the time difference between the target onset and the saccade onset, and the saccade duration was the time difference between the saccade onset and saccade offset. We excluded the following types of trials from our analysis of the eye movement data: (1) eye movements that occurred within 75 ms of target onset (i.e., premature saccades), (2) failures to move the eyes on target-present trials within 500 ms of the target onset, (3) failures of the first saccade to reach the target (i.e., gaze landing >2° from target center), (4) saccade latencies outside the range of three standard deviations from that participant’s mean, and (5) saccade durations outside the range of three standard deviations from that participant’s mean.

Results

Eligible trials

The proportion of eligible trials in each condition, after preprocessing, was submitted to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA. The main effect of target location was significant, F(2, 28) = 6.3, MSE = < 0.01, p = .006, η p 2 = .31: The proportion of eligible trials was higher when the target appeared at the cued location (M = .87, SE = .03) than when the target appeared at the same-object location (M = .82, SE = .03), p = .002. We found no further significant differences between the target locations, all ps > .05. The effect of orientation and the interaction were not statistically significant (all ps > .1). On average, .84 of trials were eligible for the analysis. The proportion of trials that were eliminated due to mistakenly saccading toward the cued location did not depend on the configuration, the target location, or their interaction (grand M = .11, all ps > .05). Investigation of the proportion of trials eliminated due to making a wrong saccade followed by saccading to the target showed that no corrective saccades were made when the target appeared at the cued location. Among the invalidly cued locations, the proportion of corrective saccades depended on neither the orientation (horizontal vs. vertical), nor the target location (same-object vs. different-object location), nor the interaction between orientation and target location, all ps > .5 (grand M = .07, SE = .02).

Saccade latency

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the mean saccade latencies (Fig. 3). The main effect of target location was significant [F(2, 28) = 28.54, MSE = 185.99, p < .001, η p 2 = .67]. The mean saccade latency was faster to the cued location (M = 208.68 ms, SE = 5.68 ms) than to the same-object (M = 224.99 ms, SE = 7.1 ms, Cohen’s d = 1.29) and different-object (M = 230.29 ms, SE = 7.77 ms, Cohen’s d = 1.58) locations, all ps < .001. Importantly, the saccade latency to the same-object location was faster than that to the different-object location (p = .012, Cohen’s d = 0.74). The effect of orientation [F(1, 14) = 0.29, MSE = 42.73, p = .596, η p 2 = .02] and the interaction [F(2, 28) < 0.01, MSE = 117.01, p = .992, η p 2 < .01] were not statistically significant.

Fig. 3
figure 3

Mean saccade latencies as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same object, different object). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

Saccade gain and landing distance

The endpoint accuracy of the saccades was measured to assess whether the observed differences in saccade latencies could be explained by a trade-off between saccade speed and landing precision. There are two measures of landing precision: The saccade gain ratio is calculated by dividing the measured distance of the saccade by the minimum distance to the target (Crawford & Müller, 1992), and the landing distance is the distance between the saccade endpoint and the centroid of the target. Since saccade latencies were not influenced by orientation or the interaction between orientation and target location, we collapsed across orientations when calculating the saccade gain ratios and landing distances. From Experiment 1b onward, paired-sample t tests are employed to compare the saccade gain ratios and landing distances between target locations.

The saccade gain ratio at the cued location (M = 1.01, SE = 0.01) was larger than the saccade gain ratios at the same-object (M = 0.99, SE = 0.01) and different-object (M = 0.97, SE = 0.01) locations. The saccade gain ratio at the same-object location was also higher than the ratio at the different-object location, all ps < .005. Landing distances did not differ between the target locations (grand M = 0.81, SE = 0.03), all p > .2.Footnote 1

Discussion

We aimed to measure the effect of object-based attention on oculomotor programming. Our findings parallel those in the keypress condition. Here, saccade latencies were fastest at the cued location, as compared to all other locations. This finding supports findings in the previous literature (Abrams & Jonides, 1988; Crawford & Müller, 1992; Dorris & Munoz, 1998; Kawagoe et al., 1998; Kustov & Robinson, 1996; Murray & Giggey, 2006; Rothkirch et al., 2013; Takikawa et al., 2002) showing that the deployment of space-based attention shortens the saccade latency at the attended location. With respect to object-based attention, saccade latencies were faster for targets at the same-object location than for those at the different-object location. We observed no effect of object orientation for either the saccade or the keypress experiments, although the object-based effect was numerically smaller in the horizontal than in the vertical orientation with keypress responses (Exp. 1a). A speed–endpoint accuracy trade-off was not evident. Indeed, saccades to the different-object locations slightly undershot the target, more so than saccades to the same-object location. Taken together, these results suggest that object-based attentional selection is responsible for the observed saccade latency differences between the same-object and different-object locations.

Because the peripheral cue in this experiment was also predictive of the target location (i.e., a hybrid cue), the object-based effect could have been driven by either the endogenous or exogenous properties of the cue. In the next two experiments, we further investigated whether purely exogenous or endogenous cues can drive object-based effects, as measured by saccade latency.

Experiment 2: Exogenous cue

In this experiment, we used a purely exogenous cue to direct attention. The target could appear at any one of the four ends of the two rectangles with equal probabilities. Macquistan (1997) claimed that an exogenous cue is not only sufficient to direct object-based attention to the cued object, it is the only type of cue that can elicit object-based attention. The role of exogenous cues in evoking object-based attention is very well documented (Abrams & Law, 2000; Goldsmith & Yeari, 2003, 2012), which leads us to predict significant effects of both space-based and object-based attention during keypress responses (Exp. 2a). In the eye movement experiment, we expected to find significant effects of space-based attention (Exp. 2b). Finding a correlate of object-based attention during eye movements would indicate that exogenously driven object-based attention is sufficient to influence saccade latencies.

Experiment 2a

Method

Participants

Fifteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment: All were students from Michigan State University. One participant had taken part in Experiment 1b. The participant recruitment criteria were the same as for Experiment 1a.

Apparatus

The apparatus was identical to that of Experiment 1a for the seven participants who participated in the same room in which Experiment 1a had been conducted. For the remaining eight participants (who completed the experiment in a different room), the stimuli were displayed on a 19-in. CRT monitor (resolution = 1,024 × 768 pixels) with a vertical refresh rate of 100 Hz, and the viewing distance was 85 cm, ensured by the use of a chinrest.

Stimuli and procedure

The sizes and colors of the background, fixation cross, inner areas and outer edges of the rectangles, and the cue and target were the same as in Experiment 1a. Seven participants performed the experiment in the room from Experiment 1a, and eight participants perfomed in another room on a different CRT monitor, in which the luminance values of the stimuli were slightly different (background and inner areas of the rectangles = 0.01 cd/m2, edges of the rectangles and the fixation cross = 5 cd/m2, target = 13.5 cd/m2).

The sequence and duration of the stimuli, and the response modalities were the same as in Experiment 1a. The primary difference between the experiments was the cue validity during target-present trials (80 % target-present, 20 % catch). The target could appear at the cued, same-object, different-object, and the opposite (the farthest corner to the cue) locations with equal probabilities. The instructions were the same as in Experiment 1a. There were ten blocks, each consisting of 80 trials. Eye movements were not recorded.

Results

Eligible trials

We excluded all trials in which the RT was shorter than 200 ms or longer than 1,500 ms. The number of eligible trials in each condition was subjected to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 4 (Target Location: cued location, same-object location, different-object location, opposite location) repeated measures ANOVA. The proportion of eligible trials in each condition was submitted to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 4 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA. None of the effects reached significance (all ps > .1). On average, .94 of trials were eligible for analysis.

Reaction time

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 4 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object, opposite) repeated measures ANOVA was run on the mean keypress RTs (Fig. 4). The main effect of orientation was significant, F(1, 14) = 7.97, MSE = 63.13, p = .014, η p 2 = 0.36. The mean RT was faster in the vertical orientation trials (M = 378.47, SE = 21.68) than in the horizontal orientation trials (M = 382.57, SE = 22.06). The main effect of object location was also significant, F(3, 42) = 6.81, MSE = 833.7, p = .008, η p 2 = 0.33; the keypress RT was slower for the cued location (M = 394.83, SE = 22.38) than for the same-object (M = 374.66, SE = 22.42, Cohen’s d = 0.9), different-object (M = 379.47, SE = 21.4, Cohen’s d = 0.56), and opposite (M = 373.12, SE = 22.2, Cohen’s d = 0.74) locations, all ps < .05. The RTs for the remaining target location comparisons were not significantly different, all ps > .05. The interaction between target location and orientation was also not significant, F(3, 42) = 1.71, MSE = 267.37, p = .199, η p 2 = .11.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Mean reaction times (RTs, in milliseconds) as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same object, different object, opposite). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

Experiment 2b

Method

Participants

Fifteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment: All were students from Michigan State University. Four of the participants (including author G.S.) had also participated in Experiment 1b, and one had also participated in Experiment 1a. The participant recruitment criteria were the same as in Experiment 1a.

Apparatus and visual stimuli

Both the display and stimuli were identical to those of Experiment 1a. Eye movements were recorded by an EyeLink 1000 eyetracker (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) at a sampling rate of 1 kHz.

Design and procedure

The training procedure was the same as in Experiment 1b, except that we introduced an online drift correction procedure (drift correction had been performed offline in Exp. 1b). The participants were instructed to fixate at the beginning of each trial. If their gaze stayed within 1° of the fixation cross for at least 500 ms in a 2-s period, the drift check was skipped and the trial began; otherwise, the experiment paused and a message instructed the participant to fixate and make a keypress that would initiate the drift correction. After drift correction, the participant pressed a key to continue the trial. The trial continued as we explained in Experiment 1b. This online drift correction procedure improved the eyetracking data quality, since slow drifts tend to occur during an experimental session.

The main experiment was identical to Experiment 1b, except for the addition of the drift correction procedure in the training phase (described above). The only other difference between Experiments 1b and 2b was the validity of the cue during target-present trials. In Experiment 2b, the target could appear at the cued, same-object, different-object, and opposite locations with equal probabilities. The correct eye movement criteria were the same as in Experiment 1b. When a participant failed to make a successful eye movement in a target-present trial for at least three trials in a block, a block was added to compensate for the number of trials with unsuccessful eye movements. There were ten blocks initially, each consisting of 80 trials.

Data preprocessing

The preprocessing of the data was the same as in Experiment 1b.

Results

Eligible trials

The number of eligible trials in each condition was subjected to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 4 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object, opposite locations) repeated measures ANOVA. None of the effects reached significance (all ps > .1). On average, .87 of the trials were eligible for the analysis. Only a small proportion of trials were eliminated due to mistakenly making a saccade toward the cued location, and the proportion of such trials did not depend on the orientation, target location, or their interaction (M = .05, all ps > .05). In terms of corrective saccades, none were needed when the target appeared at the cued location. The proportions of the corrective saccades for the targets at the invalidly cued locations did not depend on orientation (vertical vs. horizontal), object location (same-object, different-object, and opposite), or their interaction, all ps > .05 (grand M = .03, SE = .01).

Saccade latency

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 4 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object, opposite) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the mean saccade latencies (Fig. 5). The main effect of target location was significant, F(3, 42) = 51.97, MSE = 145.21, p < .001, η p 2 = .79. The mean eye movement latency was slower to the cued location (M = 238.17 ms, SE = 11.01 ms) than to the same-object (M = 217.21 ms, SE = 9.63 ms, Cohen’s d = 1.84), different-object (M = 221.37 ms, SE = 9.35 ms, Cohen’s d = 1.49), and opposite (M = 208.35 ms, SE = 9.17 ms, Cohen’s d = 2.37) locations, all ps < .001. The saccade latency to the opposite location was faster than those to the cued (Cohen’s d = 2.37), same-object (Cohen’s d = 1.24), and different-object (Cohen’s d = 1.96) locations, all ps < .001. Importantly, the saccade latency to the same-object location was faster than that to the different-object location (p = .011, Cohen’s d = 0.75). The effect of orientation [F(1, 14) = 3.93, MSE = 19.96, p = .067, η p 2 = .22] and the interaction [F(3, 42) = 2.36, MSE = 27.74, p = .085, η p 2 = .14] were not statistically significant.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Mean saccade latencies (in milliseconds) as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same-object, different-object, opposite). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

Saccade gain and landing distance to the target

The saccade gain ratios did not differ between the cued, same-object, different-object, and opposite locations, all ps > .095 (grand M = 1.01). The landing distance at the cued location (M = 0.74, SE = 0.03) was smaller than those at the same-object (M = 0.77, SE = 0.04), different-object (M = 0.77, SE = 0.03), and opposite (M = 0.78, SE = 0.04) locations, respective ps = .039, .009, and .009. The landing distances did not differ between targets at the same-object, different-object, and opposite locations, all ps > .49.Footnote 3

Discussion

In this experiment, we examined whether an exogenous cue was capable of eliciting an object-based effect in the double-rectangle paradigm. We found evidence of an object-based effect for saccade latencies, since the same-object saccade latency was faster than the different-object latency, but this did not hold for keypress RTs. Somewhat unexpectedly, we also found that both the saccade latencies and keypress RTs were slowest when the target appeared at the cued location, which is indicative of inhibition of return (IOR). IOR is a decrease in attentional priority at a previously attended location, manifested as a slowing of target detection at that location on subsequent trials (for a review, see Klein, 2000). The cue–target stimulus onset asynchrony (SOA) in Experiment 2 was consistent with IOR effects under spatially exogenous cues (Klein, 2000). Previous studies that have employed the two-rectangle paradigm found both spatial and object-based IOR at much longer SOAs (Jordan & Tipper, 1999; List & Robertson, 2007; Reppa & Leek, 2003). However, in one experiment, List and Robertson (2007, Exp. 2) used a shorter SOA of 340 ms, and still detected spatial IOR in the presence of a significant object-based effect. It is possible that spatial and object-based IOR effects may have different time courses, such that at short SOAs (e.g., 300 ms), only spatial IOR is present, but at longer SOAs, both spatial and object-based forms of IOR are present.

Another interesting result of Experiments 2a and 2b is the performance benefit at the location opposite the cued location. We think that such enhanced performance at the location opposite from the cue can be explained by attentional momentum, or the deployment of attentional resources in the opposite direction from the spatially inhibited location, due to IOR. A number of studies have shown not only slower RTs in detection tasks at the cued location due to IOR, but also faster RTs in detecting targets at the farthest location from the cue (Bennett & Pratt, 2001; Hubbard, 2014; Machado & Rafal, 2004; Pratt, Adam, & McAuliffe, 1998; Pratt, Spalek, & Bradshaw, 1999; Spalek & Hammad, 2004; Sumner, 2006). Our finding of both inhibition at the cued location and enhancement at the farthest location is consistent with these studies, and suggests that IOR and attentional momentum were operating in tandem.

Our findings suggest that object-based attention can operate even in the presence of spatial IOR. These data provide evidence for a dissociation between space-based and object-based attention (Duncan, 1984; He et al., 2004; He, Humphreys, Fan, Chen, & Han, 2008; Vecera & Farah, 1994), contrary to the idea that object-based attention is merely the spread of space-based attention within a cued object (Hollingworth, Maxcey-Richard, & Vecera, 2012; Martinez, Ramanathan, Foxe, Javitt, & Hillyard, 2007; Martínez, Teder-Salejarvi, & Hillyard, 2007; Müller & Kleinschmidt, 2003). Furthermore, these data suggest that saccade latencies are a more sensitive measure of object-based attention than keypress RTs. We concluded that an exogenous cue is sufficient to induce object-based attention, but that this attention is only observed with a more sensitive measure such as saccades.

Experiment 3: Endogenous cue

Having established that both a hybrid and a purely exogenous cue can elicit object-based effects, we next investigated whether a purely endogenous cue can also drive object-based effects. Experiments 1 and 2 showed that saccade latencies are more sensitive to effects of attention than are keypress RTs. Thus, saccade latencies are well-suited to the observation of small effects. In addition to our primary goal, we also planned to shed light on the controversy surrounding the role of central cues in object-based attention. On the one hand, Macquistan (1997) observed that only a peripheral cue was capable of eliciting object-based effects of attention. On the other hand, several studies (Abrams and Law, 2000; Chen & Cave, 2008; Greenberg, 2009; Law & Abrams, 2002) have shown that central cues could also elicit object-based effects. More recently, some researchers have found that giving particular instructions to participants may be necessary for central cues to elicit object-based attention (Goldsmith & Yeari, 2003, 2012). Our aim was to determine whether object-based attention can be elicited by central cues without instructing participants to use any specific strategies. If central cues can elicit object-based attention, saccade latencies may be more capable of detecting the effect than keypress RTs, as we found in Experiment 2.

Experiment 3a

Method

Participants

Fifteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment; all were students from Michigan State University. Two of the participants had taken part in Experiment 2a, one in both Experiments 2a and 1b, and another in both Experiments 2b and 1a. The participant recruitment criteria were the same as in Experiment 1a.

Apparatus

The apparatus details were identical to those in Experiment 2b.

Stimuli and procedure

The sizes and colors of the background, the inner areas and outer edges of the rectangles, and the target were the same as in Experiment 1a, with the following exceptions: The luminance values of the stimuli were slightly altered because we used a different CRT monitor (as we reported in the Stimuli and Procedure section of Exp. 2a). The fixation cross in Experiment 1a was replaced in Experiment 3a with a fixation square, which was located at the center of the screen (white, 0.02° × 0.02°, luminance = 26 cd/m2). The peripheral cue in Experiment 1a was replaced by a central cue in Experiment 3a, a white line (length = 0.5°, thickness = 0.015°, luminance = 26 cd/m2) extending from the centroid of the fixation square toward one of the four possible target locations (Fig. 6).

Fig. 6
figure 6

Sequence of a trial in Experiment 3. The rectangles appeared in a horizontal or vertical orientation (vertical is shown). A central cue pointed at one of the corners for 100 ms. After a 200-ms interstimulus interval (ISI), the target appeared at the cued location (60 %), at the far end of the cued object (10 %), or at the equidistant corner of the different object (10 %), or it did not appear (20 %). ITI = intertrial interval

The sequence and duration of the stimuli, target probabilities, and response modalities were the same as in Experiment 1a. The primary difference between the experiments was the replacement of the peripheral cue with a central cue in Experiment 3a. There were ten blocks, each consisting of 80 trials. Eye movements were not recorded. The instructions and data preprocessing were the same as in Experiment 1a.

Results

Eligible trials

The proportion of eligible trials in each condition, after preprocessing, was submitted to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object location) repeated measures ANOVA. None of the effects reached significance (all ps > .1). On average, .97 of the trials were eligible for the analysis.

Reaction time

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA was run on the mean keypress RTs (Fig. 7). The main effect of target location was significant, F(2, 28) = 58.2, MSE = 211.56, p < .001, η p 2 = .81: Keypress RTs were faster for the cued location (M = 347.41, SE = 12.59) than for the same-object (M = 375.51, SE = 12.55, Cohen’s d = 1.81) and different-object (M = 386.74, SE = 13.95, Cohen’s d = 2.7) locations, all ps < 0.001. Importantly, RTs were also faster for the same-object than for the different-object location, p = .006, Cohen’s d = 0.84. The effect of orientation [F(1, 14) = 0.79, MSE = 55.86, p = .388, η p 2 = .05] and the interaction [F(2, 28) = 2.46, MSE = 205.91, p = .118, η p 2 = .15] were not significant

Fig. 7
figure 7

Mean reaction times (RTs, in milliseconds) as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same-object, different-object). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

Experiment 3b

Method

Participants

Fifteen participants (age ~18–30 years) provided written informed consent and took part in the experiment; all were students from Michigan State University. Except for an author (G.S.) and one other participant (both had participated in Exp. 2a), none had participated in the previous experiments. The participant recruitment criteria were the same as in Experiment 1a.

Apparatus and visual stimuli

Both the display and the stimuli were identical to those of Experiment 3a. Eye movements were recorded by an EyeLink 1000 eyetracker (SR Research, Ontario, Canada) with a sampling rate of 1 kHz.

Procedure

The training procedure was the same as in Experiment 2b. The main experiment procedure was identical to that of Experiment 2b: A drift check and correction phase preceded the main phase of the experiment on each trial. In Experiment 3b, the peripheral cue was replaced with a central cue, which was informative of target location (60 % valid, 10 % same-object, 10 % different-object, 20 % catch). The criteria for correct eye movements, instructions, and data preprocessing were the same as in Experiment 1b.

Results

Eligible trials

The proportion of eligible trials in each condition, after preprocessing, was submitted to a 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA. The main effect of target location was significant, F(2, 28) = 12.3, MSE < 0.01, p < .001, η p 2 = .47. The proportion of eligible trials was higher when the target appeared at the cued location (M = .86, SE = .03) rather than at the same-object (M = .81, SE = .03) and different-object (M = .81, SE = .03) locations, ps = .001. We found no further significant differences between target locations (p = .903). The effect of orientation and the interaction were not statistically significant (ps > .5). On average, .83 of the total trials were eligible for the analysis. Only a small proportion of trials were eliminated due to mistakenly executing a saccade toward the cued location, and the proportion of such trials did not depend on orientation, target location, or their interaction (M = .06, all ps > .05). In terms of corrective saccades, none were needed when the target appeared at the cued location. The proportions of corrective saccades for the targets at the invalidly cued locations did not depend on orientation (vertical vs. horizontal), object location (same-object, different-object), and their interaction, all ps > .05 (grand M = .04, SE = .01).

Saccade latency

A 2 (Orientation: vertical, horizontal) × 3 (Target Location: cued, same-object, different-object) repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on the mean saccade latencies (Fig. 8). The main effect of target location was significant, F(2, 28) = 66.31, MSE = 210.68, p < .001, η p 2 = .83. The mean eye movement latency was faster at the cued location (M = 211.84 ms, SE = 5.93 ms) than at the same-object (M = 240.23 ms, SE = 7.07 ms, Cohen’s d = 2.2) and different-object (M = 247.94 ms, SE = 7.3 ms, Cohen’s d = 2.34) locations, all ps < .001. Importantly, the saccade latency to the same-object location was also faster than that to the different-object location, p = .006, Cohen’s d = 0.83. The effect of orientation [F(1, 14) = 1.87, MSE = 63.16, p = .193, η p 2 = .19] and the interaction [F(2, 28) = 0.14, MSE = 131.35, p = .787, η p 2 = .01] were not statistically significant.

Fig. 8
figure 8

Mean saccade latencies (in milliseconds) as a function of object orientation (vertical vs. horizontal) and target location (cued, same-object, different-object). Error bars show within-subjects standard errors, as suggested by Cousineau (2005)

Saccade gain and landing distance to the target

The saccade gain ratio at the cued location (M = 1, SE = 0.01) was larger than the ratios at the same-object (M = 0.98, SE = 0.01) and different-object (M = 0.97, SE = 0.01) locations, all ps < .001. The saccade gain ratios for the same-object and different-object locations were not significantly different, p = .312.

The landing position at the cued location was closer to the target centroid (M = 0.77, SE = 0.03) than were those at the same-object (M = 0.81, SE = 0.04) and different-object (M = 0.82, SE = 0.04) locations (ps = .016 and .044, respectively). No difference in landing positions was apparent between the same-object and different-object locations, p = .588.Footnote 4

Discussion

In Experiment 3, we investigated whether an endogenous cue is capable of affecting saccade latencies and/or inducing object-based attention. Both saccade latencies and keypress RTs were fastest for targets appearing at the cued location (space-based attention). Unlike in previous studies that had claimed that endogenous cues are unable (or unlikely) to produce object-based selection, we found that both saccade latencies and keypress RTs yielded a same-object effect with endogenous cues. Pilz, Roggeveen, Creighton, Bennett, and Sekuler (2012) claimed that object-based attention is sensitive to the orientations of objects, such that object-based effects are more often observed for horizontally oriented objects. One explanation for Macquistan (1997)’s failure to find endogenously driven object-based attention is a possible interaction between object-based attention and orientation. Although Macquistan used both vertically and horizontally oriented rectangles as the objects, he did not examine the effect of orientation in his experiment. If object-based attention is sensitive to orientation, as Pilz et al. concluded, Macquistan might have overlooked a partial endogenously driven object-based attention effect for a specific orientation. However, our results show that, unlike in Macquistan’s findings, object-based attention can be endogenously driven; and unlike in Pilz et al.’s findings, we found no interaction between target location and orientation. Our results are consistent with recent reports showing that, indeed, orientation may not be the driving factor behind the differences that Pilz et al. observed (Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016; Barnas & Greenberg, 2016; Greenberg et al., 2014).

General discussion

In this study, we found that space-based attention, driven by either endogenous or hybrid cues, shortens the saccade latency to a target at the cued location. These findings are consistent with studies showing similar effects (Abrams & Jonides, 1988; Crawford & Müller, 1992; Dorris & Munoz, 1998; Kawagoe et al., 1998; Kustov & Robinson, 1996; Murray & Giggey, 2006; Rothkirch et al., 2013; Takikawa et al., 2002). More importantly, we examined how object-based attention influences saccade latencies, which has not been systematically investigated. Regardless of the mode of attention (exogenous or endogenous), we showed that the first saccade latency to a target at the same-object location is faster than that to the different-object location. We also show that this saccade latency difference between same-object and different-object targets is not due to a speed–accuracy trade-off between saccade latency and accuracy (Wu, Kwon, & Kowler, 2010). These results reveal an oculomotor correlate of object-based selection.

First versus second saccade for object-based effects

Three previous studies have investigated the role of object-based attention on saccades using Egly et al.’s (1994) double-rectangle cueing paradigm (Malcolm & Shomstein, 2015; McCarley et al., 2002; Theeuwes et al., 2010). In these experiments, participants made their first eye movement to the cued location, and the main analysis focused on the second saccade (after fixation at the cued location). A potential confound of this approach was that it did not control for the possible guidance of saccades along object contours. Once the eye lands on the target, two elongated and highly salient contours (the edges of the object) are the immediate neighbors of the target. Those lines always extend toward the same-object location, regardless of object orientation, whereas no such elongated contours exist between the target location and the different-object location. Given that saccades are likely to follow the path of a contour (Wismeijer & Gegenfurtner, 2012), faster eye movements to the same-object location could be explained by better guidance of saccades due to image contours, instead of a difference in attentional priority. To avoid this potential low-level visual confound, we investigated the first saccade, which, by definition, equated the retinal images for each target location. In other words, wherever the target appeared, the retinal image was identical at saccade initiation. Thus, our measure of the first saccade was not biased by low-level image features, and our paradigm allowed us to isolate the effects of object-based attention on saccades.

The role of cue type in object-based attention

We tested the effectiveness of hybrid (predictive and peripheral), exogenous (nonpredictive and peripheral), and endogenous (predictive and central) cues on object-based attention by measuring both keypress RTs and saccade latencies. All three cue types were capable of producing object-based effects on saccade latencies, though not on all keypress measures. In Experiment 2, in which the cue was peripheral and nonpredictive and the SOA was 300 ms, only saccade latencies showed a significant modulation by object-based attention. Although our primary interest was in determining whether the object-based effect elicited by the hybrid cue in Experiment 1 was driven by its endogenous or exogenous component, our present experiments also shed some light on the controversy regarding the effectiveness of central cues. In an earlier study, Macquistan (1997) claimed that central cues do not evoke object-based selection, whereas others have found that object-based attention can be elicited by central cues (Abrams & Law, 2000; Al-Janabi & Greenberg, 2016; Chen & Cave, 2008; Greenberg, 2009; Law & Abrams, 2002). Some researchers have claimed that certain task demands (such as widening the focus of spatial attention) are necessary for central cues to elicit object-based attention (Goldsmith & Yeari, 2003, 2012). The participants in the present experiments were not instructed to employ a particular strategy, yet we found that central cues were as capable as peripheral cues of eliciting object-based attention. Our findings, thus, support the view that central cues can elicit object-based attention.

Difference in the time courses of object- and space-based attention

In Experiment 2, we observed that both saccade latencies and keypress RTs were much slower at the cued than at the other locations. However, both keypress RTs and saccade latencies were faster at the same-object than at the different-object location, with this same-object advantage only reaching statistical significance for saccade latencies. The observation of object-based facilitation in the presence of space-based inhibition has at least two plausible explanations. First, it is possible that the bracket cue masked the target at the cued location. However, such forward masking should also have been present in Experiments 1a and 1b, in which we observed a spatial facilitation effect. Moreover, forward masking operates over very short mask-to-target intervals (Enns & Di Lollo, 2000; Ramachandran & Cobb, 1995). Thus, the 200-ms ISIs in the present experiments should be too long to allow for forward masking. We favor the alternative explanation based on IOR. IOR was originally observed for keypress responses with noninformative cues at relatively long cue–target SOAs (for a review, see Klein, 2000), as well as for saccade latency measures (Abrams & Dobkin, 1994; Briand, Larrison, & Sereno, 2000; Dorris, Klein, Everling, & Munoz, 2002; Rafal, Egly, & Rhodes, 1994). Some studies have since demonstrated that IOR operates not only at the cued spatial location, but also within cued objects (Gibson & Egeth, 1994; Jordan & Tipper, 1998, 1999; Leek, Reppa, & Tipper, 2003; List & Robertson, 2007; Reppa & Leek, 2003; Tipper, Driver, & Weaver, 1991; Tipper, Weaver, Jerreat, & Burak, 1994). Our results in Experiment 2 are consistent with extant work that have investigated the time course of object-based IOR using the two-rectangle paradigm with nonpredictive cues. When the cue–target SOA was relatively long (400 to 1,220 ms), both spatial and object-based IOR were observed (Exp. 1 in Jordan & Tipper, 1999; List & Robertson, 2007; Reppa & Leek, 2003). However, when the SOA was short (340 ms), space-based IOR was observed in the presence of an object-based facilitation (Exp. 2 in List & Robertson, 2007). Our results in Experiment 2 are thus consistent with the previous studies and suggest that, at short SOAs, space-based inhibition had taken place without any object-based inhibition.

Our results in Experiment 2 provide more evidence for the possible independence, or dissociation, between space- and object-based attention. We found that object-based facilitation does not necessarily require the presence of space-based facilitation. This dissociation—that is, object-based effects in the presence of spatial inhibition—can also inform the controversy regarding the role of space-based attention in object-based attention. Two classes of theories have attempted to explain how object- and space-based attention are interrelated: (1) that object-based attention is a spreading of space-based attention within the cued object (Hollingworth et al., 2012; Martinez, Ramanathan, et al., 2007; Martínez, Teder-Salejarvi, & Hillyard, 2007; Müller & Kleinschmidt, 2003) and (2) that object-based attention is fully independent of space-based attention (Duncan, 1984; He et al., 2004; He et al., 2008; Vecera & Farah, 1994). Our results support the latter view, as do the results of List and Robertson (2007). If space-based attention was a prerequisite of object-based attention, we should not have found an object-based effect in Experiment 2. However, we observed both space-based IOR and object-based effects concurrently. Therefore, we conclude that space-based and object-based forms of attention do not share the same time course for IOR, and that the presence of space-based facilitation is not a requirement for object-based attention.

Saccade planning and the object-based prioritization map

Our results show that saccade planning (as measured by latency) is facilitated not only at the cued location, but also at the far end of the attended object. Shorter saccade latencies to a target that appears at the cued location have also been consistently observed in several previous studies (Abrams & Jonides, 1988; Crawford & Müller, 1992; Dorris & Munoz, 1998; Kawagoe et al., 1998; Kustov & Robinson, 1996; Murray & Giggey, 2006; Rothkirch et al., 2013; Takikawa et al., 2002). This space-based cueing effect can be accommodated within the theoretical framework of a priority map, in which saccades are drawn to the highest-priority location on the map (for reviews, see Bisley & Goldberg, 2010; Zelinsky & Bisley, 2015). Our findings support a role of the priority map in saccade planning, because gaze was faster to the highest-priority location than to all other locations. However, the concept of a priority map cannot easily explain how attention is also modulated by the layout of objects and why saccades are also drawn to an attended object. This observation suggests an extension of the priority map concept. Indeed, a proto-object-based saliency model has been proposed to explain object-based attention (Russell, Mihalaş, von der Heydt, Niebur, & Etienne-Cummings, 2014; Vecera, 2000). In this model, figure–ground segmentation is achieved by assigning border ownership, wherein the regularities in a scene are grouped together. Such a grouped array attracts attention, as well as saccades, because it becomes the winner of a competition against single, ungrouped features (Russell et al., 2014). Our empirical data provide support for this theory, which is based on model simulations. Even though the attended location has an advantage in saccade latency, a competition between objects seems to take place in addition to the competition between locations. Thus, our findings favor roles for both prioritized locations and objects in saccade preparation. It is worth noting here that this is also consistent with well-accepted theories positing voluntary control over object-based attention (Drummond & Shomstein, 2010; Greenberg, 2009; Greenberg & Gmeindl, 2008; Shomstein, 2012).

Comparison between keypress RT and saccade latency

It is well-established that saccades and attention are closely related (e.g., premotor theory of attention; Rizzolatti et al., 1987), suggesting strong ecological validity for measuring attention via the saccade latency. When a potential target attracts our attention, we typically saccade to it (Herwig & Schneider, 2014). In general, we found parallel attention effects for keypress RTs and saccade latencies. Thus, saccade latencies also exhibit standard object-based effects previously demonstrated with keypress RTs. However, our data also show that saccade latency is a more sensitive measure for detecting effects of attention. Notably, in Experiment 2, an object-based effect was reliably observed via saccade latencies, but only a small numeric trend was observed for keypress RTs. The effect sizes of our reported statistical tests are another index of sensitivity. In Table 1, we list the Cohen’s ds we calculated for both keypress and saccade measures in all experiments. Keypress RT effect sizes were generally smaller than those for saccade latencies; this held for both space-based and object-based effects of attention. Presumably, the superior sensitivity of saccade latencies in comparison to keypress RTs stems from the ballistic nature of saccades, so that variability is smaller in saccade latencies than that in keypress RTs. This also accords with experimental results showing that, although the oculomotor and hand movement systems are guided by shared attentional resources, the planning of eye and hand movements to an attended target is dominated by the oculomotor system (Khan, Song, & McPeek, 2011). Given the ecological validity and superior sensitivity of saccades, future research in object-based attention may benefit from utilizing saccade latency as the dependent measure.

Table 1 Cohen’s d measures for space-based effects (measured by comparing the cued and same-object locations) and object-based effects (measured by comparing the same- and different-object locations) in each experiment

Conclusion

We found that first saccade latencies were facilitated by exogenously and endogenously driven object-based attention. Our observation of object-based effects of attention in the presence of space-based IOR suggests that object-based selection may be independent of space-based selection. Furthermore, as compared to manual keypress RTs, saccade latencies exhibited higher sensitivity in detecting effects of attention. Overall, the oculomotor correlates of object-based attention that we demonstrated in this study are consistent with the idea that attentional priority maps encode (and are influenced by) not only spatial locations, but also perceptual objects.