Background

Overuse of healthcare resources is a global issue and prescription of unnecessary medication is a key contributor to this [1]. Overprescribing of medication is defined by the UK Department of Health and Social care as medication that is not required or wanted, or where risk outweighs benefit [2]. Overprescribing is considered a ‘low-value’ practice, i.e. a practice that is not clinically [1] or cost-effective [3]. Despite efforts to encourage evidence-based practice and reduce low-value prescribing [4], healthcare professionals continue to prescribe unnecessary medication [5, 6]. To maintain a high standard of care, efficient use of resources, and to reduce potential harm to patients [1], it is important to eliminate low-value prescribing practices.

The process of eliminating ineffective or potentially harmful prescriptions can be conceptualised as ‘de-implementation’. De-implementation is not a well-defined concept and definitions vary between “abandonment of … practices” [7], or as any effort to “reduce” a practice or “address overuse” [8, 9]. In a scoping review, Niven and colleagues (2015), identified 43 terms in literature synonymous to de-implementation including “re-invest”, “discontinue” and “decline in use” [10]. Generally, these terms can fall into four types of de-implementation; removing, replacing, reducing or restricting a practice or treatment [11].

Although de-implementation has been conceptualised as the removal or reduction of a medical practice, it cannot be considered the opposite to implementation (i.e. the integration or increase) of a practice [7, 12]. The evidence base has highlighted that de-implementation is distinct from implementation [13,14,15]. Specific conceptual models for de-implementation have been developed to guide research [7, 10, 11, 16, 17] by highlighting areas for further exploration. Key features of barriers, facilitators, the type of target action, the intervention strategies and, the definition and measurement of outcomes of de-implementation have been highlighted as likely areas to differ from implementation. Further, the implications or consequences arising from achieving successful de-implementation outcomes were highlighted as another key area likely to differ from implementation [18].

Understanding the consequential effects of removing an established medical practice is crucial to evaluate de-implementation efforts [19, 20] and gain a comprehensive understanding of the full impact of de-implementation [18]. In a recent review of the consequences experienced in strategies used to reduce antibiotic prescribing, expected ‘trade-offs,’ such as an increased hospital stay, were viewed as a reasonable compromise for a successful de-implementation outcome [21]. However, unintended consequences, such as clinicians increasing their use of other medical practices to replace the practice being discontinued, may have possibly harmful repercussions [18], in addition to contradicting the process of de-implementation [21]. A fuller understanding of the nuanced impact of de-implementation alongside successful strategies and determinants will offer information to contribute to the growing evidence base which will aid better replication and optimisation of future de-implementation.

While implementation and de-implementation have been conceptualised as unique, implementation science offers a strong foundation to understand de-implementation [22, 23]. Existing implementation theory and methods have been utilised in de-implementation research considerably [24]. Frameworks such as the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) [25] have been applied to offer insight into de-implementation [26,27,28,29]. Most recently, the TDF has been successfully used in a multi-method study to identify barriers and facilitators of de-implementing of multiple low-value care practices in a critical care setting [30]. Parsons Leigh and colleagues successfully mapped findings from their systematic review and interview study to the TDF which provided a structured theoretical lens on important determinants of de-implementation in this setting. These existing approaches have also been used to understand the determinants of low-value prescribing behaviour, allow cumulative contributions to the expanding de-implementation evidence base and offer tangible information that can be used in future strategies [19].

Therefore, the aim of this study is to further understand the key features of de-implementation strategies aiming to reduce low-value prescribing. The insights of these efforts will contribute to better replication and optimisation of future de-implementation efforts. We used a systematic review approach to synthesise relevant literature.

Review questions

This systematic review aimed to answer these questions:

  1. (1)

    What strategies have been used to de-implement low-value prescribing practices of secondary healthcare prescribers, and how effective were these strategies?

  2. (2)

    What are the reported barriers, facilitators and unintended consequences of these strategies?

Review methods

Design

Protocol registration

This systematic review is reported following the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Additional file 1). The protocol was registered at PROSPERO (ID: CRD42021243944).

Identification of studies

Studies were identified by searching CINAHL, EMBASE, MEDLINE and Cochrane register of controlled trials databases from inception to February 2021.

Search

A search strategy was developed with an Information Officer. The search was designed to identify randomised control trials (RCTs) evaluating behaviour change strategies to address a low-value or unnecessary medication prescriptions, within a secondary healthcare setting. De-implementation-related terms identified in a scoping review were the basis of this search strategy [10]. See the full search strategy in Table 1; the searches were adapted to the vocabulary and syntax of each database.

Table 1 Search strategy for MEDLINE

Study selection (inclusion and exclusion criteria)

Inclusion and exclusion criteria were developed following the PICO (Population, Intervention, Context and Outcome) structure (Table 2). Eligible studies were RCTs comparing behavioural strategies to a control or other strategies. Studies had to include an intervention designed to reduce, remove, restrict or replace low-value medication prescription practice. Only studies reported in English were included. Studies had to target the initial prescribing part of the care process, rather than, at the time of review or reconciliation of medication. Prescribing here refers only to the prescription of medication, as opposed to prescribing or ordering tests or procedures. One author (JD) screened 100% of the title and abstracts, and 10% of these were independently double screened by a second author (SM). Double screening of titles and abstracts yielded a kappa score of 0.39, which is considered ‘fair’ agreement [31]. One author (JD) screened 100% of full-text articles, and 40% were independently double screened by a second author (SM).

Table 2 Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Data extraction and coding

A bespoke data extraction form was developed and a priori headings were selected to gather data on the unique complexities of de-implementation guided by Norton and Chambers conceptual de-implementation framework [11, 18]. Headings included target actions, intervention strategies, barriers, facilitators, outcomes and consequences. Data was extracted verbatim where possible.

To allow comparisons, strategies were categorised using the Effective Practice and Organisation of Care Taxonomy (EPOC) [32]. The EPOC taxonomy offers a classification of health system interventions, including an overarching description category and subcategories of interventional strategies. All studies were allocated to overarching categories and relevant subcategories. Where appropriate, multifaceted strategies were categorised into multiple relevant subcategories.

Barriers and facilitators were the interpretation of the trial results as offered by the authors of the studies. Barriers were defined as any intrinsic or extrinsic influences faced by prescribers mentioned in the article text that may have hindered the de-implementation of low-value prescribing. Facilitators were defined as any intrinsic or extrinsic influences that may have enabled intervention success. Verbatim text of identified barriers and facilitators were summarised and mapped to the Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) [25].

The TDF is a synthesis of 33 behavioural theories into 14 theoretical domains [25]: Behavioural Regulation, Knowledge, Skills, Beliefs about Capabilities, Beliefs about Consequences, Intentions, Reinforcement, Goals, Memory, Attention and Decision Processes, Environmental Context and Resources, Social Influences, Optimism, Emotions, and Social/Professional Role and Identity. The TDF is a theory-based tool that allows the categorisation of barriers and facilitators to understand key influences on a target behaviour.

Consequences were defined as repercussions arising from the implementation of the intervention. The diffusion of innovations literature provides a classification of consequences [33]. The framework suggested by Toma and colleagues was used to specify four classifications of consequences, including expected outcomes, which can be classed as either (1) desirable or (2) undesirable, and unexpected outcomes, which can also be (3) desirable or (4) undesirable [21]. Secondary outcomes, other planned or unplanned analyses and the study authors’ interpretations in the article text of potential consequences were extracted verbatim and categorised to these headings.

Data synthesis

We did not conduct a meta-analysis due to study heterogeneity in several factors of the included studies including variety in intervention strategies, the target illness and medication. Data was tabulated into evidence tables: (1) characteristics of included studies; (2) strategies within each study classified by the EPOC taxonomy; (3) reported intervention descriptions and their effects; (4) identified barriers to de-implementation categorised to the TDF; (6) identified facilitators of de-implementation categorised to the TDF; (7) identified consequences of de-implementation specified with Toma and colleagues framework.

Quality assessment

The risk of bias was assessed by one reviewer (JD) and a 10% sample was independently assessed by a second reviewer (SM). The tool recommended for assessing the risk of bias in RCTs outlined in the Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Strategies [34] was used. Risk of bias arising from the randomisation process, timing of recruitment of participants (in cluster-randomised trials), deviations from the intended interventions, missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome and selection of the reported result were assessed. Disagreements were resolved by discussion within our research team.

Results of the review

Details of included and excluded studies

The database search returned 2907 titles and abstracts. Following the removal of duplicates, 2193 records remained. Thirty-two full-text articles were screened for eligibility. Eleven studies were included in this review. See the PRISMA flow diagram in Fig. 1. Main reasons for exclusion at the full text reviewing stage, included non-RCT study designs, the setting of the study was not in secondary care, and where it was unclear if the study had a de-implementation focus.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flow diagram

Included study characteristics

Study characteristics for the 11 included studies can be found in Table 3. Eight studies compared their strategies to a usual care comparator group [35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42] and three studies offered a partial or adapted intervention [43,44,45] as their comparator. Seven studies were cluster RCTs [36, 37, 39, 43,44,45], including one stepped wedge [42]. Two interventions included strategies targeting both the healthcare professional and the patient [37, 45]. See additional file 2 for verbatim intervention and comparison arm names, descriptions and counts.

Table 3 Characteristics of included studies

The type of de-implementation as “reducing” low-value prescribing was identified in all studies except one. The majority of studies were conducted in high-income countries [35, 37,38,39,40,41,42,43, 45], and two took place in Kenya [36, 44]. Studies were set in emergency departments or urgent care units [37, 40,41,42, 45], other ward types [38, 39, 43] or whole hospitals [35, 36, 44]. The type of low-value prescribing being targeted tended to be inappropriate antibiotics for a range of illnesses [35, 37, 39, 42, 44, 45]; however, general inappropriate drug prescriptions, especially in the elderly, were also reported [40, 43].

The 11 studies reported 11 intervention arms. Table 4 shows classifications of intervention arms to the Effective Practice and Organisation of Care (EPOC) Taxonomy [32]. The overarching categories of ‘Implementation Strategies, Interventions targeted at healthcare workers’ and ‘Financial Arrangements, Targeted financial incentives for health professionals and healthcare organisations’ were identified. Four interventions were multi-faceted with between 4 and 8 subcategories identified. ‘Educational materials’ and ‘Audit and Feedback’ were the most used strategies in these multifaceted interventions [37, 43,44,45]. Seven interventions were single strategy, six were described as ‘reminders’ [35, 38,39,40,41,42] and one study was classified as ‘pay for performance’ [36]. Reminders were the most commonly used strategies (n = 6), educational strategies including educational meetings and educational materials were the second most common (n = 4).

Table 4 Classifications of included de-implementation interventions to the Effective Practice and Organisation of Care Taxonomy (EPOC) categories and subcategories

Studies with an active comparator used adapted versions of the strategies employed in the intervention arm, for example, Franchie and colleagues provided educational materials in both arms, but limited the control arm to selected modules [43].

Quality assessment of studies

Risk of bias results for studies can be seen in Fig. 2. Six studies were considered to have an overall high risk of bias [35,36,37, 41, 42, 44] and five had a moderate risk of bias [38,39,40, 43, 45]. No studies were found to have an overall low risk of bias. Generally, studies tended to perform well on biases relating to the randomisation process and missing data management. However, deviating from the intended intervention domain presented the most risk of bias, as three studies had high [36, 37, 41] and four moderate risk [38, 39, 44, 45] for this domain. It should be noted that those assigned high risk for this domain tended to suffer risk of bias due to participants being aware of their participation in the trial or the allocation of the intervention, rather than deviations from intervention strategies that may have arisen from the context. Six studies were assessed as having a moderate risk of bias in the domain of selection of reported results [37, 39,40,41, 43, 44], which was mainly due to the majority of studies not referring to a published protocol.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plot illustrating raw data and unadjusted differences

Intervention effectiveness

The main aims of the included studies were to assess the effectiveness of de-implementation strategies (e.g. evaluate the effectiveness of a clinical decision support system to reduce inappropriate antibiotic prescribing in children admitted to hospital). Table 5 shows the intervention type and where studies were found effective. Seven studies reported strategies being effective in reducing low-value prescribing [35,36,37,38,39,40,41] of these, four studies were of high risk of bias [35,36,37, 41] and three were moderate risk [38,39,40]. One study reported weak evidence of an intervention effect, after adjusting for patient, clinician level factors and study period [44]. The remaining three strategies did not show any differences across arms in de-implementing inappropriate prescribing [42, 43, 45]. Additional file 3 shows the verbatim outcome definitions and results for each study.

Table 5 Intervention description, type and reported effectiveness

The most reported effective strategies were classed as ‘reminders’, offered at the point of care [35, 38,39,40,41,42]. Reminders are defined by the EPOC Taxonomy [32] as any intervention that prompts action, such as decision support aids. Although in varying formats, ‘reminders’ were single-strategy clinical decision support systems. These decision support systems varied as some offered thresholds to guide appropriate prescribing decisions [42], accounted for or restricted additional diagnostic information [35, 39] or provided recommendations of appropriate substitutes [38, 40, 41]. These strategies were offered in emergency and non-emergency settings; however, the use of clinical decision support systems was exclusive to high-income countries. Educational strategies, including educational materials and meetings, were the second most common. However, only one of four studies using educational strategies was found to be effective [37].

Eight studies reported outcomes as ‘inappropriate’ prescribing behaviour (i.e. measured the number of unnecessary prescriptions) and three reported outcomes as ‘appropriate’ (i.e. measured the amount of necessary prescriptions), data and calculated odds ratios are presented in the forest plot in Fig. 2 for comparison. Where data was not available or easily extracted, study authors were contacted. Menya et al. (2015) and Yadav et al. (2019) were unable to offer complete data.

Barriers and facilitators of de-implementation

Barriers

Only one study proactively identified potential barriers, of alert fatigue and unimportant reminders, to inform intervention design [38]. Two other studies quantitatively measured potential sources of barriers, including a real-time assessment of reasons for rejecting prescription recommendations [40], and a survey of clinicians’ attitudes toward antibiotic resistance before and after the intervention implementation [45]. In other studies, authors interpretations referred to potential barriers but there was no formal measurement or indication if these potential barriers were considered in intervention design. Barriers and their related Theoretical Domains Framework (TDF) domains are reported in Table 6. More detailed summaries of identified barriers and related TDF domains can be found in Additional file 4.

Table 6 Categorisation of barriers to the Theoretical Domain Framework (TDF) domains

The most frequently identified barriers related to the Environmental Context and Resources domain (n = 9). Issues including; an unestablished electronic health record [38, 43], emergency settings experiencing issues of continuity of care and severely ill patients [37], a lack of morale in healthcare staff and an increased volume of patients [36] were highlighted as potential barriers to de-implementation. The need for additional information [42] or data entry required [38] for the decision support strategies to work were also reported as barriers. Additionally, the type of information offered and the timing of the decision support intervention being too late [38] were also identified.

Barriers relating to the Knowledge domain were the second most frequently identified (n = 5). Clinicians knowledge of; what constitutes an inappropriate prescription [37], the illnesses that are responsive to antibiotics [37], where a reduction in dose is required [41] and how to utilise an intervention were reported as potential barriers [36]. In two of the studies aiming to reduce antibiotic prescribing, authors reported that intervention information that contradicted clinicians training [44] or treatment decisions [39] was likely to prevent successful de-implementation. In addition, although not widely mentioned, Goal-related barriers such as a clinician’s lack of interest or underestimation of low-value prescription issues [43] may also have the potential to impact on de-implementation.

Social influences barriers were also cited to potentially impact de-implementation (n = 3). Patients were reported to demand medication [40] or have a general influence on prescriptions [43]. Social influence barriers also included clinicians attempting to be consistent with colleague’s prescriptions [40, 41] but were only cited in studies conducted in emergency settings. The specific culture of emergency settings was noted to have an impact on de-implementation. Clinicians in these contexts were ensuring the patient gets the medication they are expecting and not disrupting another prescriber’s decision. These contextual barriers were reported to impact new and refilled prescriptions [40, 41].

Facilitators

Most facilitators identified (Table 7) related to the Environmental context and resources domain (n = 8). Ensuring the intervention was easy to use [35, 38], tailored to the local context [39, 45], supported by infrastructure [36, 40, 45], available at the time of a prescription decision [40] and of low cost [35, 39,40,41] were identified as likely to facilitate de-implementation. More detailed summaries of identified barriers and related TDF domains can be found in Additional file 5.

Table 7 Categorisation of facilitators to the Theoretical Domain Framework (TDF) domains

Other possible facilitators of de-implementation included clinicians having a correct understanding of appropriate prescriptions (Knowledge) [36, 43], obtaining a fuller record of symptoms (Skills) [38], being more experienced (Skills) [40], being provided with a choice in treatment options (Social/Professional role and Identity) [45] and avoiding alert fatigue (Memory, Attention and Decision Processes) [38]. Healthcare staff feeling that changing their behaviour can have a positive impact on patients (Beliefs about consequences), and motivation to further reduce already low rates of inappropriate prescribing (Goals) [45] were identified as other possible facilitators.

Consequences of de-implementation

Consequences that could have been caused by the intervention were identified in nine studies [35,36,37,38,39,40, 42, 43, 45] and were identified through secondary outcomes and additional analyses. Table 8 provides the number of identified consequences categorised to the conceptual framework [21]. Desirable (n = 22), undesirable (n = 4), expected (n = 6) and unexpected consequences (n = 4) were identified. Many authors of included studies did not clarify if a difference (or non-difference) between arms indicated a desirable or undesirable outcome and even less so if these outcomes were expected or unexpected. More details of consequences can be found in Additional file 6.

Table 8 Number of consequences identified in studies

Desirable and undesirable consequences were usually related to patient safety. Desirable consequences included a reduction in duration of fever [39], fever at day 7 and secondary antibiotic prescriptions [42] and reduced length of stay in hospital [35, 39]; however, one study reported this could have been a cofounded finding [35]. Conversely, an intervention using educational materials (and a decision support system, which was not utilised) found patients at intervention sites had a significantly longer length of stay [43] but did not make clear if this was unexpected. Three studies reported a desirable consequence that their intervention did not adversely impact the appropriate process of care or prescriptions being given to patients who required such medications [36, 37, 42].

One study, using reminders, reported a financially desirable consequence, where a change in the type of medication used was associated with a reduction in medication cost [39], whereas, another intervention using modified lab reporting was not associated with a cost saving [35].

Other undesirable consequences were identified. Consequences arising from the clinician’s interaction with the intervention were also identified. One study using a decision support system reported that prescribing outcomes improved for all types of reminders a clinician received (i.e. reminders of evidence-based practice or potential drug interactions) and did not increase alert fatigue, which are desirable consequences [38]. However, the amount of time required to select a medication using the decision support increased over the course of the trial, again, Moja and colleagues did not define this as unexpected or a reasonable trade-off [38].

In one of the multifaceted interventions, consequences were identified through a pre-post measure of attitudes towards antibiotic stewardship [45]. More clinicians reported a positive attitude and agreed that stewardship was important following the intervention, which could indicate a desirable consequence. However, in the same study, clinicians also continued to feel that a stewardship intervention would not impact on their decision-making in future [45], which could be an undesirable consequence as it may be counterproductive in de-implementation efforts.

Other consequences could refer to the level of use or acceptance of an intervention. A high level of compliance or use of the intervention is assumed to correspond with intervention success; however generally, a potential trade-off or realistic expectation is that the intervention will not reach 100% uptake. For example, one study reported 43% of recommendations were accepted and continued to produce an effective outcome; however, it was not clear if this level of acceptance was expected or desired [40].

Discussion

This review aimed to understand the effectiveness, barriers, facilitators, and consequences of strategies de-implementing low-value medication prescribing in secondary healthcare. The majority of included studies were found to significantly reduce inappropriate medication prescribing (n = 7/11). Included studies addressed various low-value prescribing practices, the majority addressed antibiotic use (n = 6) and others targeted malaria therapies in non-malaria patients, unnecessary medication for older people or in general. We deductively extracted data to further understand the complexities of de-implementation as conceptualised in Norton and Chambers de-implementation framework [18].

Despite the widespread efforts to reduce low-value care [46], only a few strategies were identified and even fewer successfully de-implemented low-value prescribing practice in secondary healthcare. Effective strategies included single strategy clinician decision support systems (n = 6), financial incentives (n = 1) and a multi-faceted intervention including components of education and audit and feedback (n = 1). This is consistent with the literature as strategies to reduce low-value care across the health system found clinicians’ decision support and multifaceted interventions (usually including education) to be most utilised and effective [8].

In our review, clinical decision support systems were the most common and effective strategy to de-implement inappropriate prescribing. This aligns with a more recent systematic review of strategies that addressed low-value care in cancer services [47]. Alishahi Tabriz and colleagues postulated that the success of decision support strategies was due to aids being ‘active’ as opposed to ‘passive’. ‘Active’ strategies are intentional and exerted efforts to facilitate change in behaviour or ways of working [47]. ‘Active’ strategies have previously been found useful in de-implementation. In a scoping review conducted in 2015 of de-implementation interventions in healthcare more generally, de-implementation happened when evidence was diffused; however, those using an ‘active’ intervention to aid the use of evidence, such as clinician education or withdrawing a medication from the market, were more likely to lead to successful de-implementation [10]. To ensure effective de-implementation, efforts should move away from passive dissemination of evidence and offer ‘active’, intentional and exerted interventions, such as clinical decision support systems, to facilitate de-implementation.

Educational strategies, of educational materials and educational meetings, were the second most commonly used strategies to reduce low-value prescribing, however only one reported a significant effect [37]. These educational strategies were multifaceted and were usually paired with audit and feedback or monitoring. Previous literature has shown educational strategies to be effective when paired with other strategies [8]. Additionally, multi-faceted strategies using clinical educational or academic detailing among other strategies are likely to facilitate de-implementation and curb low-value care compared to being used alone [48]. Metlay and colleagues, authors of the effective multifaceted intervention, using education, audit and feedback, did not offer reasons for success; however, they noted the need to distinguish the most suitable ‘active ingredient/s’ of their intervention [37]. There have been strides to identify the active ingredients or behavioural change techniques used in de-implementation interventions however, more needs to be done to understand the utility of these components and which ingredients would facilitate the most significant effect [29]. Where educational strategies are paired with other strategies, a clearer understanding of the degree to which education content, or the behaviour change techniques within educational strategies can facilitate de-implementation is required to optimise their contribution to de-implementation in future.

Consideration of determinants that may prevent or enable de-implementation [49] were also explored in our review. Only one study in our review explicitly identified potential barriers before implementing their intervention [38], others offered interpretations of potentially influential determinants retrospectively. Most identified potential barriers related to the TDF domain of Environmental context and resources, particularly strategies utilising decision support. Intervening at the point of care was seen as beneficial [41] and can disrupt clinicians’ decision-making to influence a change in decision [50], but is likely to face many pitfalls [51]. Only one study, although effective, found prescribing decisions may have been made in advance of interacting with computer systems and decision support may have been offered “too late” [38]. Across included studies decision support was usually offered at ‘point of care’ (we assume once a prescription was decided). If the decision process for prescribing happens before the time a clinician engages with a computer, a change in the decision and subsequent de-implementation of the selected behaviour may be more difficult or unsustained. Explicit specification of ‘when’ and ‘where’ these decision aid strategies operate are required to understand the extent these context factors affect the decision support success [52]. Additionally, measurement of these factors will allow for further optimisation of ‘point of care’ de-implementation strategies.

The setting and culture in which an intervention is embedded should be a main consideration for designing and optimising de-implementation strategies [20]. Barriers relating to the wider context (as highlighted in the Environmental context and resources domain) were frequently identified across studies. Two of five studies conducted in emergency settings, referred to the ‘specific culture’ of these settings hindering de-implementation [37, 40]. Continuity of care, dealing with very sickly patients and the desire to not deviate from other clinician’s plans were viewed as potential barriers in these settings. The importance of the setting of the de-implementation intervention has been emphasised in previous research. In a systematic review of determinants of low-value care nursing practices, characteristics of the setting such as the type of department, organisational norms and structure were found to be influential in both, the use of low-value practices and de-implementation process [53]. Additionally, the wider culture including political support or pressure and economy were also notable determinants of de-implementation [53], although these factors were not identified in the current review. Intervention designers are required to have a clear appreciation of the context and setting within which de-implementation efforts are implemented to better understand and attempt to influence these determinants.

Consequences produced by the intervention, where reported, tended to be ‘desirable’ (i.e. reduced length of stay). Consequences were classified to a framework as suggested by Toma and colleagues [21] where phrasing of findings or authors’ interpretations made classification possible. It was difficult to understand if a measured outcome not having a difference between arms (e.g. no difference between arms for frequency of return hospital visits) should be categorised as a ‘desirable’ or an ‘undesirable’ consequence. However, where a difference was experienced such as a significant reduction in the length of stay in hospital in intervention arms [35, 39], classification was more easily allocated. Clearer expectations of potential consequences are required to understand if undesirable or unexpected consequences are acceptable trade-offs of de-implementation.

Consideration of how the reduction or removal of a low-value practice may impact appropriate care has already been highlighted as a potential consequence that should be captured in any de-implementation effort [19]. Only three (of 11) studies in the current review measured the impact of their strategies on appropriate prescriptions. Evaluations of strategies need to widen the scope, to consider expected, unexpected, desirable and undesirable consequences of de-implementation on patients, providers and organisations to account for any impact on appropriate care caused by the de-implementation of low-value care [20]. Further research on the impact of and recurrence of these consequences will contribute to a fuller understanding of the impact of de-implementation strategies on appropriate and necessary care [21].

The continuation of low-value prescribing practices is an evidence-practice gap which requires a change in clinical behaviour. This review considered the determinants, strategies and consequences that influence de-implementation. This review has highlighted the relevant ‘complexities’ using Norton and Chamber’s framework [18] to further understand the process of de-implementation. To ensure effective de-implementation in future, strategies need to be informed by formulation work to understand determinants and account for potential consequences. This can be achieved through rigorous and theoretically informed research which in turn will contribute to an accumulation of knowledge about how and why de-implementation strategies work [54].

Strengths and limitations

The strength of this review was its comprehensive search strategy. Developed with an information officer, the search strategy included the terms unique to the field of de-implementation [10] which ensured relevant studies were identified. Another strength was the comprehensive use of conceptual frameworks to understand the features of de-implementation and key areas required for further understanding of de-implementation. Comparisons across frameworks provide a theoretical lens and help identify where there are similarities or where gaps in knowledge exist.

However, there were limitations. First, only studies written in English, with a RCT design were included. Other study designs may have provided more information about barriers and facilitators or consequences of de-implementation strategies. Second, studies were excluded if they failed to define the setting or where the intervention target behaviour was not clearly defined as de-implementation. Unspecific language such as ‘change’ or ‘improve’ often used to describe de-implementation strategies was not captured in our search strategy. This meant that studies with an aim to reduce inappropriate practices may have been excluded on this basis.

A final limitation is that a meta-analysis was not performed to assess the effectiveness of included de-implementation strategies; instead, this review reported if they were effective or not. This review only included RCTs, considered the gold standard for the evaluation of strategies, as opposed to uncontrolled studies that may be poorer in quality. However, all included RCT risk of biases were assessed as high and moderate; therefore, quality should be a consideration in the interpretation of these results.

Conclusions

In conclusion, this review has demonstrated that multiple intervention strategies were found to effectively de-implement inappropriate prescribing in secondary healthcare. Clinical decision support systems were the most effective and educational strategies may be useful; however, more research needs to be done to establish the degree to which these components are effective. Generally, environmental, contextual, social and knowledge-driven barriers and facilitators need to be taken into consideration when replicating or optimising de-implementation strategies. Specification of features, such as timing and context, should also be reported to gain insight to how best to optimise these strategies in future. Finally, any expected, unexpected or desirable and undesirable consequences caused by the intervention need to be measured to ensure the accumulation of knowledge of possible consequences to fully account for the impact of de-implementation.