Background

It has been estimated that approximately 970 million people worldwide were living with psychiatric disorders in 2019 [1]. Notably, individuals with psychiatric disorders (e.g., depression and substance use) were reported to have a significant excess mortality risk [2] and a reduction of 10–20 years in life expectancy [3], compared to the general population. Likewise, people suffering from current psychological symptoms (e.g., depressive and anxiety symptoms) were also associated with an elevated risk of major diseases, such as cancer [4] and cardiovascular diseases [5]. Furthermore, given that one third (31.4%) of adults reported having experienced at least one lifetime traumatic experience [6], while close to half (47%) of individuals experienced at least one adverse childhood event (ACE) according to reports from England [7], the accumulated negative effects of traumatic experience on physiological, behavioral, and psychological functions could be substantial at the population level [8, 9].

Telomeres are conserved complex DNA structures at the ends of chromosomes that ensure their stability and prevent damage [10]. In most human cells, the telomere shortens with cell division, making it a biomarker of human aging [11]. Leukocyte telomere length (LTL) is a practicable measure of TL and has a close correlation with TL across different tissues within individuals [12]. Therefore, LTL could serve as a biomarker for biological aging, and shorter LTL has been significantly associated with increased morbidity and mortality [13]. However, telomere attrition is highly associated with age, gender, and genetic background, rendering it difficult to measure changes of LTL (i.e., aging acceleration) virtually induced by environmental factors, such as those aforementioned psychological and trauma-related factors (e.g., depression [14], anxiety [15], and ACEs [16]). This could be an explanation of inconsistent results observed for associations between adulthood traumatic experiences and LTL [17].

In addition, besides the biological measures of aging acceleration, facial aging acceleration (i.e., if the person looks older than his/her chronological age) is also considered as a valid indicator of accelerated aging. In a cohort of 1826 twins, the twin individual who looked older seemed to have a higher risk of mortality during a 7-year follow-up, after adjustment for chronological age and sex, than his/her twin partner [18]. However, with limited evidence for some specific psychological symptoms (e.g., depression [19]) or severe trauma (e.g., financial difficulty [20]), a comprehensive assessment on the effects of psychological and trauma-related factors on facial aging acceleration is currently lacking.

Taking advantage of enriched data on phenotypes and genotypes in the UK Biobank, we aimed to examine the association of a broad set of psychological and trauma-related factors with aging acceleration indexed by both biological and facial aging acceleration. Importantly, our study features the application of data-driven approaches to identify psycho-trauma exposure patterns, as illustrations of various existing stimulus–response constellations among the study population. Also, the conceptualization of the aging acceleration index, which was calculated based on an algorithm involving genetic determinants of LTL, sex, and chronological age, might facilitate the accurate measurement of possible changes of LTL due to an environmental exposure.

Methods

Data source

The UK Biobank prospectively recruited 502,507 participants aged 40 to 69 across the UK between 2006 and 2010 [21]. All participants filled in questionnaires providing information on socio-demographic, economic, and lifestyle factors as well as self-reported information about their mental and physical health at baseline. Physical examinations and biological sample collection were also performed at the initial health center visit. UK Biobank data were periodically linked with several national registries to track health-related outcomes with the consent of the participants [22]. Inpatient hospital data were obtained through linked data from Hospital Episode Statistics in England, the Scottish Morbidity Record, and the Patient Episode Database in Wales that has covered all UK Biobank participants since 1997 [23]. Primary care data, covering approximately 45% of the entire UK Biobank cohort [24], were derived from multiple data providers, including the Phoenix Partnership and Egton Medical Information Systems.

In the UK Biobank, genotyping data of 488,377 participants were released, based on DNA extracted from blood samples collected at baseline and measured using two similar genotyping arrays with 95% of marker content [21]. Based on the extracted DNA from 474,074 UK Biobank participants, LTL was determined as the ratio of telomere repeat copy number (T) relative to that of a single copy gene (S) using an established qPCR multiplex method [25].

Study design

As this study focused on both biological and facial aging acceleration, indexed by the presence of non-genetically determined portion of shortened LTL (i.e., LTL deviation > 0) and self-reported facial aging acceleration (see ascertainment of those indexes in the “Indexes of aging acceleration” section), respectively, we restricted our analyses to European ancestry participants with eligible genotyping data and the relevant questionnaire data (n = 334,765, Fig. 1A). To assess the biological aging acceleration (i.e., the primary outcome of the study), participants with non-European ancestry were excluded for calculating polygenic risk score (PRS) for LTL, which served as a proxy of genetically determined LTL and was calculated based on publicly available genome-wide association study (GWAS) data derived from European ancestry samples. Furthermore, we excluded 2406 individuals who had outliers of LTL deviation (i.e., values surpassed three standard deviations from the mean), leaving 332,359 participants in the analytic dataset for assessing the association of each psychological or trauma-related factor (i.e., factors related to history of psychiatric disorders, current psychological symptoms, and traumatic experiences) with biological and facial aging acceleration. Then, for 99,180 participants with complete psychological and trauma-related data (i.e., no missing items for all exposure variables), we identified clusters of participants with distinct psycho-trauma exposure patterns using the latent class analysis (LCA) method (see the “Identification of clusters of participants with distinct psychological and trauma exposure patterns” section for details) and compared those indexes of biological and facial aging acceleration between the identified psycho-trauma clusters.

Fig. 1
figure 1

The flowchart of the study. LTL, length of telomeres

Psychological and trauma-related factors

We studied psychological and trauma-related factors related to two perspectives: psychological conditions and traumatic experiences at recruitment (see Additional file 1: Table S1).

Firstly, we assessed psychological conditions through history of psychiatric disorders and current psychological symptoms. To ascertain the history of psychiatric disorders, we retrieved information regarding the diagnoses of psychiatric disorders from the UK Biobank self-report, inpatient hospital, or primary care data at recruitment. We identified four major subtypes of psychiatric disorders, including depression [14], anxiety [15], stress-related disorders [26], and substance misuse [27], according to their corresponding International Classification of Diseases [ICD] 9th or 10th edition (ICD-9/10) codes (see Additional file 1: Table S2). Besides the dichotomous variable (yes or no), to reflect the severity level, we generated ordinal variables, which were in order labeled as “No,” “Self-report or primary care only,” and “Inpatient care” based on the data sources of identified diagnosis codes. Current psychological symptoms at recruitment were measured using four psychometric screening questions (i.e., the frequency of feeling depressed, feeling little interest in doing things, feeling tense, and feeling tired over the preceding 2 weeks) asking for core symptoms of depression [28] and generalized anxiety disorder [29]. Each response used a 4-point Likert scale, ranging from “Not at all” to “Nearly every day.”

Then, the studied traumatic experiences included ACEs and recent stressful life events (SLEs) within 2 years before the baseline. ACEs were assessed by the Childhood Trauma Screener (CTS) [30] during the online follow-up, which consisted of five questions related to physical abuse, physical neglect, emotional abuse, emotional neglect, and sexual abuse, respectively. Each item was measured on a 5-point Likert scale (“Never true,” “Rarely true,” “Sometimes true,” “Often,” and “Very often true”), and we applied previously validated thresholds [30] to determine the presence or absence of each ACE (see Additional file 1: Table S3). In addition, at recruitment, participants were asked about their experiences with six recent SLEs, including serious illness, injury or assault to themselves, serious illness, injury or assault to close relative, death of close relative, death of spouse or partner, marital separation, and financial difficulty, during the last 2 years.

Indexes of aging acceleration

In the current study, we assessed aging acceleration in two ways. The primary index was biological aging acceleration, which was defined as the presence of non-genetically determined portion of shortened LTL (i.e., LTL deviation > 0). The quantification of this index was achieved through two steps. First, we calculated the polygenic risk score (PRS) for LTL, which serves as a proxy for the genetically determined proportion of LTL. Specifically, the PRS for LTL was computed using least absolute shrinkage and selection operator (LASSO) [31, 32] regression based on publicly available summary statistics of GWAS from independent samples [31], containing 5,067,039 single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) for the 332,359 eligible UK Biobank participants. Second, we constructed a linear regression model incorporating the effects of age, sex, and PRS for LTL (i.e., observed LTL ~ genetically determined LTL + age + sex). Then, we defined LTL deviation (median [range], 1.23% [− 45.61% to 45.52%]) as the non-genetically determined portion of shortened LTL which was 100 times of LTL residual divided by predicted LTL (i.e., (predicted LTL − observed LTL)/predicted LTL × 100%). LTL deviation > 0 indicated the presence of biological aging acceleration (i.e., the participants were biologically older than expected).

Facial aging acceleration was measured using a self-reported question at baseline, asking whether participants looked older or younger than their chronological age. The response options were recoded into a binary variable (i.e., “No” = “Younger than you are” or “About your age” and “Yes” = “Older than you are”), to indicate the presence of facial aging acceleration.

Covariates

Data on socio-demographics (e.g., age and sex), socioeconomic (e.g., annual household income and educational years, see Additional file 1: Table S4), and lifestyle (e.g., alcohol drinking status, smoking status, and use of sun protection) factors were collected through questionnaires at baseline. In addition, body mass index (BMI) was calculated from height and weight measured at the initial assessment center visit. To measure area-based deprivation, the Townsend deprivation index (TDI) [33] was generated based on the postcode of the participants’ family address, with higher scores indicating greater deprivation. In addition, we used the Charlson comorbidity index (CCI) [34] to assess baseline somatic comorbidities, based on self-reported, hospital inpatient, and primary care data.

Statistical analysis

Identification of clusters of participants with distinct psycho-trauma patterns

Based on the “poLCA” package in R [35], LCA was used to identify the potential clusters of participants with distinct psycho-trauma exposure patterns, taking into account the correlations and interactions of those trauma and psychological factors. LCA is a model-based approach that utilizes maximum likelihood estimation to evaluate the probability of a participant belonging to each cluster [36]. We compared a series of LCA models in which the number of clusters varied from 2 to 10. The model with 5 clusters was considered optimal as it obtained low values of consistent Akaike information criterion and sample-size adjusted Bayesian information criterion (see Additional file 1: Table S5) but higher average latent class posterior probability and entropy [37], implying superior statistical and interpretable significance. We then applied radar charts to visualize the distinct psycho-trauma patterns of those clusters, which exhibited deviations of each factor in relation to the total population level (see details about the visualization process of the identified patterns in the Additional file 2: Supplementary method).

Associations analyses for psychological and trauma-related factors and patterns with aging acceleration

Logistic regression models were used to examine the associations of 19 individual psychological and trauma factors (i.e., factor-based analyses) and the identified exposure patterns (i.e., cluster-based analyses) with the studied aging acceleration indexes, including the presence of LTL deviation > 0 (yes or no), and facial aging acceleration (yes or no). The models were adjusted for age (as a continuous variable), sex (male or female), BMI (< 25 kg/m2, 25 to 30 kg/m2, ≥ 30 kg/m2, or unknown), CCI (as a continuous variable), smoking and alcohol drinking status (never, previous, current, or unknown), annual household income (less than £18,000, £18,000 to 30,999, £31,000 to 51,999, £52,000 to 100,000, greater than £100,000, or unknown), TDI (as a continuous variable), educational years (7, 10, 13, 15, 19, 20, or unknown), and use of sun protection (most of the time, always, sometimes, never/rarely, do not go out in the sunshine, or unknown). For the factor-based analyses, we used the Benjamini-Hochberg (BH) method to adjust p-values for multiple testing. We considered a false discovery rate (FDR) of < 0.05 as statistically significant. To test the robustness of those results across different outcome definitions, all above-mentioned analyses were repeated for LTL deviation (as a continuous variable) and an alternative definition of the presence of biological aging acceleration (i.e., LTL deviation more than 1SD), based on the linear and logistic regression models, respectively. Furthermore, to detect if the involvement of possible mediators or community-level variable (i.e., TDI that coded according to postal address) in the logistic and linear models could heavily bias our estimates, we conducted additional sensitivity analyses for the factor-based and the cluster-based analyses, including (1) partial (i.e., age, sex, education, annual household income, TDI, and BMI) and full (i.e., additionally adjusted for possible mediators) adjustment for covariates in the models and (2) construction of multi-level logistic and linear regression models to consider TDI as a cluster variable (see Additional file 2: Supplementary method for detailed methodology). For LTL deviation > 0 and facial aging acceleration, we separately calculated odds ratios (ORs) for males and females by dividing the study population into two different sex subgroups. For cluster-based analyses, we then assessed the differences of sub-grouped ORs by introducing an interaction term to the regression models. We conducted repeated cluster-based analyses after performing 10 times of multiple imputation among 107,607 participants, who fully completed the online questionnaire, to assess the influence of missingness (0.0–2.58%, see Additional file 1: Table S6) of psychological and trauma-related factors on the estimation. The overview of the analysis steps was shown in Fig. 1B. All data analysis and visualization were performed based on R 4.0.2.

Results

The mean age of the 332,359 participants that included in the factor-based association analyses (i.e., the association of 19 individual psychological and trauma-related factors with biological and facial aging) was 56.82 years, and 46.40% (n = 154,224) were male (Fig. 1), which was comparable with the 99,180 participants included in the cluster-based analysis (mean age = 56.11 years, male% = 44.86%). Also, other characteristics of those two analytic populations seemed to be similar, except for a higher socioeconomic status (i.e., longer education years and higher annual family incomes) and lower proportions of aging acceleration observed in the population of the cluster-based analyses (Table 1).

Table 1 The characteristics of the included participants

The identified clusters of participants with distinct psycho-trauma patterns

We labeled the 5 identified clusters with distinct psycho-trauma patterns according to their most featured profile (Fig. 2 and Additional file 1: Table S7). Specifically, cluster 1 was labeled as “Absence of trauma and psychopathology” (n = 67,410), which was characterized by lower possibilities of suffering all studied psychopathology and trauma than the total population level. Likewise, cluster 2 to 3 were named as “recent SLEs and mild psychopathology” (n = 18,861) and “ACEs, recent SLEs, and severe psychopathology” (n = 2256). Finally, we observed two clusters of participants who both suffered ACEs but had diverse psychological conditions, named as “ACEs without psychopathology” (cluster 4, n = 7880) and “ACEs with psychopathology” (cluster 5, n = 2773), respectively.

Fig. 2
figure 2

The identified psycho-trauma clusters. The y-axis values (ranging from 0 to 10) for each psychological and trauma-related exposure represent the relative level within identified cluster compared to the total population (for detailed methodology, please refer to Additional file 2: Supplementary method). The colors represent three distinct patterns of combined psychological and trauma-related factors. Green refers to the cluster “Absence of trauma and psychopathology” (n = 67,410), characterized by lower possibilities of suffering all studied trauma and psychopathology than the total population level. Red represents the cluster “Recent SLEs and mild psychopathology” (n = 18,861) and “ACEs, recent SLEs, and severe psychopathology” (n = 2256), which have a similar pattern with combining traumatic experiences along with a matching level of psychopathology (i.e., different levels of traumatic experiences [recent SLEs and recent SLEs plus ACEs] and psychopathology [mild and severe]). Yellow represents the cluster as “ACEs without psychopathology” (n = 7880) and “ACEs with psychopathology” (n = 2773), who both suffered ACEs but had diverse psychological conditions. ACEs, adverse childhood events. SLEs, stressful life events

The associations of psychological and trauma-related factors and clusters with aging acceleration

Of the 19 psychological and trauma-related factors, 10 (mainly factors related to the history of psychiatric disorders and ACEs) showed significant associations with biological aging acceleration (i.e., 9 for LTL deviation > 0, 8 for LTL deviation, and 4 for LTL deviation > 1 SD) (see the inner circle of Fig. 3 and Additional file 1: Tables S8 and S9), with the highest estimates observed for a previous inpatient diagnosis of stress-related disorders (OR = 1.63 [95% CI 1.25 − 2.13]) and substance misuse (OR = 1.21 [95% CI 1.12 − 1.32]). Notably, most ACEs were associated with biological aging acceleration (ORs ranged from 1.06 to 1.11). For facial aging acceleration, we identified 10 factors (mainly factors related to current psychological symptoms, see the outer circle in Fig. 3). Specifically, a previous diagnosis of depression (OR = 1.18 for self-report or primary care and OR = 1.37 for inpatient) and the presence of all current psychological symptoms (ORs ranged from 1.40 to 2.45) were associated with facial aging acceleration. Also, we observed significant associations of facial aging acceleration with emotion-related ACEs (i.e., emotional abuse and emotional neglect) and some subtypes of recent SLEs (e.g., financial difficulty and serious illness, injury or assault to themselves).

Fig. 3
figure 3

The association of the psychological and trauma-related factors with biological aging acceleration (LTL deviation > 0) and facial aging acceleration. The numbers in the circle and degree of colors of the circle in Fig. 3 indicates the magnitude of odds ratios (ORs), derived from the fully adjusted logistic regression models. ORs with BH adjusted p-value < 0.05 were considered as statistically significant and shown in bold. Estimates with 95% confidence intervals are detailed in Additional file 1: Table S8. LTL, the length of telomeres. TDI, Townsend deprivation index. CCI, Charlson comorbidity index. BMI, body mass index. “a” symbol indicates the following: the associations of the psychological and trauma-related factors with biological aging acceleration were adjusted for age, sex, education, annual household income, TDI, BMI, smoking status, alcohol drinking status, and CCI. “b” indicates the following: the associations of the psychological and trauma factors with facial aging acceleration were adjusted for age, sex, education, annual household income, TDI, BMI, smoking status, alcohol drinking status, CCI, and the use of sun protection

For the cluster-based associations, using the “Absence of trauma and psychopathology” cluster as a reference, the cluster featuring “ACEs, recent SLEs, and severe psychopathology” demonstrated the most pronounced risk excess in biological aging acceleration (OR = 1.13 [95% CI 1.03 − 1.23], Table 2). Intriguingly, while the “ACEs with psychopathology” cluster showed a strong association with biological aging acceleration (OR = 1.13 [95% CI 1.05 − 1.23]), no such association was observed for the “ACEs without psychopathology” cluster (OR = 1.04 [95% CI 0.99 − 1.09]). We observed largely similar risk patterns when using LTL deviation and the presence of LTL deviation more than 1SD as alternative measurements of biological aging acceleration (Additional file 1: Table S10). For facial aging acceleration, we found significant associations for all clusters with the presence of current psychological symptoms (irrespective of symptom severity, Table 2), particularly for the cluster “ACEs, recent SLEs, and severe psychopathology” cluster (OR = 2.20 [95% CI 1.75 − 2.76]). Also, largely similar results were obtained when performing a repeated cluster-based analyses using the 10 datasets after multiple interpolation (Additional file 1: Table S11).

Table 2 The association of the identified potential psychological and trauma clusters with biological and facial aging acceleration

After conducting the two sensitivity analyses mentioned above (i.e., partial and full adjustment for covariates, and considering TDI as a cluster variable in the logistic and linear regression models), our estimation for the factor-based and the cluster-based analyses remained largely unchanged, demonstrating the robustness of the results (Additional file 1: Tables S12-S15).

The sex-specific association of psychological and trauma-related factors and patterns with aging acceleration

Compared with those of male participants, we observed a stronger magnitude of these associations for both biological and facial aging acceleration among female participants (see Additional file 1: Fig. S1). We then observed sex-specific associations between psychological and trauma-related factors and facial aging acceleration, with some recent SLEs over the past 2 years (e.g., financial difficulty) only being associated with an increased risk of aging acceleration in men. There were no significant interactions between sex group and the identified psychological and trauma-related clusters for risk of biological aging acceleration (P for interaction = 0 0.166, Additional file 1: Table S16) and facial aging acceleration (P for interaction = 0 0.640, Additional file 1: Table S17).

Discussion

Based on 332,359 participants from the UK Biobank, our study provided robust evidence that most adverse psychological and trauma exposures were associated with an elevated risk of biological aging acceleration (10 out of 19 studied factors) measured by a newly developed biological aging acceleration index (LTL deviation > 0 or LTL deviation or LTL deviation > 1 SD) which takes into account genetic determinants of LTL shortening, and facial aging acceleration (10 out of 19), highlighting the substantial impacts of psychological and traumatic experiences on accelerating the aging process. Specifically, factors related to history of psychiatric disorders and childhood adversities were primarily linked with biological aging acceleration, whereas current psychological symptoms were noted for their pronounced associations with an excess risk of facial aging acceleration. More importantly, we identified five major psycho-trauma clusters based on clustering analyses, and further found that the cluster characterized by “ACEs with psychopathology” was associated with both biological and facial aging acceleration, in contrast to null results for the cluster featured by “ACEs without psychopathology.” Such findings imply that interventions targeting on psychological resilience to ACEs might have the potential to mitigate the detrimental effects of ACEs on the aging process.

In line with the results of previous meta-analyses investigating the association of LTL with depression [14] (n = 34,347), anxiety [15] (n = 19,424), stress-related disorders [26] (n = 3851), and substance misuse [27] (n = 7203) on LTL, our study also observed significant associations between a history of those psychiatric disorders and biological aging acceleration indexed by LTL deviation, based on a much larger sample size (n = 332,359). Likewise, significant negative associations between ACEs and biological aging acceleration (e.g., shorter LTL [17], older biological age [38], and DNA methylation-based epigenetic age acceleration [39]) have been reported, while existing data on adulthood or recent traumatic events are scarce. For facial aging acceleration, only one previous prospective cohort study of 1826 twins found that higher depression scores (i.e., emotion-related current psychological symptoms) were associated with an increased risk of self-reported facial aging acceleration [19]. Therefore, our study substantially extended the current evidence on the importance of various psycho-trauma factors, particularly current psychological symptoms and emotion-related ACEs, on facial aging acceleration. Recent major adverse experiences, such as serious illness, injury, or assault to a close relative or themselves, showed moderate impacts on facial aging acceleration but a very mild effect on biological aging acceleration. This fits findings based on a study of 3034 US residents aged 25 to 74, suggesting self-reported health condition decline would produce increases in subjective age [40]. However, financial difficulty experienced during the last 2 years seemed to be linked to facial aging acceleration among males. Sex-specific associations between certain SLEs (e.g., financial difficulty) and health adversities (e.g., cardiovascular disease and all-cause mortality [41]) were also found in prior investigations, possibly reflecting the differentiated social roles related to each sex.

The complexity of psycho-trauma adversities has been revealed [42]. For instance, individuals exposed to adverse childhood events were also prone to adversities during adulthood [43], both of which contributed to undermined physical conditions among exposed individuals [44]. Therefore, it is crucial, but challenging, to determine the patterns of psycho-trauma exposure for weighting the attributable risk to each individual factor on a specific outcome of interest. In the present study, with enriched data on psychological and trauma factors and well-developed unsupervised machine learning algorithms, we, for the first time, identified five major exposure patterns among our study population. In brief, more than two thirds (67.97%, 67,410/99,180) of the included individuals were considered to have total absence of trauma and psychopathology, while all the others suffered certain trauma and psychopathology. The most common exposure patterns are those combining traumatic experiences (mainly recent SLEs) along with a matching extent of psychopathology (e.g., patterns 2–3, including different levels of traumatic experience [recent SLEs and recent SLEs plus ACEs] and psychopathology [mild and severe]). Intriguingly, we additionally identified two clusters of individuals with similar ACEs exposure but opposite psychological condition (i.e., with and without psychopathology). The finding that an excess risk of biological and facial aging acceleration was only noted for the “ACEs with psychopathology” pattern but not “ACEs without psychopathology” pattern imply a strong mediating role of psychological distress on the effect of ACEs on the aging acceleration. Without any directly comparable data, a similar conclusion could be drawn from a Danish study of 324 male adults, which also found that 16% of the effect of ACEs on LTL shortening (i.e., biological aging acceleration) was mediated by current depressive symptoms [45]. As aging acceleration, particularly biological aging acceleration, is considered as a proxy of overall health status [46] and has been linked with many disease outcomes in later life (e.g., cancer [47] and cardiovascular diseases [48]), our findings (i.e., psychological distress might play a mediating role on the effect of ACEs on the aging acceleration) provide strongest evidence so far, prompting the use of psychological interventions for preventing adverse health consequences among individuals exposed to ACEs.

The potential mechanisms linking trauma and psychopathology to accelerated aging might include neuroendocrine responses through the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) axis, oxidative stress, and inflammation [49]. The activation of the HPA axis by stress from trauma causes a surge of glucocorticoids [50] such as cortisol, leading to the generation of reactive oxygen species (ROS) by increasing the metabolic rate and mitochondrial activity [51]. Subsequently, ROS might damage telomeres and inhibit telomerase activity, both of which contribute to telomeres shortening [49]. Elevated glucocorticoids can increase the expression of pro-inflammatory genes (e.g., iNOS, IL-1β, TNF-α) and decrease the expression of anti-inflammatory genes including IL-1ra, IL-10, and MKP-1, also leading to telomeres shortening [49]. Additionally, the production of glucocorticoids, ROS, and inflammatory markers would destroy collagen and elastin in the skin, accelerating wrinkling and sagging and leading to facial aging [52]. Interestingly, it is reported that the alternations of HPA axis induced by stress actions after traumatic events [53] or psychopathology (e.g., depression [54] and anxiety [55]) are possibly reversible, which explains our findings of no accelerated aging among participants with childhood adversities but who maintained good psychological conditions.

The major merit of this study is the use of a large community-based cohort from the UK Biobank, providing data of comprehensive assessments on psychological and trauma-related factors (e.g., history of psychiatric disorders, current psychological symptoms, recent SLEs, and ACEs), social demographic factors, somatic comorbidities, and lifestyles. In addition, benefiting from the availability of individual-level genotyping data and measurement of telomeres, we proposed LTL deviation > 0 as an index of biological aging, which represented the presence of non-genetically determined proportions shortened LTL (i.e., non-genetically determined aging acceleration), thereby contributing to a more accurate measure of aging and better interpretation of results. Last, to account for the complex interactions between psychological and trauma-related factors, we used LCA to capture clusters with distinct psycho-trauma patterns. The cluster-based analyses provide clearer clues about the substantial contributing factors to the studied outcomes, as well as identifying individuals at high risk of aging acceleration who need targeted interventions.

Major limitations include its cross-sectional design. Although the studied trauma-related factors (such as ACEs and recent SLEs) ensure the chronological order (from exposure to outcomes of interest) by their definitions, the fact that data on most exposure factors and measurements of aging acceleration were collected at the same time provides a very weak basis for any causal inference. The inclusion of several covariates (e.g., smoking status and alcohol drinking status) that might mediate the impact of psycho-trauma related factors and clusters on aging acceleration resulted an underestimation of the studied association. In addition, despite the self-related facial aging acceleration have also been widely used in previous studies [56, 57], as an index for subjective aging [58] with proven associations with risk of mortality [59], the validity of such measurement remains uncertain. It is particularly notable as merely less than 2% of participants reported “looking older than the chronological age” in the present study. Furthermore, there are biases in the assessment of facial aging acceleration, which can be influenced by psychological and traumatic factors, making this measure less valid compared to biological aging acceleration in our study. Therefore, the results for facial aging acceleration analyses need to be interpreted with cautious, and studies which applied more objective or preferable approach for facial aging acceleration measurement (e.g., facial aging evaluated by nurses or other healthcare professionals [18]) are needed to verify our findings. Finally, the generalization of our findings to both the general UK population and other populations needs to be done with caution, as the UK Biobank participants were only 5.5% of the invited individuals [23].

Conclusions

In conclusion, our study demonstrates significant associations of psycho-trauma related factors and patterns with both biological and facial aging acceleration. The findings of aging acceleration among ACEs exposed individuals with psychopathology, but not those with psychopathology, underscore the potential of interventions aiming to improve individuals’ psychological resilience on preventing aging acceleration.