Abdellaoui, A., Hugh-Jones, D., Kemper, K. E., Holtz, Y., Nivard, M. G., Veul, L., Yengo, L., Zietsch, B. P., Frayling, T. M., Wray, N., Yang, J., Verweij, K. J. H., & Visscher, P. M. (2018). Genetic consequences of social stratification in Great Britain. BioRxiv, 457515. https://doi.org/10.1101/457515.
Alexander, P. A. (2019). Individual differences in college-age learners: the importance of relational reasoning for learning and assessment in higher education. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 416–428. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12264.
Article
Google Scholar
Amd, M., & Roche, B. (2018). Assessing the effects of a relational training intervention on fluid intelligence among a sample of socially disadvantaged children in Bangladesh. The Psychological Record, 68(2). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0273-4.
Andrés, M. L., Richaud De Minzi, M. C., Castañeiras, C., Canet-Juric, L., & Rodríguez-Carvajal, R. (2016). Neuroticism and depression in Children: The Role of Cognitive Emotion Regulation Strategies. Journal of Genetic Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1080/00221325.2016.1148659.
Au, J., Sheehan, E., Tsai, N., Duncan, G. J., Buschkuehl, M., & Jaeggi, S. M. (2015). Improving fluid intelligence with training on working memory: a meta-analysis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 22(2), 366–377.
Article
Google Scholar
Bergold, S., & Steinmayr, R. (2018). Personality and intelligence interact in the prediction of academic achievement. https://doi.org/10.3390/jintelligence6020027.
Book
Google Scholar
Buschkuehl, M., Hernandez-Garcia, L., Jaeggi, S. M., Bernard, J. A., & Jonides, J. (2014). Neural effects of short-term training on working memory. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(1), 147–160.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., Colbert, D., Stewart, I., & Grey, I. M. (2016). A relational frame skills training intervention to increase general intelligence and scholastic aptitude. Learning and Individual Differences, 47, 222–235. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2016.03.0011041-6080.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & Hayes, S. C. (2011). A relational frame training intervention to raise intelligence quotients: a pilot study. The Psychological Record, 61(2), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395755.
Article
Google Scholar
Cassidy, S., Roche, B., & O’Hora, D. (2010). Relational frame theory and human intelligence. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 11(1), 37–51. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2010.11434333.
Article
Google Scholar
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Ahmetoglu, G., & Furnham, A. (2008). Little more than personality: dispositional determinants of test anxiety (the big five, core self-evaluations, and self-assessed intelligence). Learning and Individual Differences, 18(2), 258–263. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.09.002.
Article
Google Scholar
Chooi, W.-T., & Thompson, L. A. (2012). Working memory training does not improve intelligence in healthy young adults. Intelligence, 40(6), 531–542.
Article
Google Scholar
Colbert, D., Dobutowitsch, M., Roche, B., & Brophy, C. (2017). The proxy-measurement of intelligence quotients using a relational skills abilities index. Learning and Individual Differences, 57, 114–122. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.010.
Article
Google Scholar
Colbert, D., Tyndall, I., Roche, B., & Cassidy, S. (2018). Can SMART training really increase intelligence? A replication study. Journal of Behavioral Education, 27(4), 509–531. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10864-018-9302-2.
Article
Google Scholar
Colom, R., & Román, F. J. (2018). Enhancing intelligence: from the group to the individual. Journal of Intelligence, 6(1), 11.
Article
Google Scholar
Colom, R., Román, F. J., Abad, F. J., Shih, P. C., Privado, J., Froufe, M., Escorial, S., Martínez, K., Burgaleta, M., & Quiroga, M. A. (2013). Adaptive n-back training does not improve fluid intelligence at the construct level: gains on individual tests suggest that training may enhance visuospatial processing. Intelligence, 41(5), 712–727.
Article
Google Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., & Kovacs, K. (2015). New and emerging models of human intelligence. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(5), 419–426.
PubMed
Google Scholar
Davies, G., Lam, M., Harris, S. E., Trampush, J. W., Luciano, M., Hill, W. D., Hagenaars, S. P., Ritchie, S. J., Marioni, R. E., Fawns-Ritchie, C., Liewald, D. C. M., Okely, J. A., Ahola-Olli, A. V., Barnes, C. L. K., Bertram, L., Bis, J. C., Burdick, K. E., Christoforou, A., DeRosse, P., et al. (2018). Study of 300,486 individuals identifies 148 independent genetic loci influencing general cognitive function. Nature Communications, 9(1), 2098. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-018-04362-x.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Davis, T., Goldwater, M., & Giron, J. (2017). From concrete examples to abstract relations: the rostrolateral prefrontal cortex integrates novel examples into relational categories. Cerebral Cortex, 27(4), 2652–2670. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw099.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
De Oviedo, U., Carrasco Ortiz, E., Ángel, M., Tello, H., Pablo, F., Gandara, D. B., & Victoria, M. (2005). Dimensionalidad del cuestionario de los cinco grandes (BFQ-N) en población infantil española [Big five questionnaire dimensions in Spanish children (BFQ-C)]. Psicothema, 17(2), 286–291 http://www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=72717216.
Google Scholar
Detterman, D. K. (2017). Education and intelligence: pity the poor teacher because student characteristics are more significant than teachers or schools. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 19, E93. https://doi.org/10.1017/sjp.2016.88.
Article
Google Scholar
Dymond, S., & Roche, B. (2013). Advances in relational frame theory: research and application. Context Press.
Everaert, M. B. H., Huybregts, M. A. C., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2015). Structures, not strings: linguistics as part of the cognitive sciences. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(12), 729–743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.09.008.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Fissler, P., Müller, H.-P., Küster, O. C., Laptinskaya, D., Thurm, F., Woll, A., Elbert, T., Kassubek, J., von Arnim, C. A. F., & Kolassa, I.-T. (2017). No evidence that short-term cognitive or physical training programs or lifestyles are related to changes in white matter integrity in older adults at risk of dementia. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 11, 110.
Article
Google Scholar
Gaio, V. (2012). Psychometric Properties of the Big Five Questionnaire-Children (BFQ-C) in American Adolescents [Arizona State University]. https://repository.asu.edu/attachments/93558/content//tmp/package-It1sWU/Gaio_asu_0010N_11584.pdf
Goldwater, M. B. (2017). Grammatical constructions as relational categories. Topics in Cognitive Science. https://doi.org/10.1111/tops.12272.
Goldwater, M. B., Don, H. J., Krusche, M. J. F., & Livesey, E. J. (2018). Relational discovery in category learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 1–35. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000387.
Article
Google Scholar
Goldwater, M. B., & Schalk, L. (2016). Relational categories as a bridge between cognitive and educational research. Psychological Bulletin, 142(7), 729–757. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000043.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Gottfredson, L. S., & Deary, I. J. (2004). Intelligence predicts health and longevity, but why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(1), 1–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01301001.x.
Article
Google Scholar
Haier, R. J. (2016). The neuroscience of intelligence. Cambridge University Press.
Halford, G. S., Wilson, W. H., & Phillips, S. (2010). Relational knowledge: the foundation of higher cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 497–505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.005.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hayes, J., & Stewart, I. (2016). Comparing the effects of derived relational training and computer coding on intellectual potential in school-age children. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(3), 397–411. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjep.12114.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: a post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. Plenum Press.
Hilbert, S., Schwaighofer, M., Zech, A., Sarubin, N., Arendasy, M., & Bühner, M. (2017). Working memory tasks train working memory but not reasoning: a material- and operation-specific investigation of transfer from working memory practice. Intelligence, 61, 102–114. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.INTELL.2017.01.010.
Article
Google Scholar
Hill, W. D., Marioni, R. E., Maghzian, O., Ritchie, S. J., Hagenaars, S. P., McIntosh, A. M., Gale, C. R., Davies, G., & Deary, I. J. (2018b). A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, 1–13. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0001-5.
Hill, W. D., Arslan, R. C., Xia, C., Luciano, M., Amador, C., Navarro, P., Hayward, C., Nagy, R., Porteous, D. J., McIntosh, A. M., Deary, I. J., Haley, C. S., & Penke, L. (2018a). Genomic analysis of family data reveals additional genetic effects on intelligence and personality. Molecular Psychiatry, 1–16. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41380-017-0005-1.
Ivcevic, Z., & Brackett, M. (2014). Predicting school success: comparing conscientiousness, grit, and emotion regulation ability. Journal of Research in Personality, 52(10), 29–36. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2014.06.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Jonides, J., & Shah, P. (2011). Short-and long-term benefits of cognitive training. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(25), 10081–10086.
Article
Google Scholar
Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., Shah, P., & Jonides, J. (2014). The role of individual differences in cognitive training and transfer. Memory & Cognition, 42(3), 464–480.
Article
Google Scholar
Jang, K. L., Livesley, W. J., & Vemon, P. A. (1996). Heritability of the big five personality dimensions and their facets: a twin study. Journal of Personality, 64(3), 577–592.
Article
Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1989). Raising IQ without increasing g? A review of the Milwaukee project: preventing mental retardation in children at risk the Milwaukee project: preventing mental retardation in children at risk. By. Developmental Review, 9, 234–258. http://arthurjensen.net/wp-content/uploads/2014/06/Raising-IQ-Without-Increasing-g-Review-of-Howard-L.-Garbers-The-Milwaukee-Project-Preventing-Mental-Retardation-in-Children-at-Risk-1989-by-Arthur-Robert-Jensen.pdf
Jones, K. T., Peterson, D. J., Blacker, K. J., & Berryhill, M. E. (2017). Frontoparietal neurostimulation modulates working memory training benefits and oscillatory synchronization. Brain Research, 1667, 28–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.BRAINRES.2017.05.005.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Kaptchuk, T. J. (2001). The double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial: Gold standard or golden calf? In Journal of Clinical Epidemiology (Vol. 54). https://www.medschool.lsuhsc.edu/internal_medicine/residency/docs/JC 2015-03 gold standard or golden calf_Nesh.PDF.
Kassai, R., Futo, J., Demetrovics, Z., & Takacs, Z. K. (2019). A meta-analysis of the experimental evidence on the near- and far-transfer effects among children’s executive function skills. Psychological Bulletin, 145(2), 165–188. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000180.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman brief intelligence test. Wiley Online Library.
Google Scholar
Kaufman, S. B., DeYoung, C. G., Gray, J. R., Brown, J., & Mackintosh, N. (2009). Associative learning predicts intelligence above and beyond working memory and processing speed. Intelligence, 37(4), 374–382.
Article
Google Scholar
Kishita, N., Ohtsuki, T., & Stewart, I. (2013). The training and assessment of relational precursors and abilities (TARPA): a follow-up study with typically developing children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 2(1–2), 15–21. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2013.01.001.
Article
Google Scholar
Komarraju, M., Karau, S. J., Schmeck, R. R., & Avdic, A. (2011). The big five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 472–477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019.
Article
Google Scholar
Lipkens, R., Hayes, S. C., & Hayes, L. J. (1993). Longitudinal study of the development of derived relations in an infant. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1993.1032.
West, M. R., Kraft, M. A., Finn, A. S., Martin, R. E., Duckworth, A. L., Gabrieli, C. F. O., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2016). Promise and Paradox: Measuring Students’ Non-Cognitive Skills and the Impact of Schooling. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis. https://doi.org/10.3102/0162373715597298.
McKey, R. H. (1985). The impact of head start on children, families and communities. Synthesis and Utilization Project: Final Report of the Head Start Evaluation. https://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED263984.pdf.
Google Scholar
McLoughlin, S., & Stewart, I. (2017). Empirical advances in studying relational networks. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(3), 329–342. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.JCBS.2016.11.009.
Article
Google Scholar
McLoughlin, S., Tyndall, I., & Pereira, A. (2018). Piloting a brief relational operant training program: analyses of response latencies and intelligence test performance. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 19(2), 228–246. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2018.1507087.
Article
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2013). Is working memory training effective? A meta-analytic review. Developmental Psychology, 49(2), 270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028228.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., & Hulme, C. (2016). There is no convincing evidence that working memory training is effective: A reply to Au et al.(2014) and Karbach and Verhaeghen (2014). Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 23(1), 324–330. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-015-0862-z.
Article
Google Scholar
Melby-Lervåg, M., Redick, T. S., & Hulme, C. (2016). Working memory training does not improve performance on measures of intelligence or other measures of “far transfer” evidence from a meta-analytic review. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11(4), 512–534. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691616635612.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Moran, L., Walsh, L., Stewart, I., McElwee, J., & Ming, S. (2015). Correlating derived relational responding with linguistic and cognitive ability in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 19, 32–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.rasd.2014.12.015.
Article
Google Scholar
Morris, S. B. (2008). Estimating effect sizes from pretest-posttest-control group designs. Organizational Research Methods. https://doi.org/10.1177/1094428106291059.
Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & Elwee, J. M. (2017). Investigating relational framing of categorization in young children. Psychological Record, 67(4), 519–536. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0255-y.
Article
Google Scholar
Mulhern, T., Stewart, I., & McElwee, J. (2018). Facilitating relational framing of classification in young children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 8, 55–68. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.04.001.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Hora, D., Pelaez, M., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2005). Derived relational responding and performance in verbal subtests of the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 55, 155–175.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Hora, D., Peláez, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., Rae, G., Robinson, K., & Chaudhary, T. (2008). Temporal relations and intelligence: correlating relational performance with performance on the WAIS-III. The Psychological Record, 58(1), 569–584. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395638.
Article
Google Scholar
O’Toole, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Murphy, C., O’Connor, J., & Barnes-Holmes, Y. (2009). Relational flexibility and human intelligence: extending the remit of skinner’s verbal behavior. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 9(1), 1–17.
Google Scholar
Parra, I., & Ruiz, F. J. (2016). The effect on intelligence quotient of training fluency in relational frames of coordination. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 16(1), 1–12.
Google Scholar
Plomin, R., & Von Stumm, S. (2018). The new genetics of intelligence. Nature Reviews Genetics, 19(3), 148–159. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2017.104.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. (2015). Intelligence: all that matters. Hodder & Stoughton.
Roberts, B. W., Luo, J., Briley, D. A., Chow, P. I., Su, R., & Hill, P. L. (2017). A systematic review of personality trait change through intervention. Psychological Bulletin, 143(2), 117–141. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000088.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Ruiz, F. J., & Luciano, C. (2011). Cross-domain analogies as relating derived relations among two separate relational networks. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95(3), 369–385. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.2011.95-369.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017a). Does chess instruction improve mathematical problem-solving ability? Two experimental studies with an active control group. Learning & Behavior, 45(4), 414–421. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-017-0280-3.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017b). Does far transfer exist? Negative evidence from chess, music, and working memory training. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(6), 515–520. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417712760.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017c). When the music’s over. Does music skill transfer to children’s and young adolescents’ cognitive and academic skills? A meta-analysis. Educational Research Review, 20, 55–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.edurev.2016.11.005.
Article
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2017d). Working memory training in typically developing children: a meta-analysis of the available evidence. Developmental Psychology, 53(4), 671–685. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000265.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sala, G., & Gobet, F. (2019). Cognitive training does not enhance general cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(1), 9–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2018.10.004.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sala, G., Tatlidil, K. S., & Gobet, F. (2018). Video game training does not enhance cognitive ability: a comprehensive meta-analytic investigation. Psychological Bulletin, 144(2), 111–139. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000139.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Sandoz, E. K., & Hebert, E. R. (2017). Using derived relational responding to model statistics learning across participants with varying degrees of statistics anxiety. European Journal of Behavior Analysis, 18(1), 113–131. https://doi.org/10.1080/15021149.2016.1146552.
Article
Google Scholar
Savage, J. E., Jansen, P. R., Stringer, S., Watanabe, K., Bryois, J., de Leeuw, C. A., Nagel, M., Awasthi, S., Barr, P. B., Coleman, J. R. I., Grasby, K. L., Hammerschlag, A. R., Kaminski, J. A., Karlsson, R., Krapohl, E., Lam, M., Nygaard, M., Reynolds, C. A., Trampush, J. W., et al. (2018). Genome-wide association meta-analysis in 269,867 individuals identifies new genetic and functional links to intelligence. Nature Genetics, 1. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0152-6.
Schlinger, H. D. (2003). The myth of intelligence. In The Psychological Record (Vol. 53, pp. 15–32).
Schulte, M. J., Ree, M. J., & Carretta, T. R. (2004). Emotional intelligence: not much more than g and personality. Personality and Individual Differences, 37(5), 1059–1068. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2003.11.014.
Article
Google Scholar
Schwaighofer, M., Fischer, F., & Bühner, M. (2015). Does working memory training transfer? A meta-analysis including training conditions as moderators. Educational Psychologist, 50(2), 138–166.
Article
Google Scholar
Shipstead, Z., Redick, T. S., & Engle, R. W. (2012). Is working memory training effective? Psychological Bulletin, 138(4), 628.
Article
Google Scholar
Simons, D. J., Boot, W. R., Charness, N., Gathercole, S. E., Chabris, C. F., Hambrick, D. Z., & Stine-Morrow, E. A. L. (2016). Do “brain-training” programs work? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 17(3), 103–186. https://doi.org/10.1177/1529100616661983.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
Smeets, P. M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Derived stimulus-response and stimulus-stimulus relations in children and Adults: Assessing Training Order Effects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.2000.2563.
Smith-Woolley, E., Pingault, J.-B., Selzam, S., Rimfeld, K., Krapohl, E., von Stumm, S., Asbury, K., Dale, P. S., Young, T., Allen, R., Kovas, Y., & Plomin, R. (2018). Differences in exam performance between pupils attending selective and non-selective schools mirror the genetic differences between them. Npj Science of Learning, 3(1), 3. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41539-018-0019-8.
Article
PubMed
PubMed Central
Google Scholar
Soveri, A., Karlsson, E., Waris, O., Grönholm-Nyman, P., & Laine, M. (2017). Pattern of near transfer effects following working memory training with a dual n-back task. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 240.
Article
Google Scholar
Stephenson, C. L., & Halpern, D. F. (2013). Improved matrix reasoning is limited to training on tasks with a visuospatial component. Intelligence, 41(5), 341–357.
Article
Google Scholar
Strenze, T. (2007). Intelligence and socioeconomic success: a meta-analytic review of longitudinal research. Intelligence, 35(5), 401–426.
Article
Google Scholar
Studer-Luethi, B., Jaeggi, S. M., Buschkuehl, M., & Perrig, W. J. (2012). Influence of neuroticism and conscientiousness on working memory training outcome. Personality and Individual Differences, 53(1), 44–49.
Article
Google Scholar
Thirus, J., Starbrink, M., & Jansson, B. (2016). Relational frame theory, mathematical and logical skills: a multiple exemplar training intervention to enhance intellectual performance. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 16(2), 141–155.
Google Scholar
Thompson, T. W., Waskom, M. L., Garel, K.-L. A., Cardenas-Iniguez, C., Reynolds, G. O., Winter, R., Chang, P., Pollard, K., Lala, N., & Alvarez, G. A. (2013). Failure of working memory training to enhance cognition or intelligence. PLoS One, 8(5), e63614.
Article
Google Scholar
Van Der Maas, H. L. J., Dolan, C. V, Grasman, R. P. P. P., Wicherts, J. M., Huizenga, H. M., & Raijmakers, M. E. J. (2006). A dynamical model of general intelligence: the positive manifold of intelligence by mutualism. Psychological Review, 113(4), 842.
Viken, R. J., Rose, R. J., Kaprio, J., & Koskenvuo, M. (1994). A developmental genetic analysis of adult personality: extraversion and neuroticism from 18 to 59 years of age. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 722–730. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.66.4.722.
Vizcaíno-Torres, R. M., Ruiz, F. J., Luciano, C., López-López, J. C., Barbero-Rubio, A., & Gil, E. (2015). The effect of relational training on intelligence quotient: a case study. Psicothema, 27, 120–127. https://doi.org/10.7334/psicothema2014.149.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar
von Bastian, C. C., Langer, N., Jäncke, L., & Oberauer, K. (2013). Effects of working memory training in young and old adults. Memory & Cognition, 41(4), 611–624.
Article
Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Hayes, S. C. (2018). Evolution and contextual behavioral science: an integrated framework for understanding, predicting, and influencing human behavior. New Harbinger: Context Press.
Google Scholar
Zabaneh, D., Krapohl, E., Gaspar, H. A., Curtis, C., Lee, S. H., Patel, H., Newhouse, S., Wu, H. M., Simpson, M. A., Putallaz, M., Lubinski, D., Plomin, R., & Breen, G. (2017). A genome-wide association study for extremely high intelligence. Molecular Psychiatry, April, 2017, 1226–1232. https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2017.121.
Article
Google Scholar
Zahra, D., Hedge, C., Pesola, F., & Burr, S. (2016). Accounting for test reliability in student progression: the reliable change index. Medical Education, 50(7), 738–745. https://doi.org/10.1111/medu.13059.
Article
PubMed
Google Scholar