Skip to main content
Log in

Reading in French-speaking adults with dyslexia

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study investigated the reading and reading-related skills of 15 French-speaking adults with dyslexia, whose performance was compared with that of chronological-age controls (CA) and reading-level controls (RL). Experiment 1 assessed the efficiency of their phonological reading-related skills (phonemic awareness, phonological short-term memory, and rapid automatic naming (RAN)) and experiment 2 assessed the efficiency of their lexical and sublexical (or phonological) reading procedures (reading aloud of pseudowords and irregular words of different lengths). Experiment 1 revealed that adults with dyslexia exhibited lower phonological reading-related skills than CAs only, and were better than RL controls on the RAN. In experiment 2, as compared with RL controls, only a deficit in the sublexical reading procedure was observed. The results of the second experiment replicated observations from English-language studies but not those of the first experiment. Several hypotheses are discussed to account for these results, including one related to the transparency of orthographic systems.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. However, in one study (Ransby & Swanson, 2003) no differences in accuracy were found between English-speaking adults with dyslexia and RL controls on a phoneme deletion task.

  2. The Alouette-R (Lefavrais, 2005) has been standardized on pupils up to the age of 16 years. However, the groups used for this standardization were fairly small (on average 36 readers per level), so we preferred to rely on the original version.

  3. Note that a dyslexic pseudoword deficit can be observed on accuracy scores in a transparent orthography, at least for very poor young readers (e.g. Sprenger-Charolles et al., 2009).

  4. However, see Ransby and Swanson (2003), who found English-speaking adults with dyslexia and reading-level controls to perform similarly on pseudoword reading accuracy.

References

  • Backman, J., Bruck, M., Hebert, M., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1984). Acquisition and use of spelling-sound correspondences in reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38(1), 114–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron, J., & Strawson, C. (1976). Use of orthographic and word-specific knowledge in reading words aloud. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2(3), 386–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Dror, I., Pollatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college dyslexic students. Brain and Language, 40(4), 471–490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogliotti, C., Serniclaes, W., Messaoud-Galusi, S., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2008). Discrimination of speech sounds by dyslexic children: Comparisons with chronological age and reading level controls. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 137–155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed's unique contributions to reading disabilities over time. Reading and Writing, 7(2), 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L., & Bryant, P. E. (1985). Children's reading problems. Oxford: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., & Goswami, U. (1986). Strengths and weaknesses of the reading level design: A comment on Backman, Mamen, and Ferguson. Psychological Bulletin, 100(1), 101–103.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, P., & Impey, L. (1986). The similarities between normal readers and developmental and acquired dyslexics. Cognition, 24(1–2), 121–137.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M., Demetre, J., Hamill, S., Robson, K., Shepherd, H., & Cody, G. (2002). Executive functioning in adults and children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 40, 2144–2155.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1988). The word recognition and spelling of dyslexic children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23(1), 51–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26(3), 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics' phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28(5), 874–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1993). Word recognition and component phonological processing skills of adults with childhood diagnosis of dyslexia. Developmental Review, 13, 258–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics: A search for Wernicke's Wortschatz? Brain, 122(10), 1901–1917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Casalis, S. (1995). Lecture et dyslexies de l'enfant. Lille: Presses Universitaires du Septentrion.

    Google Scholar 

  • Casalis, S., Colé, P., & Sopo, D. (2004). Morphological awareness in developmental dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 54(1), 114–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chiappe, P., Stringer, R., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (2002). Why the timing deficit hypothesis does not explain reading disability in adults. Reading and Writing, 15(1), 73–107.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (1978). Lexical access in simple reading tasks. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Strategies of information processing (pp. 151–216). London: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Curtis, B., Atkins, P., & Haller, M. (1993). Models of reading aloud: Dual-route and parallel-distributed-processing approaches. Psychological Review, 100(4), 589–608.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: a dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108(1), 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Content, A., & Radeau, M. (1988). Données statistiques sur la structure orthographique du Français. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/European Bulletin of Cognitive Psychology (Special issue), 8(4), 399–404.

  • Coulmas, F. (1996). The Blackwell Encyclopedia of Writing Systems. Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Delattre, P. (1966). Studies in French and comparative phonetics. The Hague: Mouton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Downey, D., Snyder, L., & Hill, B. (2000). College students with dyslexia: persistent linguistic deficits and foreign language learning. Dyslexia, 6(2), 101–111.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dufor, O., Serniclaes, W., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Demonet, J.-F. (2007). Top-down processes during auditory phoneme categorization in dyslexia: A PET study. Neuroimage, 34(4), 1692–1707.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Petersen, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Foorman, B., Francis, D., Fletcher, J., & Lynn, A. (1996). Relation of phonological and orthographic processing to early reading: comparing two approaches to regression-based, reading-level-match designs. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(4), 639–652.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gonzalez, J. E. J., & Valle, I. H. (2000). Word identification and reading disorders in the Spanish language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(1), 44–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (2002). In the beginning was the rhyme? A reflection on Hulme, Hatcher, Nation, Brown, Adams, and Stuart (2002). Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 82(1), 47–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U., Gombert, J. E., & de Barrera, L. F. (1998). Children’s orthographic representations and linguistic transparency: nonsense word reading in English, French, and Spanish. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19(1), 19–52.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grainger, J., Bouttevin, S., Truc, C., Bastien, M., & Ziegler, J. (2003). Word superiority, pseudoword superiority, and learning to read: a comparison of dyslexic and normal readers. Brain and Language, 87, 432–440.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffiths, S., & Frith, U. (2002). Evidence for an articulatory awareness deficit in adult dyslexics. Dyslexia, 8(1), 14–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, J. R. (1997). Reading and Spelling Impairments in Undergraduate Students with Developmental Dyslexia. Journal of Research in Reading, 20(1), 22–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harm, M. W., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1999). Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: insights from connectionist models. Psychological Review, 106(3), 491–528.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, P. J., Hulme, C., & Ellis, W. (1994). Ameliorating early reading failure by integrating the teaching of reading and phonological skills: the phonological linkage hypothesis. Child Development, 65, 41–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hatcher, J., Snowling, M. J., & Griffiths, Y. M. (2002). Cognitive assessment of dyslexic students in higher education. The British Journal of Educational Psychology, 72, 119–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heiervang, E., Stevenson, J., & Hugdahl, K. (2002). Auditory processing in children with dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(7), 931–938.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Helenius, P., Utela, K., & Hari, R. (1999). Auditory stream segregation in dyslexic adults. Brain, 122(5), 907–913.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jackson, N. E., & Butterfield, E. C. (1989). Reading-level-match designs: myths and realities. Journal of Reading Behavior, 21(4), 387–412.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D. (1997). Evidence that strategy effects in word naming reflect changes in output timing rather than changes in processing route. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 23(6), 1424–1438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnston, R. S., Rugg, M. D., & Scott, T. (1987). Phonological similarity effects, memory span and developmental reading disorders: the nature of the relationship. British Journal of Psychology, 78(2), 205–211.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Silvestri, R., Allingham, B. H., Parrila, R., & La Fave, C. B. (2008). Learning strategies and study approaches of postsecondary students with dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(1), 85–96.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lecocq, P. (1991). Apprentissage de la lecture et dyslexie. Bruxelles: Mardaga.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefavrais, P. (1967). Test de l'Alouette. Paris: E.C.P.A.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lefavrais, P. (2005). Test de l’Alouette Révisé. Paris: Editions du Centre de Psychologie Appliquée.

  • Lété, B., Sprenger-Charolles, L., & Colé, P. (2004). MANULEX: a grade-level lexical database from French elementary school readers. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 36(1), 156–166.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3(2), 129–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McDougall, S., Hulme, C., Ellis, A., & Monk, A. (1994). Learning to Read: The Role of Short-Term Memory and Phonological Skills. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 58(1), 112–133.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metsala, J. L., Stanovich, K. E., & Brown, G. D. A. (1998). Regularity effects and the phonological deficit model of reading disabilities: A meta-analytic review. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 279–293.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Misra, M., Katzir, T., Wolf, M., & Poldrack, R. A. (2004). Neural Systems for Rapid Automatized Naming in Skilled Readers: Unraveling the RAN-Reading Relationship. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(3), 241–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Bertelson, P., Cary, L., & Alegria, J. (1986). Literacy training and speech segmentation. Cognition, 24(1–2), 45–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J., & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7(4), 323–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R. K., Kliegel, R., Davidson, B. J., & Foltz, G. (1985). Individual differences and developmental differences in reading disability. In G. E. MacKinnon & T. G. Waller (Eds.), Reading research: Advances in theory and practice (pp. 1–64). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & McQuarrie, L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: longitudinal predictors of early reading development? Scientific Studies of Reading, 8(1), 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Paulesu, E., Démonet, J.-F., Fazio, F., McCrory, E., Chanoine, V., Brunswick, N., et al. (2001). Dyslexia: cultural diversity and biological unity. Science, 291, 2165–2167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peereman, R., & Content, A. (1998). Is perception a two-way street? The case of feedback consistency in visual word recognition. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 151–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Peereman, R., Lété, B., & Sprenger-Charolles, L. (2007). Manulex-infra: distributional characteristics of grapheme_phoneme mappings, and infralexical and lexical units in child-directed written material. Behavior Research Methods, 39(3), 579–589.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., Cardoso-Martins, C., Green, P. A., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Comparing the phonological and double deficit hypotheses for developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 14(7–8), 707–755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., van Orden, G. C., Smith, S. D., Green, P. A., & Haith, M. M. (1990). Phonological processing skills and deficits in adult dyslexics. Child Development, 61(6), 1753–1778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2007). Nested incremental modeling in the development of computational theories: The CDP + model of reading aloud. Psychological Review, 114(2), 273–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27(1), 28–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., et al. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126(4), 841–865.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., & Szenkovits, G. (2008). What phonological deficit? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61(1), 129–141.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ransby, M., & Swanson, H. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36(6), 538–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1956). Coloured Progressive Matrices. London: Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J., & Court, J. (1995). Standard Progressive Matrices Series A, B, C, D, E (revised 1956). Paris: Editions techniques et psychotechniques.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reid, A., Szczerbinsky, M., Iskierka-Kasperek, E., & Hansen, P. (2007). Cognitive profiles of adult developmental dyslexics: theoretical implications. Dyslexia, 13(1), 1–24.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., Waters, G. S., Barnes, M. A., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (1984). When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition? Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 23(3), 383–404.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94(2), 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55(2), 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1999). Phonological recoding and orthographic learning: A direct test of the self-teaching hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(2), 95–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the Anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134(4), 584–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidence for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. Psychological Science, 6(4), 250–254.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia. Dyslexia, 7(1), 37–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Goulandris, N., & Defty, N. (1996). A longitudinal study of reading development in dyslexic children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 653–669.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Nation, K., Moxham, P., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (1997). Phonological processing of dyslexic students in higher education: A preliminary report. Journal of Research in Reading, 20(1), 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Béchennec, D., & Kipffer-Piquard, A. (2005). French normative data on reading and related skills from EVALEC, a new computerized battery of tests (end Grade 1, Grade 2, Grade 3, and Grade 4). European Review of Applied Psychology/Revue Europeenne de Psychologie Appliquee, 55(3), 157–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Kipffer-Piquard, A., Pinton, F., & Billard, C. (2009). Reliability and prevalence of an atypical development of phonological skills in French-speaking dyslexics. Reading and Writing, 22(7), 811–842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., Lacert, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2000). On subtypes of developmental dyslexia: evidence from processing time and accuracy scores. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 54(2), 87–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Colé, P., & Serniclaes, W. (2006). Reading Acquisition and Developmental Dyslexia. Hove and New York: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., Béchennec, D., & Serniclaes, W. (2003). Development of phonological and orthographic processing in reading aloud, in silent reading, and in spelling: A four-year longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 84(3), 194–217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L. S., & Bonnet, P. (1998). Reading and Spelling Acquisition in French: The Role of Phonological Mediation and Orthographic Factors. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68(2), 134–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sprenger-Charolles, L., Siegel, L., Jimenez, J., & Ziegler, J. (accepted). Prevalence and Reliability of Phonological, Surface, and Mixed Profiles in Dyslexia: A Review of Studies Conducted in Languages Varying in Orthographic Depth. Scientific Studies of Reading.

  • Stanovich, K. E., & Siegel, L. (1994). Phenotypic performance profile of children with reading disabilities: a regression-based test of the phonological-core variable-difference model. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86(1), 24–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stoodley, C. J., Harrison, E. P., & Stein, J. F. (2006). Implicit motor learning deficits in dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 795–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Szenkovits, G., & Ramus, F. (2005). Exploring dyslexics' phonological deficit I: lexical vs. sub-lexical and input vs. output processes. Dyslexia, 11(4), 253–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Ijzendoorn, M. H., & Bus, A. G. (1994). Meta-analytic confirmation of the nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia. Reading Research Quarterly, 29(3), 266–275.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(1), 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). The development of reading-related phonological processing abilities: New evidence of bi-directional causality from a latent variable longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 30, 73–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., Torgesen, J. K., Rashotte, C. A., Hecht, S. A., Barker, T. A., Burgess, S. R., et al. (1997). Changing relations between phonological processing abilities and word-level reading as children develop from beginning to skilled readers: a 5-year longitudinal study. Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 468–479.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Waters, G. S., Seidenberg, M. S., & Bruck, M. (1984). Children's and adults' use of spelling-sound information in three reading tasks. Memory & Cognition, 12(3), 293–305.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1981). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale_Revised. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 50(2), 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of Developmental Dyslexia in a Regular Writing System. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14(1), 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1996). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from children learning to read German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61(1), 80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wise, B. W., Ring, J., & Olson, R. K. (1999). Training phonological awareness with and without explicit attention to articulation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 72(4), 271–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(3), 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33(4), 387–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., O'Rourke, A. G., Gidney, C., Lovett, M., Cirino, P., & Morris, R. (2002). The second deficit: An investigation of the independence of phonological and naming-speed deficits in developmental dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 15(1–2), 43–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zabell, C., & Everatt, J. (2002). Surface and phonological subtypes of adult developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia, 8(3), 160–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Castel, C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., Alario, F. X., & Perry, C. (2008). Developmental dyslexia and the dual route model of reading: Simulating individual differences and subtypes. Cognition, 107(1), 151–178.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., & Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: A psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychological Bulletin, 131(1), 3–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Pech-Georgel, C., George, F., & Lorenzi, C. (2009). Speech-perception-in-noise deficits in dyslexia. Developmental Science, 15(5), 732–745.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Korne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86(3), 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from the Agence Nationale de la Recherche (A.N.R.) for a project called “Les traitements morphologiques dans l’apprentissage de la lecture et ses troubles” (ANR-06-APPR-006). The authors would like to thank two anonymous reviewers for their very helpful comments on the manuscript.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Pascale Colé.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Martin, J., Colé, P., Leuwers, C. et al. Reading in French-speaking adults with dyslexia. Ann. of Dyslexia 60, 238–264 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0043-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-010-0043-8

Keywords

Navigation