Abstract
The study investigated the joint contribution of the self-regulated learning (SRL) and individual differences approaches to the prediction of university students’ grade point average (GPA) obtained at three separate time points throughout their degree (3 years). We assessed cognitive (i.e., previous academic performance, cognitive ability, and cognitive SRL strategies) and non-cognitive variables (i.e., personality, trait emotional intelligence, motivation, and non-cognitive SRL strategies) in a sample of Spanish undergraduates. Results showed that GPA correlated with previous academic performance (i.e., combination of high school’s GPA and college admission test score), academic self-efficacy, academic engagement, SRL strategies, and conscientiousness. Hierarchical regression analyses indicated that non-cognitive factors (i.e., academic engagement, academic self-efficacy, regulation of behavior and context, and conscientiousness) alone explained 17–25% of the variance in GPA across three years, and previous academic performance accounted up to an additional 25% of the variance, jointly reaching an explained variance of up to 50% in GPA. Specifically, academic engagement and regulation of behavior and context demonstrated incremental validity over and above cognitive predictors such as previous academic performance, inductive reasoning and regulation of cognition and metacognition. The role of intelligence, whether cognitive or emotional, was not as obvious as a predictor. Two nested structural equation models explained about 27–29% of the variance in a latent GPA factor exclusively from a proxy of a global variable of non-cognitive factors as a latent predictor, which is a novel and promising proof of its robust criterion validity. Implications and recommendations for future studies are discussed.
Azevedo (2020) highlighted that on the pursuit of a unified theory of metacognition and self-regulated learning (SRL), the study of individual differences must be incorporated, since “there is a need for more research on the role of motivational processes (e.g., self-efficacy and regulatory skills), emotional processes (e.g., emotion regulation skills), personality traits, working memory capacity, etc.” (Azevedo, 2020, p. 95). From a different but complementary perspective, Jensen (1989) highlighted that the historical separation of the study of learning and of individual differences is seen as an anomaly in the development of scientific psychology. In fact, those few studies integrating these two perspectives indicate that taking into account both the SRL and individual differences may provide a better understanding of students’ academic achievement than either one alone (De Feyter et al., 2012; Wolters & Hussain, 2015). In the same vein, De Raad & Schouwenburg (1996) emphasized the desirability of conducting integrated research including competing predictors of academic performance (AP) derived from different theoretical frameworks in a coherent system. Thomas et al., (2017) also advocated the need for a broader perspective in which multiple factors in higher education are integrated in concert, including different predictors from the affective domain.
Despite the prominent role of intelligence or general cognitive ability as a general predictor of AP (Galla et al., 2019; Kuncel & Hezlett, 2010; Richardson et al., 2012), some robust investigations indicate that greater intelligence does not imply higher AP. Hence, the positive association between intelligence and AP may not be linear nor exempt from several moderating factors (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009; Díaz-Morales & Escribano, 2013). As we have pointed out, some authors (Jensen, 1989; De Raad & Schouwenburg, 1996; Thomas et al., 2017; Wolters & Hussain, 2015) emphasize the importance of an integrative approach to predict AP, combining both cognitive and non-cognitive variables. In their key meta-analysis, Richardson et al., (2012) observed that, in addition to SRL, within the individual differences perspective, the so-called non-cognitive factors (Wechsler, 1943; Heckman & Kautz, 2012) such as personality traits such as conscientiousness (Poropat, 2009), emotional intelligence (EI; MacCann et al., 2020), and motivation are individual difference variables usually associated with university AP. In their book chapter, Pintrich & Zusho (2007) also provided a conceptual framework for student motivation and SRL, which consists of personal characteristics, classroom context, motivational processes, self-regulatory processes, and outcomes. Pintrich & Zusho (2007) highlighted the importance of personality differences in addition to the regulation of affect and motivation in the SRL process. Some studies have integrated, to some extent, these individual differences within the SRL framework. For example, Bidjerano & Dai (2007) found that SRL strategies mediated the effect of personality on academic achievement. However, to our knowledge, no study examined the specific role of trait EI within this framework. Thus, the relationship between the SRL framework and both conscientiousness and trait EI remains under-studied, and the present study aims to provide a unique contribution in this direction.
The first purpose of the present work is twofold: on the one hand, to provide a parsimonious but tentative framework with which to integrate the SRL perspective and the perspective of individual differences, and, on the other hand, to explore the predictive power of a selected combination of variables from each of these approaches on university AP as a partial test of the application of this theoretical framework to the prediction of university AP. The second purpose is to also focus on non-cognitive factors combining as a subset of less studied individual differences in the context of university AP, in particular conscientiousness and trait EI, together with SRL variables such as academic engagement and academic self-efficacy.
SRL in the prediction of AP
SRL is a core conceptual framework to understand the cognitive, motivational, and emotional aspects of learning (Panadero, 2017). According to the theory of SRL (Pintrich, 2000, 2004; Zimmerman, 1990), high levels of metacognition and learning-appropriate cognitions, motivations, and behaviors are predictive of learning and higher AP, possibly over and above intelligence in some cases (Minnaert & Janssen, 1998). In particular, a meta-analysis by Ohtani & Hisasaka (2018) revealed an average correlation of r = .20 between SRL (measured by the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire or MSLQ; Pintrich et al., 1991) and AP. More recently, Kickert et al., (2019) specifically underlined the role of motivational and self-regulatory factors and confirmed that their relations with university AP are not only strong, but relatively independent of two different assessment policies in a joint sample of psychology and education university students.
The first model of SRL developed by Zimmerman (the Triadic Analysis of SRL) integrated the interaction of three levels of SRL, namely the environment, the behavior and the person (Zimmerman, 1989). When referring to the “person”, Zimmerman focuses on individual strategies rather than the personal predispositions influencing their use. Other authors grounded in Zimmerman’s theory, such as Efklides (2011) and Kuhl (2000) are more similar to our approach in the sense that they explicitly recognize the prominent role of personal characteristics, including cognitive and non-cognitive traits, such as emotional self-control or trait rumination.
The second model by Zimmerman, the cyclical model of SRL (Zimmerman & Moylan, 2009), represents the interrelation of metacognition and motivational processes through three major phases, namely forththought, performance, and self-reflection. These three phases mirror those of Printich’s (2000): phase 1 (forethought, planning and activation), phase 2 (monitoring), phase 3 (control), and phase 4 (reaction and reflection). For the purpose of this study, we adopted Pintrich’s (2004) model because it explicitly acknowledges the influence of personal characteristics and individual differences in the SRL process.
Non-cognitive predictors of AP
While SRL strategies allow for the regulation of cognition, motivation, affect, behavior and context, a content examination of the gold standard measure of learning strategies (i.e., MSLQ) shows greater weight on the regulation of cognition and metacognition, as well as behavior and context. The present study contributes to the examination of the complementary role of non-cognitive factors in the regulation of motivation and affect.
The role of non-cognitive factors or soft skills is pervasive in cognition and metacognition processes (e.g., Heckman & Kautz 2012; Román-González et al., 2018). Research has indicated that non-cognitive factors may directly or indirectly predict university AP (e.g., Heckman & Kautz 2012; Putwain et al., 2019; Richardson et al., 2012; Robbins et al., 2004). The five-factor model of personality has been proposed as a comprehensive operationalisation of non-cognitive factors or soft skills by Borghans et al., (2008). This model is a taxonomy that consists of five categories of broad personality dimensions, also known as the “Big Five”, namely openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism (see Goldberg 1993 for a historical overview). Several studies have explored the relationship between the Big Five traits and AP, and conscientiousness has been the most consistent significant predictor (e.g., Hakimi et al., 2011; Sorić et al., 2017; Wagerman & Funder, 2007), showing predictive validity over and above cognitive ability (Poropat, 2009). Conscientiousness is closely related to motivation and drive (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003; Siegling & Petrides, 2016), given that is a propensity associated with persistence, self-discipline, and achievement motivation (Román-González et al., 2018). Openness to experience has also been shown to be relevant to AP (e.g., Komarraju et al., 2011), while extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism have shown less persistent and weaker links with AP (e.g., Poropat 2009).
Emotional intelligence (EI) is interpreted as part of personality in the meta-analysis by Richardson et al., (2012). Two predominant theoretical models of emotional intelligence (EI) in the literature are trait EI and ability EI (Pérez-González et al., 2020). Trait EI, or trait emotional self-efficacy, is conceptualized as a constellation of dispositions and self-perceptions assessed using questionnaires and rating scales (Petrides et al., 2007b), whereas ability EI is conceptualized as an emotion-related cognitive ability best assessed using maximum performance tests (Mayer & Salovey, 1997). Meta-analyses and other research studies have supported the theoretical distinction between trait and ability EI (Joseph & Newman, 2010; Martins et al., 2010), including a study by Qualter et al., (2012) that found that trait and ability EI differentially predicted AP in a longitudinal, school-based study, further corroborating their theoretical distinction. Using trait EI to predict AP might be advantageous since it taps into emotion-related constructs that are not well covered by existing personality trait taxonomies (Pérez-González & Sanchez-Ruiz, 2014). This is important since trait EI has already shown incremental validity over cognitive ability, personality traits, and self-concept in the prediction of AP (Ferrando et al., 2011; Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2013; Sanchez-Ruiz & El-Khoury, 2019), despite weak to moderate effect sizes (Petrides et al., 2018). This is corroborated by a meta-analysis by MacCann et al., (2020) which found trait EI to show incremental validity in the prediction of AP over and above intelligence and conscientiousness. However, the average correlation between trait EI and AP has not reached the thresholds of r = .15 and r = .20, according to Richardson et al.’s (2012) and MacCann et al.’s (2020) meta-analyses, respectively, so it does not yet appear to be an established predictor. For a review on the recent developments of trait EI research, see Petrides et al., (2016).
Academic self-efficacy is a robust predictor of AP as a motivation-related construct that consists of one’s beliefs in one’s ability to succeed academically. Academic self-efficacy has consistently shown to predict AP over and above other variables (Bandura et al., 1996; Lee et al., 2014; Richardson et al., 2012; Multon et al., 1991). For instance, Zuffianò et al., (2013) has shown self-efficacy to predict high school AP over and above previous AP, intelligence, personality traits, gender, socioeconomic status, and self-esteem.
Additionally, academic engagement is another promising non-cognitive predictor. It is a motivation-related construct that refers to a positive, persistent, and pervasive work-related state, characterized by vigor, dedication, and absorption (Schaufeli et al., 2002b). Several studies suggested a positive association between academic engagement and academic success in school and university (e.g., Schaufeli et al., 2002a). Although it has been a less studied construct in the SRL framework, academic engagement is in line with the taxonomy of self-regulation of learning areas (Pintrich, 2004), since academic engagement encompasses both the regulation of motivation (i.e., absorption and engagement) and affect (i.e., vigor).
A Novel and Eclectic Framework
The meta-analysis by Richardson et al., (2012) showed the need for a comprehensive theoretical and applied framework for the different psychological variables associated with AP. Nonetheless, this meta-analysis, despite it is regarded as a modern milestone (e.g., Ferrão & Almeida 2019), organized almost 40 psychological correlates of university AP in arbitrary and non-exclusive categories, which does not facilitate the construction of integrative models and the design of theory-driven empirical research. Pintrich (2004) described five general SRL areas: cognition, motivation, affect, behavior, and context. These areas encompass self-regulatory learning strategies that are conceptualized as potential mediators between personal characteristics (cognitive and non-cognitive) and performance (Pintrich, 2004). It should be noted that these personal characteristics might be individual differences in both cognitive and non-cognitive traits, which may play a role in the kind or functioning of the self-regulation strategies chosen by each student. For example, the executive function required for self-regulation is probably moderated by both cognition ability and personality traits such as conscientiousness. In short, these five SRL areas can be subsumed under the three areas of psychological functioning (Hilgard, 1980): cognition, motivation, and affect. Given that motivation and affect belong to the non-cognitive area, for the sake of parsimony, in this study we organized the self-regulatory learning strategies identified by Pintrich et al., (1991; Pintrich, 2004) and assessed by the MSLQ into the greater categories of cognitive and non-cognitive domains.
The cognitive domain consists of cognitive learning strategies (i.e., rehearsal, elaboration, organization, and critical thinking and metacognitive learning strategies). The non-cognitive domain covers the regulation of motivation, affect, behavior and context (MABC) described by Pintrich (2004), including time and study environment regulation, effort regulation, peer learning, and help-seeking. SRL strategies have been shown to display weak-to-moderate associations to AP, with the strongest associations being with effort regulation, time, study environment, and metacognitive self-regulation (Credé & Phillips, 2011).
Our integrative framework (see Table 1) summarizes these taxonomies and provides a tentative solution to the lack of a comprehensive theoretical framework of the potential predictors of AP. Following previous recommendations (Azevedo, 2020; Jensen, 1989; De Raad & Schouwenburg, 1996; Thomas et al., 2017), our proposed framework incorporates a set of the main predictors recently identified by Richardson et al., (2012) and organizes them bringing together two approaches: individual differences and SRL. Both these branches cross the five self-regulatory learning areas (Pintrich, 2004), the three traditional areas of psychological functioning (Hilgard, 1980), and the two broad domains of individual differences: intelligence and cognitive abilities as cognitive factors; and personality traits, motivation, and affect/mood as non-cognitive factors (Borghans et al., 2008; Chamorro-Premuzic, 2007; Cooper, 2010; Heckman & Rubinstein, 2001; Siegling & Petrides, 2016). While we recognize that there is no consensus on how to reconcile these different classifications of variables, Table 1 is intended to provide a parsimonious grouping consistent enough to serve as a starting point for future integrative models of SRL and individual differences approaches.
Research Aims and Hypotheses
Unfortunately, most research exploring psychological predictors of university AP focuses exclusively on one area of psychological functioning as well as on one approach, studying just a handful of predictors. Although theoretical efforts examining some permutations of predictors exist (e.g., Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2016; Chen et al., 2000), to our knowledge, no previous empirical work has met this long-standing target in university settings across a three-year long degree, which can provide comprehensive data on the long-term effects predictors.
Therefore, the first aim of this study is to explore the viability of our proposed integrative framework in explaining the combined influence of selected cognitive and non-cognitive factors, with special interest in testing the incremental validity of the latter in the prediction of AP over and above the traditional cognitive factors. Specifically, we put forth the following hypotheses:
H1a. Conscientiousness, academic engagement, academic self-efficacy and the MSLQ Factor 2, as the joint operationalization of the regulation of motivation, affect, behaviour and context (see Table 1), will predict AP throughout the 3-year period of undergraduate studies.
H1b. The previous non-cognitive factors will remain as significant predictors of AP in the presence of cognitive factors (i.e., cognitive ability, previous AP, cognitive SRL strategies measured by MSLQ Factor 1) throughout the 3-year period of undergraduate studies.
The second aim is to identify a reduced set of core non-cognitive factors with robust criterion validity. Following the preceding arguments and taking into account the non-cognitive areas for self-regulation of learning (regulation of motivation, affect, behavior, and context) identified by Pintrich (2004), and based on some of the most robust correlates of AP (i.e., those with an average r > .20) in the meta-analysis by Richardson et al., (2012), we expect to identify a valid structural equation model (SEM) and test the criterion validity of a global proxy of non-cognitive factors (i.e., a latent multifaceted non-cognitive factor) in the prediction of a latent university GPA factor. To this end, we set out our second hypothesis as follows:
H2. A latent global proxy of the non-cognitive factors (conscientiousness, academic engagement, academic self-efficacy and the MSLQ Factor 2) will significantly predict a latent factor of AP throughout three years of university degree.
Method
Participants
The convenience sample consisted of 386 Spanish undergraduate students (291 female) ranging in age between 17 and 46 years (M = 21.05; SD = 4.85). Participants were enrolled in a 3-year degree in Education at the University of Lleida, which is a public institution in Spain.
Measures
Academic performance (AP)
Official records were used as objective indicators of previous AP and the criterion variable, university AP. University students’ grade point average (GPA) score was used as a measure of university AP. It was obtained within three years of the degree program, at three separate time points, at the end of each academic year: GPA year 1, GPA year 2, GPA year 3. The interest in taking three different indicators of GPA rests on the assumption that the nature of the classes taken during a given year might demand different sets of non-cognitive skills than those demanded in another year, thus allowing for potential differences in the relationship of the predictors to the criterion to be observed.
Each year students took approximately 60 credits and an average of 10 different subjects per year. In order to make students’ average grades comparable with each other, the GPA score for each year was calculated for each student by weighting their average grade according to the ratio of enrolled credits over maximum credits for the year. The possible range of non-standardised GPA scores was from 0 to 3, but the actual range was from 0.12 to 2.88, with the average GPA for each year being progressively increasing from GPA first year = 1.45 (SD = 0.51) to GPA second year (t (228) = 6.52; p < .001), as well as from GPA second year = 1.58 (SD = 0.52) to GPA third year = 1.74 (SD = 0.54) (t (228) = 10.63; p < .001). Most of the students completed the degree, since only 8 students dropped out of the degree.
Concerning previous AP, it was a composite calculated with 60% being the cumulative GPA obtained in high school and 40% being the score on the Spanish university entry examination (knowledge-based test), called ‘Selectividad’ or "Prueba de Acceso ala Universidad"(i.e., University entrance exam). All achievement scores were previously standardized to be combined in the resulting prior AP scores.
Cognitive ability (Fluid Intelligence)
The Primary Mental Abilities test - Factor R ‘Reasoning’ (PMA-R; Thurstone & Thurstone 1962) is considered a good indicator of inductive reasoning, which entails the identification of new relationships in patterns by inferring the rules (e.g., Hertzog & Bleckley 2001). Inductive reasoning is a first-order factor in the hierarchical structure of cognitive ability located under Gf (fluid intelligence; Carroll 2014). The PMA-R involves a series of letters in a particular pattern and is asked to identify the next letter based on the pattern. The PMA-R was used as a proxy of Gf, in line with previous studies (e.g., Cejudo et al., 2017; Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009). In this study, this measure displayed good internal consistency (MacDonald’s total omega, ω = 0.85).
Personality
The Goldberg’s Bipolar Adjectives - Short Form (GBA-SF; Goldberg 1992; Spanish adaptation by García et al., 2004) is a 25-item questionnaire that assesses five personality dimensions corresponding to the Big Five personality traits (e.g., Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2011): extraversion, agreeableness, conscientiousness, emotional stability (low neuroticism), and openness. Each item consists of a pair of bipolar personality traits reflecting one dimension and is rated on a 9-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (very [pole A of the trait]) to 9 (very [pole B]). This questionnaire has shown strong correlations with the NEO Five Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI; García et al., 2004). In this study, subscale internal consistencies were acceptable to good (MacDonald’s total omega, for openness ω = 0.78, conscientiousness ω = 0.85, extraversion ω = 0.90, agreeableness ω = 0.71, and emotional stability ω = 0.84).
Trait EI
The Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue v. 1.50; Petrides 2009; Spanish adaptation by Pérez-González 2010) is a 153-item questionnaire used to assess global (total) trait EI as well as 15 facets of EI reflecting four factors: wellbeing, emotionality, sociability, and self-control. A 7-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). There is well-founded evidence on the criterion and incremental validity of the Spanish version of the TEIQue long form (Petrides et al., 2007a) and on the soundness of its psychometric properties (e.g., Pérez-González & Sanchez-Ruiz 2014). In this study, the internal consistency of the global score was satisfactory (MacDonald’s total omega, ω = 0.89). The validity evidence for the global score of trait EI is very sound (Andrei et al., 2016).
Academic self-efficacy
Academic self-efficacy was assessed via four items developed for this study. The items were based on an established measure, the Academic Self-Efficacy Scale (ASE; McIlroy, 2000) (i.e., “My performance is good in the majority of the courses”, “I study hard”, “I know I will obtain good grades during my academic career”, “I am able to organize my study time well according to the requirements of each subject”). A 5-point Likert scale was used ranging from 1 (totally disagree) to 5 (totally agree). The internal consistency of the composite score was acceptable (MacDonald’s total omega, ω = 0.70).
Academic Engagement
The Student Academic Engagement scale (SAE; Spanish version; Schaufeli et al., 2002b) is a 17-item questionnaire assessing three dimensions of academic engagement: vigor, dedication, and absorption. A 7-point Likert scale was used ranging from 0 (never) to 6 (always or every day). Internal consistencies were good in this study (MacDonald’s total omega for global, ω = 0.92, vigor = 0.84, dedication = 0.80, and absorption = 0.78).
Self-regulated learning strategies
The Learning Strategies subscale of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ; Pintrich et al., 1991; Spanish version; Roces et al., 1995) was used to assess students’ use of learning strategies (50 items) through a 7-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all true of me) to 7 (very true of me). This construct encompasses nine subscales: students’ cognitive learning strategies (i.e., rehearsal, elaboration, organization, and critical thinking), metacognitive learning strategies (i.e., ability to adaptively regulate one’s mental processes), and management of resources available for studying (i.e., time and study environment regulation, effort regulation, peer learning, and help-seeking). The partial use of this instrument is common in the literature (e.g., Kickert et al., 2019; Suárez Riveiro et al., 2001).
For the purposes of the set of hierarchical regressions, which required the extraction of a smaller number of factors for analysis, an exploratory factor analysis (EFA) was conducted on subscales (Bernstein & Teng, 1989). Likewise, despite the MSLQ is a well-known instrument, there is unsatisfactory evidence for the hypothesized structures given the scarcity of measurement work that examines the entire latent factor structure of the instrument has been highlighted as an important gap in the MSLQ literature (e.g., Cho & Summers 2012; Hilpert et al., 2013). Using principal axis extraction and oblimin/promax rotations from the nine subscales, Horn’s (1965) parallel analysis indicated a two-factor solution. The two factors explained 51% of the variance, tapping into ‘Regulation of cognition and metacognition’ (Factor 1) and ‘Regulation of behavior and context’ (Factor 2). Factor 1 showed good internal consistency (MacDonald’s total omega, ω = 0.84) and included elaboration, organization, and critical thinking and the metacognitive self-regulation subscales. Factor 1 may indicate the ‘Deep’ student approach to learning (SAL) according to Pintrich (2004). Factor 2 was also reliable (MacDonald’s total omega, ω = 0.72) and included the rehearsal subscale (related to ‘Surface’ SAL; Pintrich 2004) and the time and study environment and effort regulation subscales. Help-seeking and peer-learning subscales did not significantly load on any factor, so they were excluded in further analyses.
Procedure
The study was approved by the university’s institutional review board. The questionnaires were administered as hard copies by the researchers and trained university staff at the beginning of the participants’ degree in a classroom setting. The length of the testing session ranged between 50 and 75 min. Participants had a 10-minute break after 45 min. Students’ participation was voluntary and was not part of an assignment. Participants received no incentives for their participation.
Statistical analyses
Correlations and hierarchical regressions
Intercorrelations between all the study variables were conducted. Three two-step hierarchical regressions were run, one for each university year with its respective GPA score as the outcome and cognitive and non-cognitive variables as predictors, at step 1 and 2, respectively.
Structural equation models (SEMs)
In order to find a sample size sufficient to minimize Type II error, a power analysis was performed using WebPower (Zhang & Yuan, 2018; https://webpower.psychstat.org/wiki/) following the recommendations by MacCallum et al., (1996). The parameters established for the calculation of the appropriate sample size were: 320 degrees of freedom, taking a RMSEA of 0.06 over the Null Hypothesized RMSEA of 0.05, a significance level at 0.05, and a power goal of 0.80. The resulting required sample size was 300 participants. Therefore, the final power for our study with 386 students exceeded the expectations (0.90).
Two SEMs were tested combining measures representing the shared variance among each of the areas of regulation of learning (Pintrich, 2004; see Table 1). Concerning the criterion variable, a latent university GPA variable was constituted from the shared variance among the manifest GPA year 1, GPA year 2 and GPA year 3.
Results
Correlations
Bivariate correlations among the key study variables are presented in Table 2. Bonferroni correction was applied to reduce Type I error rate. After applying Bonferroni correction, only p values less than 0,00022 were considered significant (depicted in bold in Table 2). First year university GPA showed the strongest correlation with previous AP (r = .51, p < .00022), followed by academic self-efficacy (r = .42, p < .00022), academic engagement (r = .38, p < .00022), MSLQ’s factor 2 (r = .37, p < .00022), MSLQ’s factor 1 (r = .32, p < .00022), and conscientiousness (r = .30, p < .00022).
Second year university GPA showed the strongest correlation with previous AP (r = .63, p < .00022), followed by academic self-efficacy (r = .48, p < .00022), MSLQ’s factor 2 (r = .41, p < .00022), academic engagement (r = .41, p < .00022) and MSLQ’s factor 1 (r = .41, p < .00022), and conscientiousness (r = .28, p < .00022).
Third year university GPA had the strongest correlation with previous AP (r = .63, p < .00022), followed by academic self-efficacy (r = .43, p < .00022), academic engagement (r = .40, p < .00022), MSLQ’s factor 2 (r = .38, p < .00022), MSLQ’s factor 1 (r = .32, p < .00022), and conscientiousness (r = .25, p < .00022). In addition, the GPA scores for year 1st, 2nd and 3rd were highly intercorrelated (r = .80-0.95, p < .00022).
Hierarchical regressions
No multicollinearity problems were observed since tolerance values were > 0.20 and VIF was < 4.00 in all steps. The subject-to-predictor ratio is over 10:1. Three separate analyses were carried out with GPA as the criterion for each of the three academic years (see Table 3). We have followed two steps, first introducing the short set of selected non-cognitive predictors. In the second step we added the selected cognitive predictors, in order to see which variables from the first step still showed predictive validity. After applying Bonferroni correction, only p values less than 0,012 were considered significant in Step 1, and only p values less than 0,007 were considered significant in Step 2.
Using first year GPA as the criterion variable, at step 1, the three non-cognitive variables were significant predictors (F(4, 184) = 16.94, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.25): academic engagement (β = 0.21, p < .01), academic self-efficacy (β = 0.21, p < .05) and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) (β = 0.22, p < .05). In the second step (F(7, 181) = 23.54, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.46), the only significant cognitive predictor was previous AP (β = 0.52, p < .001), while two non-cognitive predictors were significant: academic engagement (β = 0.20, p < .01) and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) (β = 0.21, p < .01). The Δ R2 was significant from step 1 to 2.
Using second year GPA as the criterion variable, at step 1, two out of the three non-cognitive variables were significant predictors (F(4, 179) = 14.15, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.22): academic self-efficacy (β = 0.27, p < .01) and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2; β = 0.23, p < .01). However, the p values did not reach significance after Bonferroni correction. In the second step (F(7, 176) = 26.69, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.50), the only significant cognitive predictor was previous AP (β = 0.60, p < .001), while only significant non-cognitive predictor was regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) (β = 0.21, p < .01). The Δ R2 was significant from step 1 to 2.
Using third year GPA as the criterion variable, at step 1, the three non-cognitive variables were significant predictors (F(4, 175) = 10.19, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.17): academic engagement (β = 0.17, p < .05), academic self-efficacy (β = 0.20, p < .05) and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) (β = 0.19, p < .05). In the second step (F(7, 172) = 21.17, p < .001, adjusted R2 = 0.44), the only significant cognitive predictor was previous AP (β = 0.59, p < .001), while two non-cognitive predictors were significant: academic engagement (β = 0.16, p < .05) and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) (β = 0.16, p < .05). The Δ R2 was significant from step 1 to 2.
Structural equation models (SEMs)
A structural equation model was set up in AMOS, whereby a latent predictor factor was modeled as an exogenous variable and university GPA across three years as an endogenous variable. We explored statistics of skewness and kurtosis as indicators of normality. The absolute values of the minimum and maximum skewness and kurtosis were 0.06 and 0.79, which were well within the limits of past research recommendations: skewness < 2, kurtosis < 7 (Curran et al., 1996). Since data were relatively normally distributed, we tested two SEM models with maximum likelihood estimation (see Figs. 1 and 2 for a depiction of the models, respectively; see Table 4 for a summary of fit results for the SEMs).
The first model is a theory-driven proposed model. In the first model, a comprehensive latent variable of non-cognitive predictor reflecting regulation of Motivation, Affect, Behavior, and Context (MABC) was operationalized via next manifest variables: academic engagement, academic self-efficacy, conscientiousness, and MSLQ’s factor 2. This latent variable was regressed onto the same latent GPA factor representing the shared variance among the manifest GPAs from the first, second, and third year of the degree. Figure 1 depicts the resultant standardized estimates. This Model 1 (or MABC model) resulted in good fit to the data according to cut off criteria by Hooper et al., (2008), except for the parsimony index, χ2(14) = 39.555, p < .001, TLI = 0.950, RMSEA = 0.069, CFI = 0.975, PCFI = 0.488. The TLI, an index that prefers simpler models (Hooper et al., 2008), was acceptable but suggested room for improvement.
The second model is partially a data-driven model, based on the above model, so that it is a modified model 1. This second model was conducted in order to explore the possibility for improvement of model 1 in terms of both TLI and parsimony (PCFI). In this second model, the regulation of Behavior and Context was omitted, so that a narrower latent variable of non-cognitive predictor reflecting regulation of Motivation and Affect (MA) was operationalized via next manifest variables: academic engagement, academic self-efficacy, and conscientiousness. This latent variable was regressed onto the same latent GPA factor representing the shared variance among the manifest GPAs from the first, second, and third year of the degree. Figure 2 depicts the resultant standardized estimates. This Model 2 (or MA model) resulted in good fit to the data, except for the parsimony index, χ2(9) = 25.047, p = .003, TLI = 0.956, RMSEA = 0.068, CFI = 0.981, PCFI = 0.421. Except for parsimony (PCFI), all the indices improved slightly with this model with respect to Model 1.
In sum, Model 1 showed greater explanatory power (29% of the variance) vs. Model 2 (27% of the variance), as well as slightly higher level of parsimony (Model 1’s PCFI = 0.49 vs. Model 2’s PCFI = 0.42).
Discussion
The present exploratory study aimed to identify core non-cognitive predictors of AP and their differential significance as a first step to build an integrative model to predict AP in higher education.
Results confirmed previous AP to be the strongest predictor of university GPA, supporting the premise of this study in line with previous research (Richardson et al., 2012). Surprisingly, contrary to most of the previous investigations into individual differences, cognitive ability displayed no significant association with AP in any of the analyses. Nevertheless, non-significant or weak correlations have been previously reported (e.g., Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009; Mavroveli & Sanchez-Ruiz, 2011) between intelligence and AP. Specifically, also using a sample of Spanish undergraduate students, Chamorro-Premuzic et al., (2009) found a non-significant correlation between fluid intelligence and previous AP in terms of university entry examinations, which is a quite similar result to the correlation we found.
Regarding non-cognitive factors, academic self-efficacy, academic engagement, conscientiousness, and non-cognitive SRL strategies were strongly associated with AP. Our results indicate that it is possible to explain a quarter of the variance in university GPA from a reduced set of non-cognitive variables alone, in accordance with the conclusions of Heckman and colleagues (e.g., Heckman & Kautz 2012).
Through hierarchical regressions, up to about 25% of the variance in university GPA scores can be accounted for only by non-cognitive factors such as academic engagement (a proxy of regulation of motivation and affect) and strategies for the regulation of behavior and context, thus showing support for H1a. As expected in H1b, with the addition of previous AP as cognitive predictor, up to about 50% of the variance in university GPA was explained. Conscientiousness was confirmed as a distal robust correlate of GPA scores, but in the hierarchical regressions it loses predictive capacity in the presence of other proximal motivational constructs such as academic engagement, academic self-efficacy and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ factor 2). However, although there was empirical overlap between conscientiousness and these other three variables, the overlap was not problematic, therefore the variables can still capture the individual differences in optimal personal qualities (Pintrich & Zusho, 2007) for the efficient regulation of motivation in learning.
These findings have important implications on the significance of personality traits such as conscientiousness in relation to academic achievement. According to the findings of the present study, those students who believe in their capabilities, are making deliberate efforts in their academic endeavors, are showing responsibility, and are using SRL strategies were more academically successful. To go further, the findings of the present study can imbue professionals in education who are able to implement strategies that foster the development of behaviors associated with these non-cognitive factors, focusing on counseling and training for the improvement of the regulation of motivation, affect, behavior and context. Additionally, these findings can inform interventions that target students who are facing academic difficulties (McKenzie & Schweitzer, 2001).
Additionally, the SEMs were carried out with statistical power guarantees and represented a good fit to the data. The SEMs explained about 30% of the variance in university GPA with only a reduced set of 3–4 non-cognitive factors as combined predictors, thus supporting the robust criterion validity of non-cognitive factors or soft skills, as advocated by Heckman & Kautz (2012). Focusing on fit results, RMSEA, TLI and CFI slightly preferred Model 2 while PCFI preferred Model 1 (although PCFI still indicates acceptable but not good parsimony in both cases). Nonetheless, considering that Model 1 is the theory-based model derived from the literature (H2), and both power of variance explained, and level of parsimony are higher, it seems pertinent to recommend that Model 1 be subjected to replication studies in the future. Thus, H2 was fully supported by the results obtained through SEM.
In our theory-based proposed model 1, non-cognitive factors were operationalised as a latent factor capturing the variance shared between academic engagement, academic self-efficacy, regulation of behavior and context, and conscientiousness. Both hierarchical regression and SEMs findings have demonstrated the remarkable predictive validity of these four non-cognitive factors. Furthermore, it is worth noting that step 2 in the hierarchical regressions has revealed the unique incremental validity of academic engagement and regulation of behavior and context (MSLQ Factor 2) over and above cognitive variables such as previous AP, inductive reasoning and regulation of cognition and metacognition, which constitutes hard evidence in favor of soft skills, thus extending the work by James Heckman and colleagues (Borghans et al., 2001; Heckman & Kautz 2012).
These novel findings support the role of non-cognitive factors in university AP, as well as our theoretical integrative model of psychological predictors as a framework for the design of new empirical studies. The pattern of results suggested the possible presence of a mediator effect in the relation between conscientiousness and AP. This is in line with the widespread assumption that distal predictors affect AP via proximal processes (Chen et al., 2000; Conrad & Patry, 2012). To some extent, motivation can be conceptualized in terms of personality traits such as conscientiousness, which is closely related to effort regulation (Chamorro-Premuzic & Furnham, 2003) and has been referred to as the “will to achieve” (Digman, 1989). Hence, the role of conscientiousness as a predictor of AP is defined by a propensity to effortful and ambitious achievement-striving (Chamorro-Premuzic et al., 2009), which may be better captured through self-reports of proximal and situational variables (e.g., academic self-efficacy). Perhaps for this reason the significant effect of conscientiousness as a predictor of AP disappeared in the presence of these self-reports in our hierarchical regressions. However, this is rather speculative and remains to be explored by further studies.
Contrary to previous findings (e.g., MacCann et al., 2020; Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2013) trait EI showed no significant association to AP in either of the analyses, although it significantly correlated with academic engagement, SRL strategies, and academic self-efficacy. It should be considered that many studies, including the meta-analyses by MacCann et al., (2020) and Perera & DiGiacomo (2013), reported weak to moderate correlations between trait EI and AP (Petrides et al., 2018; Perera & DiGiacomo, 2013) and also reported moderating effects of academic level and age, whereby the relationship between trait EI and AP was weaker at the university level compared to primary school. Nevertheless, the findings of the present study may be also explained by the indirect effects of trait EI on AP. Specifically, emotional self-efficacy, an important aspect of trait EI, is likely to influence many factors that impact AP (Petrides et al., 2018; Qualter et al., 2012). For instance, emotional self-efficacy has been shown to positively impact academic self-efficacy, leading to enhanced AP (Adeyemo, 2007; Hen & Goroshit, 2014). Additionally, emotional self-efficacy may influence the self-regulatory strategies chosen for various educational activities and one’s emotional response to academic stressors (Bandura, 1999; Bandura, 2001; Qualter et al., 2012). The latter is due to how one’s beliefs regarding his or her emotional response in past academic settings can influence the responses in the future (Parsons & Ruble, 1977; Qualter et al., 2012).
Regulation of behavior and context (factor 2 of MSLQ) explained additional variance in AP over and above cognitive variables like previous AP. However, the rehearsal, time and study management, and effort regulation subscales were previously found to have small to moderate correlations with university GPA (Richardson et al., 2012). In addition, academic self-efficacy had the highest correlation with AP following previous AP, supporting previous research (e.g., Lee et al., 2014). Furthermore, this may give insight into research pointing to the role of self-efficacy in academic motivation (e.g., Schunk 1991) and self-motivated academic goal setting (Zimmerman et al., 1992). This may also suggest a pathway of influence where previous AP enhances academic self-efficacy which, in turn, influences AP, in line with Bandura’s (1977) suggestion. Our study has focused on quantifying the joint predictive power of individual dispositional differences along with SRL on AP. As we have noted earlier, cognitive and non-cognitive factors, such as conscientiousness and academic engagement, may influence the SRL cycle in both Printich’s and Zimmerman’s model. Future studies could build on our findings by investigating the differential impact of such factors in each of the phases of the SRL process.
Concerning the SEMs, where the predictor variable was conceived as a latent factor composed by the shared variance of several non-cognitive factors, both Model 1 and Model 2 showed good fit to the data as well as very similar size effects, but the Model 1 was somewhat more parsimonious, as well as more comprehensive and slightly higher in predictive validity. This is the first time, to our knowledge, that a proxy of a general variable of non-cognitive factors is generated through structural equation modeling and its criterion validity on university GPA is successfully tested, extending the work by Heckman and colleagues with a sample coming from a non-English speaking country (i.e., Spain) and a specific major (i.e., Teachers Education College).
Our contribution is threefold: first, for having integrated multiple predictors from the SRL perspective and from the perspective of individual differences, filling the gap noticed by Azevedo (2020), Jensen (1989) or De Raad & Schouwenburg (1996); second, for having verified, in a non-Anglo-Saxon country, the combined and separate predictive power of the main variables identified in the meta-analysis by Richardson et al., (2012); and, third, for having demonstrated for the first time that a general latent factor of non-cognitive factors formed from variables from the two research traditions can evidence criterion validity on the university GPA recorded over three subsequent years, as suggested by econometric analyses by the Nobel laureate James Heckman.
Limitations and future directions
Strengths of the present study include its comprehensive approach integrating cognitive and non-cognitive aspects to predict AP across years of study, which significantly contributes to the scarce literature with the same purpose (e.g., Richardson et al., 2012). Nevertheless, this study has several limitations. First, concerning the method, although the sample is overwhelmingly female, this is representative of the Spanish education sector whereby around 70% of education professionals (Instituto Nacional de Estadística, 2020) and 77.5% of undergraduate students in education degrees are female (Corporación de Radio y Televisión Española, 2019). Second, the length of the data collection session may be considered a limitation as it may have fatigued the participants and so biased the results, although, as explained above, participants were given the opportunity to take a 10-minute break after 45 min. Third, although research in this area has consistently used GPA to indicate AP (Richardson et al., 2012) since GPA has been found to be internally consistent (Bacon & Bean, 2006) and reliable over time (Schuler et al., 1990), future research should attempt to adjust for potential statistical artifacts such as grade inflation (e.g., Johnson 2006) that can undermine the strength of observed correlations with GPA (Poropat, 2009). Fourth, the non-significant findings of cognitive ability obtained in this study might be due to (a) the narrow assessment of the construct (Reeve et al., 2014), (b) the range restriction in intelligence scores among university students, and (c) the sample consisting of students pursuing the same degree (Sanchez-Ruiz et al., 2013). Thus, future research may benefit from recruiting a more diverse sample and adopting a multilevel analysis approach to predicting AP. Finally, even though we used an objective measure of AP as the outcome (official GPA records) and a maximum-performance test to measure cognitive ability (i.e., fluid intelligence via the PMA-R), the rest of predictors were assessed using self-report measures, which allows for the possibility of mono-method bias.
Additionally, the academic challenges and demands in high school and college may differ and this may be associated with changes in AP (Credé & Kuncel, 2008). However, in spite of this, the main non-cognitive predictors of AP in this study were relevant to both high school and college GPAs. In addition, even though our study measured a wide set of non-cognitive variables, additional ones may be worth exploring, such as, students’ approaches to learning, study habits, study attitudes, and psychosocial contextual variables (e.g., academic majors). In the same vein, future research expanding on the role of moderators and mediators in the trait EI and AP relationship is required.
Finally, this study examined MSLQ subscales as static predictors of AP but not the context specificity embedded in these measures. Future research should consider the context in which these learning strategies are applied and measured and examine within-person variations across different majors (Credé & Phillips, 2011). Moreover, future studies can benefit from exploring trait EI in terms of separate factor scores (i.e., wellbeing, emotionality, sociability, and self-control) instead of one global score.
Conclusions
Our findings confirm the significance of both SRL and non-cognitive factors in predicting university GPA (e.g., Heckman & Kautz 2012; Mega et al., 2014; Richardson et al., 2012), as well as extends the empirical literature integrating the SRL approach and the individual differences approach as previously demanded by experienced researchers such as Azevedo (2020), Jensen (1989), and De Raad & Schouwenburg (1996), among others.
Our proposed theoretical framework summarized in Table 1 as well as our Structural Model 1 depicted in Fig. 1 can be a useful road map for counseling and assessing students in education, targeting interventions for students at risk of academic difficulties, and designing future research investigating how different cognitive and non-cognitive personal characteristics contribute to (moderating or mediating) each phase and subprocess of the SRL cycle.
Data Availability
Dataset is available for research purposes from the first author.
Code Availability
Not applicable.
References
Adeyemo, D. A. (2007). Moderating influence of emotional intelligence on the link between academic self-efficacy and achievement of university students. Psychology and Developing Societies, 19(2), 199–213. https://doi.org/10.1177/097133360701900204
Andrei, F., Siegling, A. B., Aloe, A. M., Baldaro, B., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). The incremental validity of the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaire (TEIQue): A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Personality Assessment, 98(3), 261–276. https://doi.org/10.1080/00223891.2015.1084630
Azevedo, R. (2020). Reflections on the field of metacognition: issues, challenges, and opportunities. Metacognition and Learning, 15(2), 91–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-020-09231-x
Bacon, D. R., & Bean, B. (2006). GPA in research studies: An invaluable but neglected opportunity. Journal of Marketing Education, 28(1), 35–42. https://doi.org/10.1177/0273475305284638
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191–215. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.84.2.191
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Multifaceted impact of self-efficacy beliefs on academic functioning. Child Development, 67(3), 1206–1222. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01791.x
Bandura, A. (1999). A social cognitive theory of personality. In L. Pervin, & O. John (Eds.), Handbook of Personality (2nd ed., pp. 154–196). Guilford Publications
Bandura, A. (2001). Social cognitive theory: An agentic perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 52(1), 1–26. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.52.1.1
Bernstein, I. H., & Teng, G. (1989). Factoring items and factoring scales are different: Spurious evidence for multidimensionality due to item categorization. Psychological Bulletin, 105(3), 467–477. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.105.3.467
Bidjerano, T., & Dai, D. Y. (2007). The relationship between the big-five model of personality and self-regulated learning strategies. Learning and Individual Differences, 17(1), 69–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2007.02.001
Borghans, L., Duckworth, A. L., Heckman, J. J., & Ter Weel, B. (2008). The economics and psychology of personality traits. Journal of Human Resources, 43(4), 972–1059. https://doi.org/10.3368/jhr.43.4.972
Carroll, J. B. (2014). Human cognitive abilities: A critique. In J. J. McArdle, & R. W. Woodcock (Eds.), Human cognitive abilities in theory and practice (pp. 21–40). Psychology Press
Cejudo, J., Losada, L., & Pérez-González, J. C. (2017). Multiple intelligences and their relationships with cognitive and emotional intelligences in adolescents. Universitas Psychologica, 16(3), 78–90. https://doi.org/10.11144/javeriana.upsy16-3.imri
Chamorro-Premuzic, T. (2007). Personality and Individual Differences. Blackwell Publishing
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., & Furnham, A. (2003). Personality predicts academic performance: Evidence from two longitudinal university samples. Journal of Research in Personality, 37(4), 319–338. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00578-0
Chamorro-Premuzic, T., Quiroga, M. A., & Colom, R. (2009). Intellectual competence and academic performance: A Spanish study. Learning and Individual Differences, 19(4), 486–491. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2009.05.002
Chen, G., Gully, S. M., Whiteman, J. A., & Kilcullen, R. N. (2000). Examination of relationships among trait-like individual differences, state-like individual differences, and learning performance. Journal of Applied Psychology, 85(6), 835–847. https://doi.org/10.1037/0021-9010.85.6.835
Cho, M. H., & Summers, J. (2012). Factor validity of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ) in asynchronous online learning environments. Journal of Interactive Learning Research, 23(1), 5–28. https://www.learntechlib.org/primary/p/34129/
Conrad, N., & Patry, M. W. (2012). Conscientiousness and academic performance: a mediational analysis. International Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 6(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.20429/ijsotl.2012.060108
Cooper, C. (2010). Individual Differences and Personality. Hodder Education. https://doi.org/10.4324/9780203785218. 3rd ed.
Corporación de Radio y Televisión Española (2019, January 25). Igualdad. La paridad en la Universidad: casi todos en Informática son hombres y en Educación dominan las mujeres. https://www.rtve.es/noticias/20190125/paridad-universidad-casi-todos-informatica-son-hombres-educacion-dominan-mujeres/1874180.shtml
Credé, M., & Kuncel, N. (2008). Study habits, skills, and attitudes: The third pillar supporting collegiate academic performance. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(6), 425–453. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00089.x
Credé, M., & Phillips, L. A. (2011). A meta-analytic review of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire. Learning and Individual Differences, 21(4), 337–346. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.03.002
Curran, P. J., West, S. G., & Finch, J. F. (1996). The robustness of test statistics to nonnormality and specification error in confirmatory factor analysis. Psychological Methods, 1(1), 16–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.1.16
De Feyter, T., Caers, R., Vigna, C., & Berings, D. (2012). Unraveling the impact of the Big Five personality traits on academic performance: The moderating and mediating effects of self-efficacy and academic motivation. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(4), 439–448. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.03.013
De Raad, B., & Schouwenburg, H. C. (1996). Personality traits in learning and education. European Journal of Personality, 10, 185–200. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0984(199609)10:3<185::AID-PER256>3.0.CO;2-M
Díaz-Morales, J., & Escribano, C. (2013). Predicting school achievement: The role of inductive reasoning, sleep length and morningness–eveningness. Personality and Individual Differences, 55(2), 106–111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2013.02.011
Digman, J. M. (1989). Five robust trait dimensions: Development, stability, and utility. Journal of Personality, 57(2), 195–214. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.1989.tb00480.x
Efklides, A. (2011). Interactions of metacognition with motivation and affect in self-regulated learning: The MASRL model. Educational Psychologist, 46(1), 6–25. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.2011.538645
Ferrão, M., & Almeida, L. (2019). Differential effect of university entrance score on first-year students’ academic performance in Portugal. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 44(4), 610–622. https://doi.org/10.1080/02602938.2018.1525602
Ferrando, M., Prieto, M. D., Almeida, L. S., Ferrándiz, C., Bermejo, R., López-Pina, J. A. … Fernández, M. C. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence and academic performance: controlling for the effects of IQ, personality, and self-concept. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 29(2), 150–159. https://doi.org/10.1177/0734282910374707
Galla, B. M., Shulman, E. P., Plummer, B. D., Gardner, M., Hutt, S. J., Goyer, J. P. … Duckworth, A. L. (2019). Why high school grades are better predictors of on-time college graduation than are admissions test scores: the roles of self-regulation and cognitive ability. American Educational Research Journal, 56(6), 2077–2115. https://doi.org/10.3102/0002831219843292
Garcia, O., Aluja, A., & Garcia, L. F. (2004). Psychometric properties of Goldberg’s 50 personality markers for the Big Five Model. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 20(4), 310–319. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.20.4.310
Goldberg, L. R. (1992). The development of markers for the Big-Five factor structure. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 26–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.26
Goldberg, L. R. (1993). The structure of phenotypic personality traits. American Psychologist, 48(1), 26–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.1.26
Hakimi, S., Hejazi, E., & Lavasani, M. G. (2011). The relationships between personality traits and students’ academic achievement. Procedia-Social and Behavioral Sciences, 29, 836–845. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2011.11.312
Heckman, J. J., & Kautz, T. (2012). Hard evidence on soft skills. Labour Economics, 19(4), 451–464. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.labeco.2012.05.014
Heckman, J. J., & Rubinstein, Y. (2001). The importance of noncognitive skills: Lessons from the GED testing program. American Economic Review, 91(2), 145–149. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.91.2.145
Hen, M., & Goroshit, M. (2014). Academic self-efficacy, emotional intelligence, GPA and academic procrastination in higher education. Eurasian Journal of Social Sciences, 2(1), 1–10
Hertzog, C., & Bleckley, M. K. (2001). Age differences in the structure of intelligence: Influences of information processing speed. Intelligence, 29(3), 191–217. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0160-2896(00)00050-7
Hilgard, E. R. (1980). The trilogy of mind: Cognition, affection, and conation. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 16(2), 107–117. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6696(198004)16:2<107::AID-JHBS2300160202>3.0.CO;2-Y
Hilpert, J. C., Stempien, J., van der Hoeven Kraft, K. J., & Husman, J. (2013). Evidence for the latent factor structure of the MSLQ: A new conceptualization of an established questionnaire. SAGE Open, 3(4), 2158244013510305. https://doi.org/10.1177/2158244013510305
Hooper, D., Coughlan, J., & Mullen, M. R. (2008). Structural Equation Modelling: Guidelines for Determining Model Fit. Electronic Journal of Business Research Methods, 6(1), 53–60
Horn, J. L. (1965). A rationale and test for the number of factors in factor analysis. Psychometrika, 30(2), 179–185. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02289447
Instituto Nacional de Estadística (2020). Educación: 3.10 Mujeres en el profesorado por enseñanza que imparten. https://www.ine.es/uc/k3Ow4Bzu
Jensen, A. R. (1989). The relationship between learning and intelligence. Learning and Individual Differences, 1(1), 37–62. https://doi.org/10.1016/1041-6080(89)90009-5
Johnson, V. E. (2006). Grade inflation: A crisis in college education. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/b97309
Joseph, D. L., & Newman, D. A. (2010). Emotional intelligence: an integrative meta-analysis and cascading model. Journal of Applied Psychology, 95(1), 54–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017286
Kickert, R., Meeuwisse, M., Stegers-Jager, M., Koppenol-Gonzalez, K. V., Arends, G. R., L., & Prinzie, P. (2019). Assessment policies and academic performance within a single course: the role of motivation and self-regulation. Assessment & Evaluation in Higher Education, 44(8), 1177–1190. https://doi.org/10.1080/02602938.2019.1580674
Komarraju, M., Karau, S. J., Schmeck, R. R., & Avdic, A. (2011). The Big Five personality traits, learning styles, and academic achievement. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 472–477. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2011.04.019
Kuncell, N. R., & Hezlett, S. A. (2010). Fact and fiction in cognitive ability testing for admissions and hiring decisions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19(6), 339–345. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721410389459
Kuhl, J. (2000). A functional-design approach to motivation and self-regulation: The dynamics of personality systems and interactions. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 111–169). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50034-2
Lee, W., Lee, M. J., & Bong, M. (2014). Testing interest and self-efficacy as predictors of academic self-regulation and achievement. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(2), 86–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.02.002
MacCallum, R. C., Browne, M. W., & Sugawara, H. M. (1996). Power analysis and determination of sample size for covariance structure modeling. Psychological Methods, 1(2), 130. https://doi.org/10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.130
MacCann, C., Jiang, Y., Brown, L. E., Double, K. S., Bucich, M., & Minbashian, A. (2020). Emotional intelligence predicts academic performance: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(2), 150–186. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000219
Martins, A., Ramalho, N., & Morin, E. (2010). A comprehensive meta-analysis of the relationship between emotional intelligence and health. Personality and Individual Differences, 49(6), 554–564. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2010.05.029
Mayer, J. D., & Salovey, P. (1997). What is emotional intelligence?. In P. Salovey, & D. Sluyter (Eds.), Emotional development and emotional intelligence: Implications for educators (pp. 3–31). Basic Books
Mavroveli, S., & Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J. (2011). Trait emotional intelligence influences on academic achievement and school behavior. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(1), 112–134. https://doi.org/10.1348/2044-8279.002009
McIlroy, D., Bunting, B., & Adamson, G. (2000). An evaluation of the factor structure and predictive utility of a test anxiety scale with reference to students’ past performance and personality indices. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 70(1), 17–32. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709900157949
McKenzie, K., & Schweitzer, R. (2001). Who succeeds at university? Factors predicting academic performance in first year Australian university students. Higher Education Research & Development, 20(1), 21–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/07924360120043621
Mega, C., Ronconi, L., & De Beni, R. (2014). What makes a good student? How emotions, self-regulated learning, and motivation contribute to academic achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106(1), 121–131. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0033546
Minnaert, A., & Janssen, P. J. (1998). The additive effect of regulatory activities on top of intelligence in relation to academic performance in higher education. Learning and Instruction, 9(1), 77–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0959-4752(98)00019-X
Multon, K. D., Brown, S. D., & Lent, R. W. (1991). Relation of self-efficacy beliefs to academic outcomes: A meta-analytic investigation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38(1), 30–38. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0167.38.1.30
Ohtani, K., & Hisasaka, T. (2018). Beyond intelligence: a meta-analytic review of the relationship among metacognition, intelligence, and academic performance. Metacognition and Learning, 13(2), 179–212. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-018-9183-8
Panadero, E. (2017). A review of self-regulated learning: Six models and four directions for research. Frontiers in psychology, 8, 422. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00422
Parsons, J. E., & Ruble, D. N. (1977). The development of achievement-related expectations. Child Development, 48(3), 1075–1079. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128364
Perera, H., & DiGiacomo, M. (2013). The relationship of trait emotional intelligence with academic performance: A meta-analytic review. Learning and Individual Differences, 28, 20–33. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2013.08.002
Pérez-González, J. C. (2010). Trait emotional intelligence operationalized through the TEIQue: Construct validity and psycho- pedagogical implications. Unpublished PhD Dissertation. Universidad Nacional de Educación a Distancia (UNED).
Pérez-González, J. C., Saklofske, D. H., & Mavroveli, S. (2020). Editorial: Trait emotional intelligence: Foundations, assessment, and education. Frontiers in Psychology, 11. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00608
Pérez-González, J. C., & Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J. (2014). Trait emotional intelligence anchored within the Big Five, Big Two and Big One frameworks. Personality and Individual Differences, 65, 53–58. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.01.021
Petrides, K. V. (2009). Technical manual for the Trait Emotional Intelligence Questionnaires (TEIQue; 1st ed., 1st printing). London: London Psychometric Laboratory
Petrides, K. V., Mikolajczak, M., Mavroveli, S., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Furnham, A., & Pérez-González, J. C. (2016). Developments in trait emotional intelligence research. Emotion Review, 8(4), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1177/1754073916650493
Petrides, K. V., Pérez-González, J. C., & Furnham, A. (2007a). On the criterion and incremental validity of trait emotional intelligence. Cognition and Emotion, 21(1), 26–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930601038912
Petrides, K. V., Pita, R., & Kokkinaki, F. (2007b). The location of trait emotional intelligence in personality factor space. British Journal of Psychology, 98(2), 273–289. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712606X120618
Petrides, K. V., Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Siegling, A. B., Saklofske, D. H., & Mavroveli, S. (2018). Emotional Intelligence as Personality: Measurement and Role of Trait Emotional Intelligence in Educational Contexts. In K. Keefer, & J. Parker,Saklofske D. (Eds.) (Eds.), Emotional intelligence in education. The Springer series on human exceptionality (pp. 49–81). Cham: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-90633-1_3
Pintrich, P. R., Smith, D. A., Garcia, T., & McKeachie, W. J. (1991). A manual for the use of the Motivated Strategies for Learning Questionnaire (MSLQ). Ann Arbor: National Center for Research to Improve Postsecondary Teaching and Learning: University of Michigan
Pintrich, P. R. (2004). A conceptual framework for assessing motivation and self-regulated learning in college students. Educational Psychology Review, 16(4), 385–407. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-004-0006-x
Pintrich, P. R. (2000). The role of goal orientation in self-regulated learning. In M. Boekaerts, P. P. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 451–502). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-012109890-2/50043-3
Pintrich, P. R., & Zusho, A. (2007). Student motivation and self-regulated learning in the college classroom. In R.P. Perry & J.C. Smart (Eds.), The scholarship of teaching and learning in higher education: An evidence-based perspective, (pp. 731–810). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/1-4020-5742-3_16
Poropat, A. E. (2009). A meta-analysis of the Five-Factor Model of personality and academic performance. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 322–338. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014996
Putwain, D. W., Nicholson, L., Pekrun, R., Becker, S., & Symes, W. (2019). Expectancy of success, attainment value, engagement, and achievement: A moderated mediation analysis. Learning and Instruction, 60, 117–125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2018.11.005
Qualter, P., Gardner, K. J., Pope, D. J., Hutchinson, J. M., & Whiteley, H. E. (2012). Ability emotional intelligence, trait emotional intelligence, and academic success in British secondary schools: A 5 year longitudinal study. Learning and Individual Differences, 22(1), 83–91. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.007
Reeve, C. L., Bonaccio, S., & Winford, E. C. (2014). Cognitive ability, exam-related emotions and exam performance: A field study in a college setting. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 39(2), 124–133. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cedpsych.2014.03.001
Richardson, M., Abraham, C., & Bond, R. (2012). Psychological correlates of university students’ academic performance: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 138(2), 353–387. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026838
Robbins, S. B., Lauver, K., Le, H., Davis, D., Langley, R., & Carlstrom, A. (2004). Do psychosocial and study skill factors predict college outcomes? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 130(2), 261–288. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.261
Roces, C., Touron, J., & Gonzalez, M. C. (1995). Motivación, estrategias de aprendizaje y rendimiento de los alumnos universitarios. Bordon, 47(1), 107–121. https://hdl.handle.net/10171/21728
Román-González, M., Pérez-González, J. C., Moreno-León, J., & Robles, G. (2018). Extending the nomological network of computational thinking with non-cognitive factors. Computers in Human Behavior, 80, 441–459. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2017.09.030
Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., & El Khoury, J. (2019). A model of academic, personality, and emotion-related predictors of university academic performance. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2435. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02435
Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Khoury, E., Saade, J., G., & Salkhanian, M. (2016). Non-cognitive variables and academic achievement: The role of general and academic self-efficacy and trait emotional intelligence. In M. S. Khine (Ed.), Non-cognitive Factors and Educational Attainment (pp. 65–85). Brill Sense
Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Hernández-Torrano, D., Pérez-González, J. C., Batey, M., & Petrides, K. V. (2011). The relationship between trait emotional intelligence and creativity across subject domains. Motivation and Emotion, 35(4), 461–473. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-011-9227-8
Sanchez-Ruiz, M. J., Mavroveli, S., & Poullis, J. (2013). Trait emotional intelligence and its links to university performance: An examination. Personality and Individual Differences, 54(5), 658–662. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2012.11.013
Schaufeli, W. B., Martínez, I., Marques-Pinto, A., Salanova, M., & Bakker, A. (2002a). Burnout and engagement in university students: A cross-national study. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 33(5), 464–481. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022102033005003
Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., & Bakker, A. B. (2002b). The measurement of engagement and burnout: A two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. Journal of Happiness Studies, 3(1), 71–92. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1015630930326
Schuler, H., Funke, U., & Baron-Boldt, J. (1990). Predictive validity of school grades: A meta-analysis. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 39(1), 89–103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1464-0597.1990.tb01039.x
Schunk, D. H. (1991). Self-efficacy and academic motivation. Educational Psychologist, 26(3–4), 207–231. https://doi.org/10.1080/00461520.1991.9653133
Siegling, A. B., & Petrides, K. V. (2016). Drive: Theory and construct validation. Plos One, 11(7), e0157295. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0157295
Sorić, I., Penezić, Z., & Burić, I. (2017). The Big Five personality traits, goal orientations, and academic achievement. Learning and Individual Differences, 54, 126–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.024
Suárez Riveiro, J. M., Cabanach, R. G., & Arias, A. V. (2001). Multiple-goal pursuit and its relation to cognitive, self‐regulatory, and motivational strategies. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 71(4), 561–572. https://doi.org/10.1348/000709901158677
Thomas, C. L., Cassady, J. C., & Heller, M. L. (2017). The influence of emotional intelligence, cognitive test anxiety, and coping strategies on undergraduate academic performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 55, 40–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2017.03.001
Thurstone, L. L., & Thurstone, C. T. (1962). Primary mental abilities test. Chicago: Science Research Associates
Wagerman, S. A., & Funder, D. C. (2007). Acquaintance reports of personality and academic achievement: A case for conscientiousness. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 221–229. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2006.03.001
Wechsler, D. (1943). Non-intellective factors in general intelligence. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 38(1), 101–103. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0060613
Wolters, C. A., & Hussain, M. (2015). Investigating grit and its relations with college students’ self-regulated learning and academic achievement. Metacognition and Learning, 10(3), 293–311. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9128-9
Zhang, Z., & Yuan, K. H. (2018). Practical Statistical Power Analysis Using Webpower and R. Granger, IN: ISDSA Press
Zimmerman, B. J. (1989). A social cognitive view of self-regulated academic learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81(3), 329–339. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.81.3.329
Zimmerman, B. J. (1990). Self-regulation and academic achievement: An overview. Educational Psychologist, 25(1), 3–17. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15326985ep2501_2
Zimmerman, B. J., Bandura, A., & Martinez-Pons, M. (1992). Self-motivation for academic attainment: The role of self-efficacy beliefs and personal goal setting. American Educational Research Journal, 29(3), 663–676. https://doi.org/10.3102/00028312029003663
Zimmerman, B. J., & Moylan, A. R. (2009). Self-regulation: Where metacognition and motivation intersect. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook of metacognition in education (1st ed., pp. 311–328). Routledge
Zuffianò, A., Alessandri, G., Gerbino, M., Luengo Kanacri, B. P., Di Giunta, L., Milioni, M., & Caprara, G. V. (2013). Academic achievement: The unique contribution of self-efficacy beliefs in self-regulated learning beyond intelligence, personality traits, and self-esteem. Learning and Individual Differences, 23, 158–162. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lindif.2012.07.010
Acknowledgements
The authors wish to thank Natalie Tadros, Tatiana Khalaf, and Elma Nassar for their valuable assistance in reviewing this manuscript. The last author wishes to express gratitude to the Universidad de La Laguna for supporting her research in the last year through the Viera y Clavijo research contract.
Funding
Not applicable.
Open Access funding provided thanks to the CRUE-CSIC agreement with Springer Nature.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization and review after revisions, JCPG and GF; methodology, JCPG and GF; data collection, GF and AS; data cleaning and preparation, GF and AS; formal analysis, JCPG and MJSR; writing—original draft preparation, MJSR, YF and JCPG; writing—review and editing, JCPG, MJSR, YF, GF and AS; supervision, JCPG and MJSR; coordination of revisions, MJSR. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflicts of interest
The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.
Ethical Standards
This research involved human subjects, who filled out an informed consent. All ethical documentation regarding the study is available upon request.
Additional information
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Pérez-González, JC., Filella, G., Soldevila, A. et al. Integrating self-regulated learning and individual differences in the prediction of university academic achievement across a three-year-long degree. Metacognition Learning 17, 1141–1165 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09315-w
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-022-09315-w
Keywords
- Academic performance
- Conscientiousness
- Individual differences
- Self-regulated learning
- Noncognitive skills