Skip to main content
Log in

The Influence of Guilt Cognitions on Taxpayers’ Voluntary Disclosures

  • Published:
Journal of Business Ethics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Guilt is a powerful emotion that is known to influence ethical decision-making. Nevertheless, the role of guilt cognitions in influencing restorative behaviour following an unethical action is not well understood. Guilt cognitions are interrelated beliefs about an individual’s role in a negative event. We experimentally investigate the joint impact of three guilt cognitions—responsibility for a decision, justification for a decision, and foreseeability of consequences—on a taxpayer’s decision to make a tax amnesty disclosure. Tax amnesties encourage delinquent taxpayers to self-correct to avoid severe penalties that would result if their tax evasion were discovered. Our findings suggest a three-way interaction effect such that taxpayers are likely to make tax amnesty disclosures when they foresee that they will be caught by the tax authority, unless they can diffuse responsibility for their evasion and justify their evasion. Implications for tax policy and tax professionals are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Guilt and shame are the two emotions most relevant to ethical decision-making (Tangney et al. 2007). Shame is a dejection-based emotion arising from public exposure (Ferguson and Stegge 1998) and is not relevant to the tax amnesty decision, since a taxpayer who makes an amnesty disclosure is known only to a tax authority and would not be publicly exposed.

  2. The other two aspects of guilt, in Kubany and Watson’s (2003) model, are distress and personal values. Distress is an affective state involving feelings of unhappiness, depression, and anxiety (Hardy et al., 2003) that can result in emotional exhaustion (Tepper et al. 2007). It is impossible for us to manipulate these emotional feelings in an experimental context, and so we controlled for distress in our experiment. The other guilt cognition is personal values. “A personal value system is viewed as a relatively permanent perceptual framework which shapes and influences the general nature of an individual’s behavior. Values are similar to attitudes but are ingrained, permanent, and stable in nature” (England 1967, p. 54). In a tax situation, personal values refer to beliefs about the ethics of tax evasion. Since there is variation in personal values (England 1967), it is not possible for us to operationalize these in our experiment, and so we also controlled for attitudes concerning the ethicality of tax evasion.

  3. We do not predict main effects because main effects should not be tested if there is a significant interaction effect (Maxwell and Delaney 2004, p. 331).

  4. For any variables, a VIF value greater than 10 may indicate multicollinearity (Field 2009). The highest VIF value in the model was 3.5, and all but two values were below 1.2.

  5. As with any experiment, the results should be interpreted with caution. While we expect directional differences across conditions to hold in a different setting, factors unique to our scenarios may result in different magnitudes of amnesty participation.

References

  • Alm, J. (2012). Measuring, explaining, and controlling tax evasion: lessons from theory, experiments, and field studies. International Tax and Public Finance, 19(1), 54–77.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Beck, W. (1991). Wiping the slate clean: Individual response to state tax amnesties. Southern Economic Journal, 57(4), 1043–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Beck, W. (1993). Tax amnesties and compliance in the long run: A time series analysis. National Tax Journal, 46(1), 53–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., McKee, M., & Beck, W. (1990). Amazing grace: Tax amnesties and compliance. National Tax Journal, 43(1), 23–37.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & Torgler, B. (2011). Do ethics matter? Tax compliance and morality. Journal of Business Ethics, 101(4), 635–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J. (1991). The desirability of a permanent tax amnesty. Journal of Public Economics, 45(2), 143–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baer, K., & Le Borgne, E. (2008). Tax amnesties: Theory, Trends, and some alternatives. Washington, DC: International Monetary Fund.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldry, J. (1986). Tax evasion is not a gamble. Economics Letters, 22(1), 333–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bobek, D., Hageman, A., & Kelliher, C. (2013). Analyzing the role of social norms in tax compliance behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 115(3), 451–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bohns, V., & Flynn, F. (2013). Guilt by design: Structuring organizations to elicit guilt as an affective reaction to failure. Organizational Science, 24(4), 1157–1173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brockner, J., Greenberg, J., Brockner, A., Bortz, J., Davy, J., & Carter, C. (1986). Layoffs, equity theory, and work performance: Further evidence of the impact of survivor guilt. Academy of Management Journal, 29(2), 373–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Canadians for Tax Fairness (2014). Tackle tax havens. http://www.taxfairness.ca/action/tackle-tax-havens.

  • Cho, J., Linn, S., & Nakibullah, A. (1996). Tax evasion with psychic costs and penalty renegotiation. Southern Economic Journal, 63(1), 172–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christian, C., Gupta, S., & Young, J. (2002). Evidence on subsequent filing from the state of Michigan’s income tax amnesty. National Tax Journal, 55(4), 703–721.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chung, J., & Trivedi, V. (2003). The effect of friendly persuasion and gender on taxpayers’ compliance behavior. Journal of Business Ethics, 47(2), 133–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coricelli, G., Joffily, M., Montmarquette, C., & Villeval, M. (2010). Cheating, emotions, and rationality: An experiment on tax evasion. Experimental Economics, 13(2), 226–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crawford, J., McCaul, K., Veltum, L., & Bouechko, V. (1990). Understanding attributions of victim blame for rape: Sex, violence, and foreseeability. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 20(1), 1–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curtis, M. (2006). Are audit-related ethical decisions dependent upon mood? Journal of Business Ethics, 68(2), 191–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dulleck, U., Fooken, J., Newton, C., Ristl, A., Schaffner, M., & Torgler, B. (2012). Tax compliance and psychic costs: Behavioral experimental evidence using a physiological marker. CREMA Working Paper Series 2012–2019. http://www.crema-research.ch/papers/2012-19.pdf.

  • Eisenberg, N. (2000). Emotion, regulation, and moral development. Annual Review of Psychology, 51(1), 665–697.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • England, G. (1967). Personal value systems of American managers. Academy of Management Journal, 10(1), 53–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1994a). The role of moral sentiments and audit perceptions in tax compliance. Public Finance, 49(Supplement), 70–89.

    Google Scholar 

  • Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1994b). Honesty and evasion in the tax compliance game. RAND Journal of Economics, 25(1), 1–19.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferguson, T., & Stegge, H. (1998). Measuring guilt in children: A rose by any other name still has thorns. In J. Bybee (Ed.), guilt in children (pp. 19–74). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Field, A. (2009). Discovering statistics using SPSS (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R. (1993). Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning. Journal of Consumer Research, 20(2), 303–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, R., Goddeeris, J., & Young, J. (1989). Participation in tax amnesties: The individual income tax. National Tax Journal, 42(1), 15–27.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ghorbani, M., Liao, Y., Çayköylü, S., & Chand, M. (2013). Guilt, shame, and reparative behavior: The effect of psychological proximity. Journal of Business Ethics, 114(2), 311–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goodstein, J., & Butterfield, K. (2010). Extending the horizon of business ethics: Restorative justice and the aftermath of unethical behavior. Business Ethics Quarterly, 20(3), 433–480.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grasmick, H., & Scott, W. (1982). Tax evasion and mechanisms of social control: A comparison with grand and petty theft. Journal of Economic Psychology, 2(3), 213–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Griffin, M., Babin, B., & Attaway, J. (1996). Anticipation of injurious consumption outcomes and its impact on consumer attributions of blame. Journal of the Academy of Marketing Science, 24(4), 314–327.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haines, E., & Jost, J. (2000). Placating the powerless: Effects of legitimate and illegitimate explanation on affect, memory, and stereotyping. Social Justice Research, 13(3), 219–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamilton, J., & Strutton, D. (1994). Two practical guidelines for resolving truth-telling problems. Journal of Business Ethics, 13(11), 899–912.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hardy, G., Woods, D., & Wall, T. (2003). The impact of psychological distress on absence from work. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88(2), 306–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hasseldine, J. (1998). Tax amnesties: An international review. Bulletin for International Fiscal Documentation, 52(7), 303–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, R., & Huby, M. (2004). The construction and interpretation of vignettes in social research. Social Work and Social Sciences Review, 11(1), 36–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ilies, R., Peng, A., Savani, K., & Dimotakis, N. (2013). Guilty and helpful: An emotion-based reparatory model of voluntary work behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 98(6), 1051–1059.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Internal Revenue Service (2011). Tax gap “map” tax year 2006. http://www.irs.gov/pub/newsroom/tax_gap_map_2006.pdf.

  • Jaccard, J. (2001). Interaction effects in logistic regression. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Kubany, E., & Watson, S. (2003). Guilt: Elaboration of a multidimensional model. The Psychological Record, 53(1), 51–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lagnado, D., & Channon, S. (2008). Judgments of cause and blame: The effects of intentionality and foreseeability. Cognition, 108(3), 754–770.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lévy-Garboua, L., Masclet, D., & Montmarquette, C. (2009). A behavioral Laffer curve: Emergence of a social norm of fairness in a real effort experiment. Journal of Economic Psychology, 30(2), 147–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, L., Brown, M., Mantzke, R., Tower, R., & Wright, L. (2006). State tax amnesties: Forgiveness is divine—and possible profitable. State Tax Notes, 48(8), 497–511.

    Google Scholar 

  • Maciejovsky, B., Schwarzenberger, H., & Kirchler, E. (2012). Rationality vs. emotions: The case of tax ethics and compliance. Journal of Business Ethics, 109(3), 339–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maitlis, S., & Ozcelik, H. (2004). Toxic decision processes: A study of emotion and organizational decision making. Organizational Science, 15(4), 375–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Malik, A., & Schwab, R. (1991). The economics of tax amnesties. Journal of Public Economics, 46(1), 29–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marceau, N., & Mongrain, S. (2000). Amnesties and co-operation. International Tax and Public Finance, 7(3), 259–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mason, R., & Calvin, L. (1984). Public confidence and admitted tax evasion: Abstract. National Tax Journal, 37(4), 489–498.

    Google Scholar 

  • Masten, C., Telzer, E., & Eisenberger, N. (2011). An fMRI investigation of attributing negative social treatment to racial discrimination. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 1042–1051.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maxwell, S., & Delaney, H. (2004). Designing experiments and analyzing data: A model comparison perspective (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molero, J., & Pujol, F. (2012). Walking inside the potential tax evader’s mind: Tax morale does matter. Journal of Business Ethics, 105(2), 151–162.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mulilis, J.-P., Duval, T., & Rombach, D. (2001). Personal responsibility for tornado preparedness: Commitment or choice? Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 31(8), 1659–1688.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD (2015). Global forum on transparency and exchange of information for tax purposes. http://www.oecd.org/tax/transparency/.

  • Roberts, M. (1994). An experimental approach to changing taxpayers’ attitudes towards fairness and compliance via television. The Journal of the American Taxation Association, 16(1), 67–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, R. D., & Orleans, S. (1967). On legal sanctions. University of Chicago Law Review, 34, 282–300.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shalvi, S., Dana, J., Handgraaf, M., & De Dreu, C. (2011). Justified ethicality: Observing desired counterfactuals modifies ethical perceptions and behavior. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 115(2), 181–190.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slemrod, J. (2007). Cheating ourselves: The economics of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(1), 25–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, K., & Stalans, L. (1991). Encouraging tax compliance with positive incentives: A conceptual framework and research directions. Law and Policy, 13(1), 35–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, R., Webster, J., Parrott, W., & Eyre, H. (2002). The role of public exposure in moral and nonmoral shame and guilt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(1), 138–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steenhaut, S., & Van Kenhove, P. (2006). The mediating role of anticipated guilt in consumers’ ethical decision-making. Journal of Business Ethics, 69(3), 269–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tangney, J., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345–372.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tepper, B., Moss, S., Lockhart, D., & Carr, J. (2007). Abusive supervision, upward maintenance communication, and subordinates’ psychological distress. Academy of Management Journal, 50(5), 1169–1180.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2004). Moral suasion: An alternative tax policy strategy? Evidence from a controlled field experiment in Switzerland. Economic of Governance, 5, 235–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2007). Tax compliance and tax morale: A theoretical and empirical analysis. Cheltenham, UK: Edward Elgar.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2013). A field experiment on moral suasion and tax compliance focusing on underdeclaration and overdeduction. FinanzArchiv: Zeitschrift für das Gesamte Finanzwesen, 69(4), 393–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B., & Schaltegger, C. (2005a). An exploration of tax amnesties around the world with a special focus on Switzerland. Tax Notes International, 38, 1193–1203.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B., & Schaltegger, C. (2005b). Tax amnesties and political participation. Public Finance Review, 33(3), 403–431.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weber, J. (1992). Scenarios in business ethics research: Review, critical assessment, and recommendations. Business Ethics Quarterly, 2(2), 137–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weinreb, A. (2009). Tax amnesty programs and voluntary compliance initiatives: A way to mitigate declining state revenues. The Tax Adviser, 40(6), 396–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wenzel, M., Okimoto, T., Feather, N., & Platow, M. (2008). Retributive and restorative justice. Law and Human Behavior, 32(5), 375–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zhang, H.-J., Zhou, L.-M., & Luo, Y.-J. (2009). The influence of responsibility on regret intensity: An ERP study. Acta Psychological Sinica, 41(5), 454–463.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was funded by the Centre for Accounting Ethics, University of Waterloo. We appreciate helpful comments received from Colin Boyd, Amy Hageman, Daryl Koehn, Ioana Moca, and the two anonymous reviewers.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Cass Hausserman.

Appendix

Appendix

Scenario Screen with Manipulations

Mary is a taxpayer who lives in Ontario. Three years ago, she received a large inheritance and transferred the proceeds to a bank account in the Cayman Islands, but transferred them back to Canada this year. The CRA does not know about this bank account and these investments. The investments generated $83,400 annually. Her total annual income for each of the last 3 years was $97,500.

Responsibility Manipulation

  • High Mary researched this investment opportunity, discovered that she would not receive any tax reporting forms from the Cayman Islands, and realized that she could save a lot of money in taxes.

  • Low Mary researched this investment opportunity, learned from her tax advisor that she would not receive any tax reporting forms from the Cayman Islands, and was told by her tax advisor that she could save a lot of money in taxes.

Justification Manipulation

  • High Mary has never reported any of this investment income on her Canadian tax return because she used the cash she would have paid in taxes on this investment income to pay for experimental drugs for her 4-year-old daughter, who was diagnosed with a rare and aggressive form of cancer.

  • Low Mary has never reported any of this investment income on her Canadian tax return because she used the cash she would have paid in taxes for home renovations.

Foreseeability Manipulation

  • High Last week Mary saw a news broadcast about the importance of paying taxes. The broadcast also reported that the CRA is working on an information sharing agreement with her bank in the Cayman Islands so that the CRA could obtain the names of Canadian investors.

  • Low Last week Mary saw a news broadcast about the importance of paying taxes. The broadcast also reported that it would be virtually impossible for the CRA to obtain information about Canadians who had investments in the Cayman Islands.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Dunn, P., Farrar, J. & Hausserman, C. The Influence of Guilt Cognitions on Taxpayers’ Voluntary Disclosures. J Bus Ethics 148, 689–701 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3031-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10551-016-3031-z

Keywords

Navigation