Skip to main content
Log in

Cheating, emotions, and rationality: an experiment on tax evasion

  • Published:
Experimental Economics Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The economics-of-crime approach usually ignores the emotional cost and benefit of cheating. In this paper, we investigate the relationships between emotions, deception, and rational decision-making by means of an experiment on tax evasion. Emotions are measured by skin conductance responses and self-reports. We show that the intensity of anticipated and anticipatory emotions before reporting income positively correlates with both the decision to cheat and the proportion of evaded income. The experienced emotional arousal after an audit increases with the monetary sanctions and the arousal is even stronger when the evader’s picture is publicly displayed. We also find that the risk of a public exposure of deception deters evasion whereas the amount of fines encourages evasion. These results suggest that an audit policy that strengthens the emotional dimension of cheating favors compliance.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allingham, M., & Sandmo, A. (1972). Income tax evasion: a theoretical analysis. Journal of Public Economics, 1(3–4), 323–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J. (1991). A perspective on the experimental analysis of taxpayer reporting. The Accounting Review, 66(3), 577–593.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., & McKee, M. (2004). Tax compliance as a coordination game. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 54(3), 297–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Jackson, B., & McKee, M. (1992b). Institutional uncertainty and taxpayer compliance. American Economic Review, 82(4), 1018–1026.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., McClelland, G. H., & Schulze, W. D. (1992a). Why do people pay taxes? Journal of Public Economics, 48, 21–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alm, J., Cronshaw, M. B., & McKee, M. (1993). Tax compliance with endogenous audit selection rules. Kyklos, 1, 27–45.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anders, S., Lotze, M., & Erb, M. (2004). Brain activity underlying emotional valence and arousal: a response-related fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 23, 200–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Andreoni, J., Erard, B., & Feinstein, J. (1998). Tax compliance. Journal of Economic Literature, 36, 818–860.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baldry, J. C. (1986). Tax evasion is not a gamble. Economics Letters, 22, 333–335.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Shakar, G., Bornstein, G., Hopfensitz, A., & van Winden, F. (2007). Reciprocity and emotions in bargaining: using physiological and self-report measures. Journal of Economic Psychology, 28, 314–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bernasconi, M., & Zanardi, A. (2004). Tax evasion, tax rates, and reference dependence. FinanzArchiv: Public Finance Analysis, 60(3), 422–445.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blumenthal, M., Christian, C., & Slemrod, J. (2001). Do normative appeals affect tax compliance? Evidence from a controlled experiment in Minnesota. National Tax Journal, 54(1), 125–138.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucsein, W. (1992). Plenum series in behavioral psychology and medicine. Electrodermal activity. New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M. (2000). Emotion and motivation. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Measuring emotion: behavior, feeling and physiology. In R. Lane & L. Nadel (Eds.), Cognitive neuroscience of emotion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, M. M., Codispoti, M., Cuthbert, B. N., & Lang, P. J. (2001). Emotion and motivation I: defensive and appetitive reactions in picture processing. Emotion, 1, 276–298.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cason, T. N., & Gangadharan, L. (2006). An experimental study of compliance and leverage in auditing and regulatory enforcement. Economic Inquiry, 44(2), 352–366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Dufwenberg, M. (2006). Promises and partnership. Econometrica, 74(6), 1579–1601.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Charness, G., & Gneezy, U. (2003). Portfolio choice and risk attitudes: an experiment. Mimeo.

  • Collins, J. H., & Plumlee, R. D. (1991). The taxpayer’s labor and reporting decision: the effect of audit schemes. The Accounting Review, 66, 559–576.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cowell, F. (1990). Cheating the government: the economics of evasion. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cummings, R. G., Martinez-Vazquez, J., McKee, M., & Torgler, B. (2009). Tax morale affects tax compliance: evidence from surveys and artefactual field experiments. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 70(3), 447–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ error. New York: Avon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., & Filion, D. L. (2000). The electrodermal system. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (2nd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • de Quervain, D. J. F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buck, A., & Fehr, E. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science, 305, 1254–1258.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dhami, S., & Al-Nowaihi, A. (2007). Why do people pay taxes? Prospect theory versus expected utility theory. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 64(1), 171–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Edelberg, R. (1967). Electrical properties of the skin. In C. C. Brown (Ed.), Methods in psychophysiology. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins.

    Google Scholar 

  • Elster, J. (1996). Rationality and the emotions. The Economic Journal, 106(438), 1386–1397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fortin, B., Lacroix, G., & Villeval, M. C. (2007). Tax evasion and social interactions. Journal of Public Economics, 91(11–12), 2089–2112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gerschlager, C. (Ed.) (2005). Deception in markets: an economic analysis. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gneezy, U. (2005). Deception: the role of consequences. American Economic Review, 95(1), 384–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greiner, B. (2004). An online recruitment system for economic experiments. In K. Kremer & V. Macho (Eds.), GWDG Bericht : Vol. 63. Forschung und wissenschaftliches Rechnen 2003 (pp. 79–93). Göttingen: Ges. für Wiss. Datenverarbeitung.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halla, M., & Schneider, F. G. (2009). Taxes and benefits: two distinct options to cheat on the state? Research paper, IZA.

  • Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316, 1622–1625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Miller, D. (1986). Norm theory: comparing reality to its alternatives. Psychological Review, 93, 136–153.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirchler, E. (2007). The economic psychology of tax behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Lang, P. J., Bradley, M. M., & Cuthbert, B. N. (1992). A motivational analysis of emotion: reflex-cortex connections. Psychological Science, 3, 44–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. (2000). Emotions in economic theory and economic behavior. American Economic Review, 90(2), 426–432.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loewenstein, G. F., Hsee, C. K., Weber, E. U., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 267–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Russell, J. A. (1974). Approach to environmental psychology. Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. O., & Low, K. (2001). Motor processes in simple, go/no-go, and choice reaction time tasks: a psychophysiological analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 27(2), 266–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Myles, G. D., & Naylor, R. A. (1996). A model of tax evasion with group conformity and social customs. European Journal of Political Economy, 12(1), 49–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rubinstein, A. (2007). Instinctive and cognitive reasoning: a study of response times. Economic Journal, 117(523), 1243–1259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sanchez-Pages, S., & Vorsatz, M. (2007). An experimental study of truth-telling in sender-receiver game. Games and Economic Behavior, 61(1), 86–112.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seiter, J. S., & Bruschke, J. (2007). Deception and emotion: the effects of motivation, relationship type, and sex on expected feelings of guilt and shame following acts of deception in United States and Chinese samples. Communication Studies, 58(1), 1–16.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Slemrod, J. (1998). On voluntary compliance, voluntary taxes, and social capital. National Tax Journal, LI, 485–492.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slemrod, J. (2007). Cheating ourselves: the economics of tax evasion. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(1), 25–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spicer, M. W., & Thomas, J. E. (1982). Audit probabilities and the tax evasion decision: an experimental approach. Journal of Economic Psychology, 2, 241–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgler, B. (2007). Tax compliance and tax morale. A theoretical and empirical analysis. Cheltenham: Edward Elgar.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Winden, F., Krawczyk, M., & Hopfensitz, A. (2008). Investment, resolution of risk, and the role of affect. Amsterdam, CREED Working Paper.

  • Wild, B. (2003). Are emotions contagious? Evoked emotions while viewing emotionally expressive faces: quality, quantity, time course and gender differences. Psychiatry Research, 102(2), 109–124.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yitzhaki, S. (1974). A note on ‘income tax evasion: a theoretical analysis’. Journal of Public Economics, 3(2), 201–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zajonc, R. B. (1984). The primacy of affect. American Psychologist, 39(2), 117–123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zeiliger, R. (2000). A presentation of regate. Internet-based software for experimental economics. http://www.gate.cnrs.fr/~zeiliger/regate/RegateIntro.ppt. Lyon: GATE.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Marie Claire Villeval.

Additional information

This research was supported by a grant from the Rhône-Alpes Region (CIBLE program).

Electronic Supplementary Material

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

(DOC 45 KB)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Coricelli, G., Joffily, M., Montmarquette, C. et al. Cheating, emotions, and rationality: an experiment on tax evasion. Exp Econ 13, 226–247 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-010-9237-5

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10683-010-9237-5

Keywords

JEL Classification

Navigation