Abstract
Efficient spatial orientation in the natural environment is crucial for the survival of most animal species. Cataglyphis desert ants possess excellent navigational skills. After far-ranging foraging excursions, the ants return to their inconspicuous nest entrance using celestial and panoramic cues. This review focuses on the question about how naïve ants acquire the necessary spatial information and adjust their visual compass systems. Naïve ants perform structured learning walks during their transition from the dark nest interior to foraging under bright sunlight. During initial learning walks, the ants perform rotational movements with nest-directed views using the earth’s magnetic field as an earthbound compass reference. Experimental manipulations demonstrate that specific sky compass cues trigger structural neuronal plasticity in visual circuits to integration centers in the central complex and mushroom bodies. During learning walks, rotation of the sky-polarization pattern is required for an increase in volume and synaptic complexes in both integration centers. In contrast, passive light exposure triggers light-spectrum (especially UV light) dependent changes in synaptic complexes upstream of the central complex. We discuss a multisensory circuit model in the ant brain for pathways mediating structural neuroplasticity at different levels following passive light exposure and multisensory experience during the performance of learning walks.
Similar content being viewed by others
Avoid common mistakes on your manuscript.
Introduction
Many insect species, particularly social insects like ants or social bees and wasps, heavily depend on navigational skills for food provisioning of their brood. As offspring is raised in a common nest, the ants search for profitable food locations and then return to their nest (central place foraging). Desert ants of the genus Cataglyphis are excellent experimental models to study the mechanisms underlying insect navigation (Wehner 2008, 2020). These thermophilic ants live under mostly harsh conditions. Cataglyphis ants leave their underground nest even during the hottest times of the day for far-ranging food searches. These excursions may extend to walking distances of up to 1,500 m and distances of 350 m or more away from the nest (e.g. Huber and Knaden 2015; Ronacher 2008). Cataglyphis species inhabiting featureless desert environments employ path integration as their main navigational routine to find their way back to the nest (Knaden and Graham 2016; Ronacher 2008; Wehner 2020). Foraging ants use the position of the sun and the associated sky-polarization cues to determine the directions of all path segments during the outbound walks, while a step integrator (Wittlinger et al. 2006, 2007) estimates the distances used to calculate a home vector that encodes both the direction and distance back to the nest entrance. Especially Cataglyphis ant species living in more cluttered environments use the panoramic scenery and landmarks as additional visual cues (Fleischmann et al. 2017; Fleischmann et al. 2018a; Huber and Knaden 2015; Wehner 2020; for a review see Cheng et al. 2014).
The navigational routines of mainly visually guided foraging ants have been characterized in much detail at the level of behavior and generated valuable models (for an excellent and extensive recent review see Wehner 2020). The question of how naïve ants acquire their navigational information during so-called learning walks at the beginning of their foraging careers, particularly the underlying neuronal processes, have gained attention only more recently (for reviews: Grob et al. 2019; Rössler 2019; Wehner and Rössler 2013). Path integration, visual learning, and learning walks are widespread in other species of ants including other desert-ant species like Ocymyrmex or Melophorus and non-desert ant species like Myrmecia bull ants and Formica wood ants (e.g. Deeti and Cheng 2021; Jayatilaka et al. 2018; Müller and Wehner 2010; Nicholson et al. 1999; Wystrach et al. 2014a, b).
In the present review, we integrate results from studies on learning walks with a special focus on Cataglyphis ants. We provide a synthesis of the knowledge gained on neuronal plasticity (neuroplasticity) in visual circuits of the ant’s brain triggered by multisensory experience during learning walks that are performed during a sensitive period before the onset of foraging behavior. Most studies on neuroplasticity have been done in Cataglyphis ants. We complement these by looking at select neuroanatomical, neurophysiological, and behavioral studies on multisensory pathways in the brain of other insects. This synthesis aims at understanding the neuronal circuits mediating multisensory experience during learning walks and at stimulating future studies to unravel the neuronal mechanisms underlying the striking flexibility of navigational systems in the brain of these remarkable navigators.
Multisensory experience during the performance of learning-walks
Cataglyphis foragers use path integration to return to their nest on the shortest distance possible by integrating directional (compass) and distance information. The resulting home vector leads the ants back to their nest entrance along an almost straight path (Wehner 2020). A recent study manipulated path integration memory by experimental cooling of ants that had built up a full home vector after finding food (Pisokas et al. 2022). In this study, the behavioral analyses of homing trajectories were combined with computer simulations of homing strategies based on different path-integration memory models. The results suggest that path integration memory is most likely stored in a redundant Cartesian coordinate system in the ant’s brain. However, despite such an efficient mechanism, the underlying neuronal processes are prone to cumulative errors, especially over long distances. To compensate for these errors and to optimize homing, the ants, whenever available, use panoramic sceneries, the skyline, and landmarks as additional visual guidance cues (e.g., Wehner et al. 2016; Wystrach et al. 2015).
How do naïve ants that leave their nest for the first time acquire the necessary information about relevant sky-compass cues and the panoramic scenery around the nest entrance to ensure efficient homing? Using a sun-based compass for navigation comprises additional challenges (for the definition of navigation see Grob et al. 2021c). How do the ants calibrate their sky compass system? The ants use the azimuthal position of the sun and associated sky-polarization patterns as compass cues. However, the azimuthal course of the sun over the day (solar ephemeris) is a non-linear function that depends on the time of the year and the geographical position. Consequently, an internal representation of the solar ephemeris cannot be inherited genetically, and the ants somehow need to calibrate their internal compass systems with the function representing the azimuthal path of the sun (Wehner 2020; Wehner and Lanfranconi 1981; Wehner and Müller 1993). How do naïve ants acquire this function and what is used as the earthbound compass reference during learning the relevant visual information? What are the neuroplastic changes underlying visual learning and long-term memory formation before the ants are heading out on their first foraging trips (Rössler 2019)?
The individual life history of Cataglyphis ants comprises a period of about 4 weeks with varying tasks inside the darkness of the underground nest followed by an outdoor foraging period of on average 7 days in bright sunlight when the ants actively search for food (Schmid-Hempel and Schmid-Hempel 1984). Because of this drastic transition and striking differences in the associated behavioral phenotypes (polyethism), Cataglyphis ants are ideal experimental models for investigating the neuronal mechanisms underlying this behavioral plasticity (Rössler 2019). The interior-exterior transition will be the focus of the following chapters.
Behavioral studies over recent years revealed important insights into so-called learning walks (also termed exploration or orientation walks) that naïve ants perform before they head out on their first foraging excursions (for recent reviews on learning walks: Fleischmann et al. 2020a; Freas et al. 2019; Zeil and Fleischmann 2019). To understand how naïve ants acquire the relevant visual information, quantitative analyses of learning-walk behavior at high spatial and temporal resolution in the natural habitat was a crucial step. Naïve ants perform structured learning walks comprising sequences of loops that explore different sectors around the nest entrance (Fleischmann et al. 2016, 2017; Stieb et al. 2012; Wehner et al. 2004) (Fig. 1). The ants perform these behavioral routines in an extending radius around the nest entrance over a period of 2–3 days. During that time, they do not collect any food items (Fleischmann et al. 2016). High-resolution video analyses revealed that the ants frequently stop their forward movements to perform rotational elements (pirouettes) interrupted by 100–200 ms stops. During the longest stop in a pirouette, the ants look back in the direction of the nest entrance. Similar findings were made earlier in a study on re-learning walks in Ocymyrmex ants (Müller and Wehner 2010). As the nest entrance is not visible from the ants’ perspectives, the ants must use path integration for the alignments of their body axis during these repeated turn-back behaviors.
The highly structured behavior during learning walks suggests that ants and other insects like wasps (during their learning flights) systematically acquire and memorize nest-directed panoramic views around the nest entrance (Graham et al. 2010; Zeil 2012). However, the acquisition of nest-related views may vary across species. Furthermore, recent work on wasps suggests that they do not pause when acquiring views 45° left and right from the nest entrance (Stürzl et al. 2016). Another study on ants indicates that views are memorized in other directions than the nest direction (Wystrach et al. 2020). In addition to acquiring panoramic snapshot memories, the ants most likely calibrate their internal solar ephemeris representation during learning walks (Grob et al. 2019). Evidence supporting this assumption most recently came from a study showing that only learning walks under a rotating sky polarization pattern resulted in plastic neuronal changes in visual integration centers of the ant’s brain (Grob et al. 2022) (see details in the following chapters). Furthermore, displacement experiments show that Cataglyphis ants need at least 2 days and a minimum distance of 0.5 m from the nest entrance for the proper performance of learning walks to memorize panoramic cues for successful homing (Fleischmann et al. 2016, 2018a). These findings are supported by earlier studies in Melophorus desert ants (Wystrach et al. 2012).
What provides the compass reference for path integration and panoramic snapshot learning during initial (or naïve) learning walks, at a time when the celestial compass has not yet been calibrated? To ask whether naïve ants nevertheless use the sun as a compass for path integration during their short learning walks, different filter settings above the nest entrance were used to manipulate relevant skylight cues (Grob et al. 2017). With important celestial cues blocked (sun position, sky polarization pattern, UV light), the ants still perform stops with accurately aligned goal-directed views during learning-walk pirouettes. Although it had been shown that spectral and light intensity gradients can also be used for a celestial compass (Wehner 1997; Wystrach et al. 2014a, b), the ants might as well use other compass cues at this early stage. The earth’s magnetic field came into focus as a candidate for an alternative compass used during initial learning walks. Under experimental conditions using disarray of the earth’s magnetic field by a circular flat coil or elimination of the horizontal component of the earth’s magnetic field by a Helmholtz coil setup, the ants gazed into random directions even with all natural celestial cues accessible (Fleischmann et al. 2018b). Moreover, systematic rotation of the horizontal component of the earth’s magnetic field resulted in gazes towards a fictive nest entrance rotated by the same angle as the magnetic field. This provided the first unambiguous evidence that an insect can use the earth’s magnetic field as a compass cue that is both necessary and sufficient for path integration. This also suggests that the geomagnetic field provides the earthbound compass reference for path integration during initial learning walks. Consequently, the magnetic sense of Cataglyphis ants provides the compass reference during view-based learning of the visual panorama and can potentially be used to calibrate the celestial compass systems. Finally, the unambiguous role of the magnetic sense in path integration renders Cataglyphis ants as a highly promising experimental model for the study of magnetoreception in an insect (Fleischmann et al. 2020b). Compared to the clear-cut results in Cataglyphis, the use of the earth’s magnetic field for directional orientation in other insect species, so far, was less distinct or only evident in combination with other cues (e.g. Dreyer et al. 2018; see reviews in Fleischmann et al. 2020b; Wajnberg et al. 2010). Interestingly, experienced Cataglyphis foragers still sense the magnetic field but the function of magnetic field information in orientation is different from the one in naïve ants. This was recently shown by using magnetic manipulations in the natural habitat and during so-called re-learning walks of foragers (Fleischmann et al. 2022).
Overall, the behavioral results in Cataglyphis suggest that during their first learning walks the ants experience panoramic snapshot views, skylight compass cues, and directional cues provided by the earth’s magnetic field as important sensory information. In the following chapters, we will have a closer look into the ants’ brains and the associated sensory pathways to then focus on neuroplasticity in visual pathways triggered during different phases of the interior-exterior transition.
The brain of Cataglyphis and sensory pathways associated with navigation
How does the relatively small brain of Cataglyphis ants process and memorize multisensory navigational information during learning walks? Where in the brain are sky-compass cues and local panoramic views computed and stored? How is visual information integrated with input from a magnetic compass and from other sensory modalities? While the first two questions can be addressed more easily, the third question, particularly regarding the magnetic sense, is more difficult to answer. Recently a comprehensive 3D atlas of the entire brain of Cataglyphis nodus has been published and provides a solid ground for neuroanatomical analyses (Habenstein et al. 2020) (Fig. 2a, c). For comparability, the nomenclature for individual neuropils was adapted to the unified nomenclature applied to the brain atlas in the fly, Drosophila melanogaster (Ito et al. 2014). The 3D brain atlas in Cataglyphis nodus workers revealed 33 distinct brain neuropils and 30 connecting fiber tracts including six visual fiber tracts between the optic lobes and the central brain (Habenstein et al. 2020). The Cataglyphis brain atlas is accessible in 3D on the Insect Brain Database website (https://www.insectbraindb.org/) (Habenstein et al. 2020; Heinze et al. 2021). Neuroanatomical analyses in another study revealed differences in 3D structures and volumes of brain compartments between the two female castes (workers and queens) and males (Grob et al. 2021b), most likely reflecting differences in their lifestyles and navigational skills.
In addition to the neuroanatomical studies, analyses of neuropeptides and their localization in the Cataglyphis brain have recently been performed. For this, transcriptomal analyses were combined with mass spectrometry detection and localization to reveal the most comprehensive neuropeptidome that has been obtained from an ant’s brain so far (Habenstein et al. 2021a, 2021b; Schmitt et al. 2017). Both the neuroanatomical and neuropeptidome databases provide rich sources for future structure–function analyses in the brain of Cataglyphis ants, for example, to detect life-stage dependent changes in brain neuropils and neuromodulators, and for comparison of those attributes with other ant species.
Visual pathways from the optic lobes
Where in the ant’s brain are sky compass cues and panoramic information processed and stored? Early behavioral manipulations have already indicated that local panoramic information is stored as long-lasting (lifetime) memories, whereas path integration information expresses a faster memory decay. This early discovery was already highly suggestive of two distinct channels for visual information transfer (Ziegler and Wehner 1997). Neuroanatomical studies revealed the anatomical details of two separate major visual pathways leading from the optic lobes to two high-order visual integration centers in Cataglyphis’ brain, the mushroom bodies (MB) and the central complex (CX) (Grob et al. 2017; Rössler 2019; Schmitt et al. 2016) (Fig. 2b, d). The presence of a visual pathway to the MB in ants was first shown by Gronenberg and Hölldober (1999) and Gronenberg (2008). In Cataglyphis these neuronal pathways were traced using fluorescent dye injections, confocal imaging, and 3D reconstructions. Recently, Habenstein et al. (2020) mapped four additional tracts and commissures from the optic lobes to the central brain in Cataglyphis nodus. In the following, we outline the two major visual circuits from the optic lobes to the CX and MB in Cataglyphis and compare these with the conditions in other insect species.
One pathway starts from the dorsal rim area (the polarization-sensitive part of the compound eye) and proceeds via the lamina, medulla, and lobula along the anterior optic tract (AOT) to the anterior optic tubercle (Schmitt et al. 2016) (Fig. 2b, d). From there, neurons project to the bulbs of the lateral complex where they form exceptionally large microglomerular synaptic complexes that are synaptically connected with the dendrites of tangential neurons which project to the lower unit of the central body, a subdivision of the CX (Grob et al. 2017, 2019; Schmitt et al. 2016). Accordingly, this pathway was termed CX pathway. Physiological and neuroanatomical studies in the locust, bee, dung beetle and monarch butterfly combined with modeling approaches show that this highly conserved pathway to the CX integrates sky-compass information and receives information about movement, the two parameters required for path integration (e.g. Heinze and Homberg 2007; Heinze and Reppert 2011; Held et al. 2016; Homberg et al. 2011; Kaiser et al. 2022; Stone et al. 2017; Webb and Wystrach 2016).
The second pathway leads from the medulla of the optic lobes via the anterior superior optic tract (ASOT) without any further relay to the visual compartments (collar) of the MB and was termed MB pathway (Ehmer and Gronenberg 2002; Grob et al. 2017, 2019) (Fig. 2b, d). Phylogenetic comparison within the Hymenoptera suggests that this prominent visual pathway to the MBs has evolved in the higher Hymenoptera, particularly parasitoid and social Hymenoptera (Farris and Schulmeister 2011). Two recent ablation studies in ants, one using injections of local anesthetics at the level of the visual projection neuron input in Formica wood ants (Buehlmann et al. 2020), and another study in Myrmecia bull ants using local applications of anesthetics to the vertical lobes, major output structures of the MBs (Kamhi et al. 2020) came to the coincident conclusion that the MBs are required for panoramic landmark orientation during homing. In the MB collar of the honeybee and Cataglyphis, visual projection neurons from the medulla of the optic lobe form very large presynaptic boutons comprising many (~ 60) active zones (synaptic contacts) connected to numerous dendrites of postsynaptic Kenyon cells (the MB intrinsic neurons) forming microglomerular synaptic complexes (Groh et al. 2012; Stieb et al. 2010, 2012).
While both the CX and MB visual pathways comprise large synaptic complexes at the input of their target neuropils including many postsynaptic contacts, there is a significant difference between the two circuits (Fig. 2d). Whereas the total number of microglomerular synapses in the MB collars of both brain hemispheres was estimated at ~ 400,000 in Cataglyphis, only ~ 100 microglomerular synapses are implemented in the bulbs of both hemispheres, the terminal synaptic relay stations in the CX pathway (reviewed in Grob et al. 2019; Rössler 2019). The enormously large and highly divergent synaptic matrix in the MB collar stands in contrast to the CX pathway converging on only ~ 100 synaptic complexes on both sides. Increasing evidence from physiological studies supports the function of the CX pathway as the crucial sky-compass pathway transferring relevant skylight cues including light polarization in the UV range, sun position, and UV-green chromatic gradients (el Jundi et al. 2014; Homberg et al. 2011). For the MB pathway, the enormous capacity of the MB collars for the storage of view-based panoramic information is further supported by modeling studies (Ardin et al. 2016; Peng and Chittka 2017) and by the above-mentioned ablation studies.
Sensory projections from Johnston’s organ
The important role of magnetic information for goal-directed views during learning walks of Cataglyphis nodus (Fleischmann et al. 2018b) provokes the question of how magnetic information is integrated with input from the two visual pathways described above. The hymenopteran antenna has repeatedly been proposed as a promising candidate for the potential location of a magnetic sensor (reviewed in Fleischmann et al. 2020b; Wajnberg et al. 2010; and study by de Oliveira et al. 2010) with Johnston’s organ (JO) representing a potential candidate. The JO is located at the base of the insect antenna and was shown to function primarily as a mechanosensory organ with highly sensitive chordotonal sensilla inserted into a delicate membrane at the joint between the antennal pedicellus and flagellum (for a review see Yack 2004). The number of sensilla varies between species and depends on the main function of the organ. In general, the JO serves the detection of antennal deflections caused by wind, gravity, vibration (air- and substrate-borne) or touch (Kamikouchi et al. 2009). Interestingly, during learning walks, Cataglyphis ants perform very characteristic antennal movements (Wehner et al. 2004, and own unpublished observations), which may suggest that specific antennal movement patterns are necessary to receive the magnetic information in an active sensing process (Fleischmann et al. 2020b).
A recent anatomical study mapped the sensilla and associated sensory structures (scolopidia) of the JO in the antenna of Cataglyphis nodus workers, queens, and males (Grob et al. 2021a). In workers, about 40 scolopidia, each comprising three sensory neurons, project axons to the antennal mechanosensory and motor center and extend sensory afferents to neuropils in the posterior brain including the saddle, ventrolateral protocerebrum, ventral complex, and the posterior slope (Grob et al. 2021a; Habenstein et al. 2020). The overall architecture of the JO in Cataglyphis shows many similarities with the JO in the honeybee (Ai et al. 2007). Most interestingly, in both Cataglyphis and the honeybee, the posterior slope also receives projections from interneurons of the ocelli that are in close apposition with sensory terminals from JO afferents. In addition, in the ventrolateral protocerebrum, ventral complex, and posterior slope neuronal projections from the optic lobes via the posterior and inferior optic tracts converge with sensory afferents from the JO (Grob et al. 2021a; Habenstein et al. 2020). Connectome studies in Drosophila (Scheffer et al. 2020) revealed that visual input from the optic lobe in these regions is preferentially from the lobula. Importantly, the posterior slope together with the lateral accessory lobe and ventrolateral protocerebrum contain post-synaptic compartments of descending neurons relaying motor commands to the ventral nerve cord (Currier and Nagel 2020; Hsu and Bhandawat 2016; Namiki et al. 2018a). This classifies these neuropils as premotor centers. In Drosophila, many descending neurons are bimodal and respond to mechanosensory and visual input (Namiki et al. 2018b).
Before we draw further conclusions on the role of multisensory convergences in these neuropils in the ant’s central brain, the next chapter reviews the results from experimental manipulations of sensory input during the performance of learning walks in the natural habitat, particularly their consequences for neuroplastic changes along the two visual pathways described above.
The role of multisensory experience in rewiring visual circuits
Recent studies have applied various manipulations of sensory input during learning walks in the natural habitat and subsequently screened the brains of the experimental ants for neuroplastic changes in visual circuits along the CX and MB pathways (Grob et al. 2019). For a behavioral readout, high-resolution video analyses of the pirouetting behavior with different stopping phases during learning walks were used as a reliable tool to test the ants’ information about the direction of the nest entrance and as an indicator of their path-integration abilities under the various experimental conditions (Fleischmann et al. 2017, 2018b, 2022; Grob et al. 2017). Subsequently in the laboratory, structural neuroplasticity along the CX and MB visual pathways served as a readout (or measure) for the role of sensory experience in triggering neuroplastic changes or calibrations in visual circuits (Grob et al. 2017, 2019, 2022; Schmitt et al. 2016; Stieb et al. 2010, 2012). Structural neuroplasticity was quantified either as changes in neuropil volume (CX and MB) or as variations in the number of synaptic complexes (MBs and bulbs of the lateral complex).
Before we bring together the outcome of these studies, let us first consider the nature of the different types of visual stimulation by differentiating two forms of experience—light exposure in the absence of learning-walk behavior (in the following termed “light exposure”) and learning-related visual experience during learning walks (in the following termed “learning walk”). Naïve ants experience light for the first time when they reach a point in their natural ontogeny where they approach the nest entrance for the first time from the inside of the nest and when ants, for example during nest-building activities, perform so-called short digging walks to expel material from the interior of the nest to the outside (Fleischmann et al. 2017; Wehner 2020; Zeil and Fleischmann 2019). In contrast, view-based sensory perception while performing pirouettes with goal-directed stops during learning walks may be regarded as an active learning process, and therefore, best characterized as learning-related experience. In addition to visual cues, the behavioral analyses have shown that the ants use the earth’s magnetic field as a compass reference for path integration during initial learning walks. If we assume that path integration combined with the experience of a nest-directed view, the homing direction, may function as an internal reward, we can assign this type of learning to a form of associative (Hebbian) learning and plasticity. Contrary to this, passive light exposure during an experimental situation where naïve ants do not (yet) perform leaning walks can be considered as non-associative sensory exposure. Consequently, the two types of experimental conditions for visual input were classified as light-exposure and learning-walk related visual experience. However, we must keep in mind that under natural conditions, the first combined experience of skylight and magnetic field may already happen during digging walks, or even earlier, when the ants sit in the nest entrance for the first time. Therefore, both behaviors may comprise transitions between the two forms of sensory experience.
Structural neuroplasticity in visual pathways triggered by first light exposure
Effects of light exposure have been tested in young ants that had not yet been outside the dark nest. When young Cataglyphis ants were precociously exposed to light, microglomerular synaptic complexes in both the CX and MB visual pathways exhibited structural plasticity (Schmitt et al. 2016; Stieb et al. 2010, 2012) (Fig. 3). Repeated application of 45 min light pulses over a period of four to five days resulted in a decrease in the number of microglomeruli in the visual compartments of the MB calyx, an indication of synaptic pruning. Interestingly, in the MB collar such light-exposure dependent synaptic changes could still be triggered in six- and twelve-month old ants that had been kept in constant darkness. This suggests that this type of plasticity adapts visual processing to different light conditions throughout the entire lifespan of the ants (Stieb et al. 2010). Similarly, precocious visual exposure in honeybees (Scholl et al. 2014) and olfactory sensory exposure to diverse plant odors in leaf-cutting ants over three to five days (Falibene et al. 2015) resulted in a decrease in the numbers of microglomeruli (pruning) in the MB collar and lip, respectively. Light exposure was also shown to induce changes in the expression of neuroplasticity-related genes in the honeybee (Becker et al. 2016).
Contrary to a decrease of microglomeruli in the MB collar following light exposure, the number of microglomerular synaptic complexes in the bulbs of the CX pathway showed an increase by up to 30% after first light exposure, even when applied to one-day old ants (Grob et al. 2022; Schmitt et al. 2016) (Fig. 3). At a first glance, the diverging effects on microglomerular synapses in the two pathways look contradictory. The reason for this difference likely resides in the function, particularly different neurotransmitter systems involved in the respective neuronal circuits. In both cases, structural synaptic plasticity was observed in microglomerular synaptic complexes comprising a large presynaptic bouton and numerous postsynaptic profiles (Rössler 2019). While visual projection neurons in the MB collar in Cataglyphis are most likely cholinergic and excitatory, tangential neurons that relay the information from bulb microglomeruli to the lower unit (ellipsoid body) of the central body in the CX are GABAergic (Schmitt et al. 2016). This feature is highly conserved in other insects (e.g. locust, Träger et al. 2008, and Drosophila, Seelig and Jayaraman 2015). If we consider the structural synaptic changes following first exposure to bright sunlight as a form of homeostatic plasticity that helps to maintain neuronal activity in a dynamic range, we can expect a reduction of excitatory synapses in the MB collars (synaptic pruning) and an increase of inhibitory (GABAergic) synapses in the bulb at the entrance to the CX. However, the exact function of this plasticity at the input remains to be determined as neuronal activity in both the MB and CX is also modulated through internal inhibitory feedback. Normalization of activity in the MB calyx is achieved through a set of GABAergic feedback neurons in the honeybee and the APL neuron in Drosophila (Grünewald 1999; Haehnel and Menzel 2012; Prisco et al. 2021). Normalization of activity in the ellipsoid body (input of the CX from the bulb) is controlled by CX local inhibition (Kim et al. 2017). A possible explanation might be that the intrinsic inhibitory circuits act on a smaller dynamic range compared to the changes at the input synapses, which may be required during the drastic changes in sensory input during the interior-exterior transition. Future synaptic connectivity studies at the ultrastructural level, especially of convergence-divergence ratios, are necessary to further determine the role of structural synaptic changes in both circuits in homeostatic plasticity.
Interestingly, the increase of microglomerular synaptic complexes in the bulbs of the CX pathway is light-quality dependent and especially sensitive to the presence of UV light (Grob et al. 2022; Schmitt et al. 2016). No structural changes were found in both the MB and CX pathways when artificially dark-kept ants were passively exposed to either a static or moving pattern of polarized light, even with the UV spectrum included (Grob et al. 2022). Similarly, no changes were found after passive light exposure regarding the overall volume of the MB calyx and CX neuropils. This shows that one or three days of passive light exposure were not sufficient to induce a decrease (pruning) of synaptic complexes in the MB collar (Stieb et al. 2012; Grob et al. 2022). We conclude that passive exposure to skylight over more than three days triggers a form of most likely homeostatic synaptic plasticity at the input to the CX and MB. In the CX pathway, the degree of change depends on the chromatic composition of the light stimulus.
Structural neuroplasticity in visual pathways triggered by learning walks
The study by Grob et al. (2017) had shown that the ants were still able to perform the turn back towards the nest entrance behavior while pirouetting during learning walks when UV light and the position of the sun were blocked (Grob et al. 2017). The structural changes in the volume and/or numbers of synaptic complexes in the CX and MB visual pathways were used as an experimental readout to quantify the effects of different experimental conditions. The resulting structural plasticity in the ants’ brains was quite distinct between passive light exposure and following visual experience during learning walks (Stieb et al. 2010; Grob et al. 2017, 2022) (Figs. 3 and 4).
In the bulbs of the CX pathway, the number of microglomerular synaptic complexes remained unchanged following all conditions that had been applied during learning walks. As the total number of synaptic complexes in the bulbs was already high compared to dark-kept naïve ants, this supports the idea that plasticity in bulb synaptic complexes occurs before the performance of learning walks (Fig. 3). This most likely happens when the ants approach the nest entrance for the first time and while performing digging walks close to the nest entrance. Most importantly, the volumes of the CX and MB calyx including the numbers of microglomeruli in the MB calyx collar increased, but only when the ants performed learning walks under natural skylight (Grob et al. 2017, 2022) (Fig. 4). The brains of ants that had been excluded from UV light or that had performed learning walks under a static linear polarization pattern were not different from those that had remained in constant darkness or had been treated by light exposure without the performance of learning walks. This suggests that learning-walk dependent plasticity requires the active performance of learning-walk behavior and, at the same time, access to natural skylight. A more sophisticated follow-up experiment narrowed down the most relevant skylight cue (Grob et al. 2022). In the experiment, the ants performed learning walks under a special setup mounted above the nest entrance comprising an automatically rotating linear polarizer. The results show that only ants that had experienced a rotating UV sky-polarization pattern responded with a volume increase in the CX and an increase of microglomeruli in the MB calyx collar (Fig. 4). Interestingly, a rotating linear polarizer was sufficient to induce this effect. Most importantly, already a single 90° rotation per day was sufficient to trigger structural plasticity in both CX and MB circuits. An hourly rotation by 45° increased this effect, but both the linear rotation by 15°/h or unrestricted exposure to the natural skylight did not lead to any significant further increase of neuroplastic changes. Most interestingly, under all experimental conditions (except for the dark kept control group) the ants were able to perceive the natural course of the sun (as a celestial body) across the sky. This means that the rotating sky-polarization pattern is the necessary and most important cue for inducing learning-walk related neuroplasticity in both the CX and MB pathways. The moving sun alone is not sufficient to induce this effect. As could be expected, under all conditions the number of bulb synapses in the CX pathway remained unchanged after learning walks (not shown) (Grob et al. 2022).
An interesting outcome of the experiments described above was that neuroplasticity triggered by a dynamic sky-polarization pattern did also occur in the MBs (Grob et al. 2022) (Fig. 4). It seems likely that UV information is transferred to the MBs, but this has not yet been shown for Cataglyphis ants. In the honeybee, for example, MB output neurons show a sensitive response to UV light (Schmalz et al. 2022; Strube-Bloss and Rössler 2018). However, it is not known whether the MBs receive any direct input from polarization-sensitive neurons. The results on plasticity in the MB collar mediated by a rotating polarization pattern indicate that information about sky polarization must somehow be relayed to the MBs, whether via a direct or indirect route needs to be explored in the future.
Another important aspect is that Cataglyphis ants perform several learning walks over at least two days (Fleischmann et al. 2016, 2017; Freas et al. 2019). This period correlates well with the time required to induce structural neuronal changes in high-order sensory integration centers. This is also the case for structural synaptic plasticity in the MB calyx associated with the formation of stable long-term memories after associative learning, which, for example, has been shown for both long-term appetitive olfactory memory in the honeybee (Hourcade et al. 2010; Scholl et al. 2015) and for long-term aversive olfactory memory in leaf cutting ants (Falibene et al. 2015). In the same line, light-exposure related (most likely homeostatic) structural neuroplasticity in the MB pathway of both the honeybee and ant requires at least three days of light exposure (Scholl et al. 2014; Stieb et al. 2012). Both associative and non-associative plasticity related structural neuronal changes almost certainly involve protein-synthesis dependent processes and the necessary activation of gene expression and subsequent protein synthesis is likely to involve a minimum of 24 h (Becker et al. 2016).
If we combine the results on the effects of a rotating sky polarization pattern with the known necessity for the experience of the earth’s magnetic field for path integration and goal-directed views during learning walks (Fleischmann et al. 2018b), we can conclude that both geostable compass information from the earth’s magnetic field together with a rotating UV sky-polarization pattern are necessary prerequisites to induce structural neuroplasticity in the CX and MB during initial learning walks. Considering the earlier results on the role of initial learning walks and re-learning walks in memorizing the panoramic scenery (Deeti and Cheng 2021; Fleischmann et al. 2016, 2018a; Jayatilaka et al. 2018; Müller and Wehner 2010; Wystrach et al. 2014a, b), this means that sensory reception of the earth’s magnetic field, access to the panoramic scenery, and the experience of a dynamic (rotating) sky-polarization pattern over a minimum period of two days and with a minimum distance of 0.5 m are necessary for successful learning walks. Although neuroplasticity in both the CX and MB strongly suggests that the experience of a dynamic sky-polarization pattern during learning walks is crucial for acquiring or calibrating an internal representation of the solar ephemeris, this hypothesis still needs further behavioral and functional proof.
Conclusions, open questions, and outlook
The results from visual manipulation studies during learning walks unambiguously demonstrate the role of celestial input, particularly a dynamic sky-polarization pattern, for inducing learning-related rewiring of visual neuronal circuits in the CX and MB. In addition, synaptic changes in the CX and MB pathways of naïve ants also occur after passive light exposure, most likely reflecting homeostatic plasticity before the start of learning walks. In the MBs, this form of plasticity can still be induced in old ants that have been kept in darkness. As the earth’s magnetic field is an important compass cue for goal-directed views during initial learning walks, the most obvious follow-up experiments will be long-term manipulations of the magnetic field during learning walks under natural skylight conditions and their consequences for neuroplastic calibrations in the visual pathways. The most likely outcome of this experiment is that plastic changes are absent when the magnetic field is eliminated or strongly disturbed and, therefore, not available as a compass reference during learning walks.
Many other questions and follow-up studies come to mind. For example, what is the sensory mechanism for magnetoreception in Cataglyphis? A recent review (Fleischmann et al. 2020b) has elaborated extensively on this question and put forward arguments favoring a magnetic particle-based mechanism that would enable light-independent and polarity-sensitive perception of the earth’s magnetic field. A study on the honeybee (Lambinet et al. 2017) provided evidence for polarity-sensitive (most likely magnetic-particle based) magnetoreception, at least in an artificial experimental setup. Systematic manipulation experiments in Cataglyphis using a 3D Helmholtz coil system can now test this hypothesis in the natural habitat using high-speed video analyses of nest-directed views during learning-walk pirouettes as a readout.
Where are the magnetoreceptors located, and what is the neuronal pathway mediating magnetosensation? Where in the brain does the information converge with visual input? Several lines of evidence have promoted the idea that the ants’ antennae might be sites for magnetoreception (de Oliveira et al. 2010; Fleischmann et al. 2020b; Wajnberg et al. 2010). The JO is a potential candidate and has recently been studied anatomically—both the peripheral structures in the receptor organ and afferent projections in the ant brain (Grob et al. 2021a). Each JO scolopidium comprises three receptor neurons, and sensory afferents from the JO in Cataglyphis converge with visual input from the ocelli and the optic lobes in posterior brain regions, the ventrolateral protocerebrum, ventral complex, saddle, and posterior slope (Grob et al. 2021a; Habenstein et al. 2020). Future tracking of antennal movements during magnetic manipulation experiments may give further hints to the potential role of the antennae in magnetoreception (Fleischmann et al. 2020b).
Further important questions come up regarding the kind of navigational information that is acquired during learning walks and how subsequent navigational performance is disturbed by visual and magnetic manipulations during learning walks. What are the behavioral consequences after manipulations of specific sky compass cues and/or the magnetic field during learning walks concerning view-based spatial orientation and the precision in the calibration of the internal representation of the solar ephemeris in foragers? Some of the required behavioral experiments are straightforward, but others are more difficult as they require follow-up studies on individually marked ants and/or experiments employing several experimental groups under the same treatment and with the necessary controls for parallel behavioral and neurobiological analyses.
It will be highly interesting and important, but also most difficult, to test whether ants experiencing manipulations during learning walks that prevent structural neuroplasticity in the CX and MB also fail in navigational tasks that require a time-compensated skylight compass. Cataglyphis fortis inhabits featureless environments in North African salt flats without or with less prominent landmarks or panoramic sceneries. The accessibility of “test fields” devoid of a prominent visual panorama in this environment provides ideal conditions for performing experiments to answer this question. It is very interesting in this context that previous high-resolution analyses of learning walk behavior in closely related C. fortis, C. nodus, and C. aenescens have shown that only C. fortis does not perform pirouettes (rotations on the spot about the vertical body axis including stops with nest-directed gazes) during learning walks (Fleischmann et al. 2017). Unlike the other two Cataglyphis species, C. fortis exclusively performs voltes—small, walked circles, with only rare and less distinct stops—but not pirouettes. This fuels the hypothesis that pirouettes with goal-directed stops and views are important for panoramic snapshot learning, whereas voltes might serve celestial compass calibration. Ants in cluttered habitats would then be able to efficiently perform both tasks at the same time—a collection of panoramic views, and calibration of the celestial compass—by performing two distinct behavioral routines for actively exploring both sets of visual parameters. However, close quantitative comparisons with similar body rotations that have been observed in other ant species (e.g., Deeti and Cheng 2021; Jayatilaka et al. 2018; Müller and Wehner 2010; Wystrach et al. 2014a, b) are needed to substantiate this hypothesis and to clarify the role of different types of body rotations during learning walks. Interestingly, dung beetles occasionally perform rotational movements on their dung ball to acquire celestial snapshots for strait-line orientation (el Jundi et al. 2016). This implies that the employment of body rotations for visual snapshot learning and skylight compass orientation may be based on behavioral routines that are conserved over phylogenetically large scales.
Do additional sensory modalities affect learning-walk experience? From the behavioral experiments and analyses of neuroplasticity described above, we can conclude that learning walks at least require the multisensory experience of dynamic sky polarization cues, magnetosensory information, and (if available) panoramic cues. Previous studies have shown that a minimum requirement of time (at least 2 days) and space (at least 0.5 m distance from the nest entrance) are required to learn the panoramic scenery around the nest (Fleischmann et al. 2016, 2018a). However, in addition to vision and magnetoreception, we cannot exclude that input from other sensory modalities come into play during initial learning walks. In Cataglyphis, olfactory orientation, especially in response to CO2, but also other odorants, is important during the final nest approach after homing and for exploring olfactory landscapes close to the nest site, which is combined with visual information (Buehlmann et al. 2012, 2014; Steck et al. 2009, 2011). Furthermore, when the ants screen the desert for finding food, they can form long-lasting olfactory memories for a substantial number of different odorants (Huber and Knaden 2018). Ants of the genus Cataglyphis possess well developed antennal lobes containing more than 200 olfactory glomeruli, the functional units for odorant processing (Grob et al. 2021b; Stieb et al. 2011). Based on this, it is conceivable that the ants also explore the olfactory landscape around the nest during their initial learning walks. Finally, wind orientation and the neuronal underpinnings of a wind compass have been explored in Cataglyphis and, for example, in the dung beetle and Drosophila (Dacke et al. 2019; Okubo et al. 2020; Wehner 2020). In some desert habitats, like North African salt flats, wind direction can be quite constant or even predictable, at least over some time periods (Wehner 2020). Much like in Drosophila, the JO in the antenna is likely to play an important role in using wind direction as a compass cue in Cataglyphis (Grob et al. 2021a). This is also supported by the demonstration of the transformation of wind perception to celestial compass coordinates (Wystrach and Schwarz 2013). Future studies are needed to characterize the importance of olfactory and mechanosensory modalities during initial learning walks.
Input from the sky compass requires time compensation, most likely by linking an internal (learned) function of the solar ephemeris to the internal clock (Wehner 2020; Wehner and Müller 1993). Neurons expressing the neuropeptide Pigment-Dispersing Factor (PDF) play important roles in neuronal clock networks of Drosophila and are potential candidates relaying day-time information to neuronal processing centers involved in visual navigation (Helfrich-Förster 2005; Helfrich-Förster et al. 2020). A recent study revealed a broad distribution of PDF neurons in the honeybee brain and suggested their potential role in circadian rhythmicity (Beer et al. 2018). Another study on the honeybee has identified the optic lobes as a potential site of modulatory input from PDF neurons (Zeller et al. 2015). It will be highly interesting for future studies to investigate PDF neurons and the presence of clock-related neurons in the Cataglyphis brain, particularly their association with visual processing and integration centers. In principle, time compensation via modulatory influences from the endogenous circadian clock could be implemented both upstream or downstream the CX and MB.
Working model for learning-walk related multisensory input and neuroplasticity
We propose a working model for multisensory input during learning walks and related rewiring of sensory pathways in the Cataglyphis brain (Fig. 5). In the scheme, color-coded solid lines indicate the known sensory pathways for visual input from the compound eyes via the optic lobes and the ocelli, mechanosensory (and potentially magnetosensory) input from the JO of the antennae, and olfactory input from the antennae. In addition to connections between brain neuropils known from studies in Cataglyphis (Grob et al. 2017, 2021a; Habenstein et al. 2020; Schmitt et al. 2016) (solid lines), connections between brain neuropils known from other insects are included, particularly from recent connectome studies in Drosophila, tracing studies in moths, or preliminary results in Cataglyphis (in all cases indicated by dashed lines in Fig. 5). For obvious reasons, this scheme is far from being complete. Its main purpose is to provide a ground for discussions and future planning for combinations of behavioral experiments, neuroanatomical and neurophysiological analyses, modeling studies, and functional studies on neuroplasticity. This also concerns studies on multisensory convergence in brain neuropils that have not been looked at so far and analyses on rewiring of circuits during the early ontogeny of foraging behavior (either measured as volume changes or, ideally, structural synaptic plasticity in identifiable circuits). Neuroplastic changes in brain neuropils that have already been demonstrated are indicated as magenta (learning-walk related) and yellow (sensory-exposure related) borders highlighting neuropils in Fig. 5. Due to the difficult neurophysiological access (e.g. electrophysiology or functional imaging) in ants, Fig. 5 is also intended to stimulate comparative studies in Drosophila and other insect models or modeling studies inspired by data on behavioral and neuronal plasticity in Cataglyphis. In the following, we will mainly focus on convergences of neuronal pathways that are likely to play a role in neuroplasticity triggered by multisensory learning during learning walks and sensory exposure prior to learning walks.
Connections between the MB and CX pathways
One obvious question is how information from panoramic view memories in the MBs is integrated with heading and path integration information processed in CX circuits (Seelig and Jayaraman 2015; Stone et al. 2017). Possible solutions have been proposed in models by Collett and Collett (2018), Sun et al. (2020), and Wystrach et al. (2020). This is not only important for view memories, but also for connecting olfactory memories to path integration information in the CX. Recent studies in Drosophila, elegantly shown by transsynaptic staining or in serial electron microscopy connectome studies, revealed that a substantial number of MB output neurons are synaptically connected with fan shaped body neurons of the CX, mainly in the superior medial protocerebrum (Hulse et al. 2021; Li et al. 2020a; Scaplen et al. 2021). This confirms earlier predictions by Collett and Collett (2018) or in a model by Hoinville and Wehner (2018) (Fig. 5, dashed line between MB and CX). MB output neurons were shown to express learning-related changes and categorize multisensory input in the honeybee, and to exhibit changes in aversive and appetitive responses to odorants after olfactory conditioning in Drosophila, in this case, independent of the original valence of the odorants (Hige et al. 2015; Owald and Waddell 2015; Strube-Bloss et al. 2011; Strube-Bloss and Rössler 2018). The activation of responses in ‘aversive’ and ‘attractive’ MB output neurons might modulate navigational decisions in CX circuits via direct interactions with fan-shaped body neurons. The combination of aversive and attractive MB output in ant navigation was suggested in recent behavioral analyses and modeling approaches (Le Möel & Wystrach 2020; Murray et al. 2020). How such learning-dependent modulatory interactions may influence behavioral decisions at a mechanistic level will be a most interesting focus for future investigations. In the same context, it is highly interesting that the CX fan-shaped body of Cataglyphis ants is innervated by a large variety of neuropeptidergic neurons (Habenstein et al. 2021a, 2021b; Schmitt et al. 2017). These neurons may convey input from further modulatory systems that mediate internal age- or status-dependent changes, and external context-dependent influences.
Studies in the honeybee have shown that more than 30% of MB output neurons are multimodal and respond to both olfactory and visual input (Strube-Bloss and Rössler 2018). This means that both modalities converge at the level of the MB output. Therefore, panoramic view memories might interact with olfactory input at the level of the MBs and, for example, lead to cross-modal interactions during learning as it has been shown for color learning and olfaction (Becker et al. 2019; Schmalz et al. 2022; Strube-Bloss and Rössler 2018). Computation of path integration information in the CX and view-based information in the MBs have recently also been implemented in models for optimal integration of navigational information (Hoinville and Wehner 2018; Sun et al. 2020). Interestingly, long-term memory after color learning was also shown to induce structural plasticity in the fan-shaped body of the central complex of Camponotus ants (Yilmaz et al. 2019).
In Cataglyphis and, for example, in the honeybee, massive visual input is relayed from the optic lobes to the collar of the MB calyx, whereas in the fly only very few direct projections from the optic lobes mediate visual input to the MB calyx (Ehmer and Gronenberg 2002; Grob et al. 2017; Habenstein et al. 2020; Li et al. 2020b; Vogt et al. 2016; Yilmaz et al. 2016, 2019). The differences in visual innervation patterns of the MB calyces between basal and higher hymenopteran species (Farris and Schulmeister 2011) and the small number of visual connections to the MB calyx in Drosophila can be expected to cause differences in the way how visually activated MB output neurons may affect CX processing. These circuit differences reflect different sensory ecologies between insect species. It also emphasizes the general importance of comparative studies on multimodal information processing (Thiagarajan and Sachse 2022) and the role of differences in visual pathways on visual behaviors (Ryu et al. 2022).
Like in other Hymenoptera, olfactory information in Cataglyphis is relayed from the antennal lobe to the MB and lateral horn via multiple antennal lobe tracts (Kirschner et al. 2006). During learning walk behavior, in addition to memorizing the panoramic scenery, Cataglyphis might acquire additional homing information by learning olfactory landmarks around the nest entrance. During homing, much like view-based information, olfactory memories might be relayed to the CX via olfactory MB output neurons and influence navigational decisions. The connections of MB output neurons with the fan shaped body (also termed upper unit of the central body) suggests a prominent role of this part of the CX in multisensory integration and modulation of spatial orientation.
Convergence of visual pathways with input from Johnston’s organ
Where does information from the JO converge with visual information and how could it be relayed to the CX? Interestingly, in both the honeybee and Cataglyphis, projections in the posterior slope are in very close apposition with input from ocellar interneurons (Ai et al. 2007; Grob et al. 2021a). Furthermore, anatomical and behavioral evidence suggest that the ocelli mediate information about celestial compass information in desert ants (Penmetcha et al. 2019; Schwarz et al. 2011). The convergence of sensory pathways from the ocelli and JO provides further evidence that the posterior slope represents a multimodal integration center (Currier and Nagel 2020). In Cataglyphis, this brain region likely integrates visual input from the ocelli, mechanosensory input (wind compass, gravity) from the JO, and, although still speculative, potentially magnetosensory information from the JO (Fleischmann et al. 2020b; Grob et al. 2021a). Furthermore, projections from the optic lobes are relayed to the posterior slope, ventrolateral protocerebrum, and ventral complex via the posterior and inferior optic commissures indicating that visual information from the compound eyes, too, may converge with multisensory input from the JO (Habenstein et al. 2020). Although for Cataglyphis it is still an open question how information from the posterior slope and ventrolateral neuropils might be relayed to the CX, several pathways shown in Drosophila provide potential candidates. For example, connections of auditory circuits from the JO to the CX (Lai et al. 2012) and for wind compass information from the anterior mechanosensory and motor center to the CX (Okubo et al. 2020). Similarly, studies in the locust and Drosophila revealed connections between the CX and lateral accessory lobe with the posterior slope and other neuropils in the central brain (Hadeln et al. 2020; Scheffer et al. 2020). Such feedback connections in Cataglyphis might be relevant for plasticity in CX neuropils observed during the active performance of learning walks under a rotating sky-polarization pattern, while the ants use the earth’s magnetic field as a compass reference (Fleischmann et al. 2018b; Grob et al. 2017, 2022). Although the posterior neuropils and central brain are not as easy to access as the CX, the bulbs, or the MBs, future studies on learning-walk induced structural neuroplasticity should also analyze these brain centers. Clearly, future tracing studies are needed to anatomically characterize the connections between these neuropils including their neurotransmitter and neuromodulator systems. The Cataglyphis brain atlas will be a helpful tool for such experiments (Habenstein et al. 2020). Furthermore, recent studies have revealed stage-related changes in neuropeptide modulators (e.g. Corazonin, Allatostatin) in distinct neuropils or brain regions of the Cataglyphis brain (Habenstein et al. 2021a, 2021b). Future studies should also characterize the neurochemistry associated with multisensory interactions and analyze potential changes in neurotransmitters and neuromodulators. As an alternative approach to live imaging techniques like calcium imaging, activity- or learning-related changes in the expression of immediate early genes in relevant brain neuropils receiving convergent multisensory input may give important insights into neuronal effects of learning-walk behavior (Sommerlandt et al. 2017, 2019).
Pathways to premotor centers
The connections between the output from the CX and MBs to premotor centers have not yet been investigated in Cataglyphis ants. Clearly, future tracing studies are necessary to characterize these pathways. However, as the CX circuitry appears to be highly conserved across insect taxa (Strausfeld 2012), we can at least make some predictions from the connections already known in other insects (Steinbeck et al. 2020). The CX output is mainly relayed to the lateral accessory lobe (Pfeiffer and Homberg 2014). The lateral accessory lobe, posterior slope and ventrolateral protocerebrum can be viewed as premotor control centers as they are targeted by descending neurons that relay the information to thoracic motor control centers (Namiki et al. 2018a, 2018b; Namiki and Kanzaki 2016) (dashed black lines in Fig. 5). Therefore, in addition to information that is relayed to the CX, it appears likely that input from the JO may also exert direct modulatory influences on visually mediated responses via these descending pathways, for example by modulatory interactions in the posterior slope, ventrolateral protocerebrum, or even downstream of these neuropils. These aspects require future studies at the synaptic and ultrastructural levels. There are no indications that information from the JO is relayed to the MBs, but interactions with MB output neurons may be possible at the level of their connections with downstream neuropils like the lateral accessory lobe. These open questions related to processing of multisensory input and the role of learning and memory during the performance of learning walk behavior in Cataglyphis ants represent a most exciting field for multidisciplinary neuroethological research aimed at elucidating the ontogeny of spatial orientation in a highly skilled insect navigator.
Data availability
If applicable, figures in this review paper refer to the data provided in the original research publications.
References
Ai H, Nishino H, Itoh T (2007) Topographic organization of sensory afferents of Johnston’s organ in the honeybee brain. J Comp Neurol 502:1030–1046. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21341
Ardin P, Peng F, Mangan M, Lagogiannis K, Webb B (2016) Using an insect mushroom body circuit to encode route memory in complex natural environments. PLoS Comput Biol 12:e1004683. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1004683
Becker N, Kucharski R, Rössler W, Maleszka R (2016) Age-dependent transcriptional and epigenomic responses to light exposure in the honey bee brain. FEBS Open Bio 6:622–639. https://doi.org/10.1002/2211-5463.12084
Becker MC, Rössler W, Strube-Bloss MF (2019) UV light perception is modulated by the odour element of an olfactory-visual compound in restrained honeybees. J Exp Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.201483
Beer K, Kolbe E, Kahana NB, Yayon N, Weiss R, Menegazzi P, Bloch G, Helfrich-Förster C (2018) Pigment-dispersing factor-expressing neurons convey circadian information in the honey bee brain. Open Biol. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsob.170224
Buehlmann C, Hansson BS, Knaden M (2012) Path integration controls nest-plume following in desert ants. Curr Biol 22:645–649. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.02.029
Buehlmann C, Graham P, Hansson BS, Knaden M (2014) Desert ants locate food by combining high sensitivity to food odors with extensive crosswind runs. Curr Biol 24:960–964. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.02.056
Buehlmann C, Wozniak B, Goulard R, Webb B, Graham P, Niven JE (2020) Mushroom bodies are required for learned visual navigation, but not for innate visual behavior, in ants. Curr Biol 30:3438-3443.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.07.013
Cheng K, Schultheiss P, Schwarz S, Wystrach A, Wehner R (2014) Beginnings of a synthetic approach to desert ant navigation. Behav Process 102:51–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.10.001
Collett M, Collett TS (2018) How does the insect central complex use mushroom body output for steering? Curr Biol 28:R733–R734. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.060
Currier TA, Nagel KI (2020) Multisensory control of navigation in the fruit fly. Curr Opin Neurobiol 64:10–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2019.11.017
Dacke M, Bell ATA, Foster JJ, Baird EJ, Strube-Bloss MF, Byrne MJ, el Jundi B (2019) Multimodal cue integration in the dung beetle compass. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 116:14248–14253. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1904308116
de Oliveira JF, Wajnberg E, Esquivel DMDS et al (2010) Ant antennae: are they sites for magnetoreception? J R Soc Interface 7:143–152. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2009.0102
Deeti S, Cheng K (2021) Learning walks in an Australian desert ant, Melophorus bagoti. J Exp Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.242177
Dreyer D, Frost B, Mouritsen H, Günther A, Green K, Whitehouse M, Johnsen S, Heinze S, Warrant E (2018) The earth’s magnetic field and visual landmarks steer migratory flight behavior in the nocturnal Australian bogong moth. Curr Biol 28:2160-2166.e5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.05.030
Ehmer B, Gronenberg W (2002) Segregation of visual input to the mushroom bodies in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J Comp Neurol 451:362–373. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.10355
el Jundi B, Pfeiffer K, Heinze S, Homberg U (2014) Integration of polarization and chromatic cues in the insect sky compass. J Comp Physiol 200:575–589. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0890-6
el Jundi B, Foster JJ, Khaldy L, Byrne MJ, Dacke M, Baird E (2016) A snapshot-based mechanism for celestial orientation. Curr Biol 26:1456–1462. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.03.030
Falibene A, Roces F, Rössler W (2015) Long-term avoidance memory formation is associated with a transient increase in mushroom body synaptic complexes in leaf-cutting ants. Front Behav Neurosci 9:84. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00084
Farris SM, Schulmeister S (2011) Parasitoidism, not sociality, is associated with the evolution of elaborate mushroom bodies in the brains of hymenopteran insects. Proc Biol Sci 278:940–951. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2161
Fleischmann PN, Christian M, Müller VL, Rössler W, Wehner R (2016) Ontogeny of learning walks and the acquisition of landmark information in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. J Exp Biol 219:3137–3145. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.140459
Fleischmann PN, Grob R, Wehner R, Rössler W (2017) Species-specific differences in the fine structure of learning walk elements in Cataglyphis ants. J Exp Biol 220:2426–2435. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.158147
Fleischmann PN, Rössler W, Wehner R (2018a) Early foraging life: spatial and temporal aspects of landmark learning in the ant Cataglyphis noda. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 204:579–592. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-018-1260-6
Fleischmann PN, Grob R, Müller VL, Wehner R, Rössler W (2018b) The geomagnetic field is a compass cue in Cataglyphis ant navigation. Curr Biol 28:1440-1444.e2. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2018.03.043
Fleischmann PN, Grob R, Rössler W (2020a) Kompass im Kopf. Biol Unserer Zeit. https://doi.org/10.1002/biuz.202070201
Fleischmann PN, Grob R, Rössler W (2020b) Magnetoreception in Hymenoptera: importance for navigation. Anim Cogn 23:1051–1061. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-020-01431-x
Fleischmann PN, Grob R, Rössler W (2022) Magnetosensation during re-learning walks in desert ants (Cataglyphis nodus). J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 208:125–133. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-021-01511-4
Freas CA, Fleischmann PN, Cheng K (2019) Experimental ethology of learning in desert ants: becoming expert navigators. Behav Process 158:181–191. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.001
Graham P, Philippides A, Baddeley B (2010) Animal cognition: multi-modal interactions in ant learning. Curr Biol 20:R639–R640. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.018
Grob R, Fleischmann PN, Grübel K, Wehner R, Rössler W (2017) The role of celestial compass information in Cataglyphis ants during learning walks and for neuroplasticity in the central complex and mushroom bodies. Front Behav Neurosci 11:226. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2017.00226
Grob R, Fleischmann PN, Rössler W (2019) Learning to navigate—how desert ants calibrate their compass systems. Neuroforum 25:109–120. https://doi.org/10.1515/nf-2018-0011
Grob R, Tritscher C, Grübel K, Stigloher C, Groh C, Fleischmann PN, Rössler W (2021a) Johnston’s organ and its central projections in Cataglyphis desert ants. J Comp Neurol 529:2138–2155. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25077
Grob R, Heinig N, Grübel K, Rössler W, Fleischmann PN (2021b) Sex-specific and caste-specific brain adaptations related to spatial orientation in Cataglyphis ants. J Comp Neurol 529:3882–3892. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25221
Grob R, el Jundi B, Fleischmann PN (2021c) Towards a common terminology for arthropod spatial orientation. Ethol Ecol Evol 33:338–358. https://doi.org/10.1080/03949370.2021.1905075
Grob R, Holland Cunz O, Grübel K, Pfeiffer K, Rössler W, Fleischmann PN (2022) Rotation of skylight polarization during learning walks is necessary to trigger neuronal plasticity in Cataglyphis ants. Proc Biol Sci 289:20212499. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2021.2499
Groh C, Lu Z, Meinertzhagen IA, Rössler W (2012) Age-related plasticity in the synaptic ultrastructure of neurons in the mushroom body calyx of the adult honeybee Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol 520:3509–3527. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23102
Gronenberg W (2008) Structure and function of ant (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) brains: strength in numbers. Myrmecol News 11:25–36
Gronenberg W, Hölldobler B (1999) Morphologic representation of visual and antennal information in the ant brain. J Comp Neurol 412:229–240. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19990920)412:2%3c229:AID-CNE4%3e3.0.CO;2-E
Grünewald B (1999) Physiological properties and response modulations of mushroom body feedback neuronsduring olfactory learning in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 185:565–576. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050417
Habenstein J, Amini E, Grübel K, el Jundi B, Rössler W (2020) The brain of Cataglyphis ants: neuronal organization and visual projections. J Comp Neurol 528:3479–3506. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24934
Habenstein J, Thamm M, Rössler W (2021a) Neuropeptides as potential modulators of behavioral transitions in the ant Cataglyphis nodus. J Comp Neurol 529:3155–3170. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25166
Habenstein J, Schmitt F, Liessem S, Ly A, Trede D, Wegener C, Predel R, Rössler W, Neupert S (2021b) Transcriptomic, peptidomic, and mass spectrometry imaging analysis of the brain in the ant Cataglyphis nodus. J Neurochem 158:391–412. https://doi.org/10.1111/jnc.15346
Haehnel M, Menzel R (2012) Long-term memory and response generalization in mushroom body extrinsic neurons in the honeybee Apis mellifera. J Exp Biol 215:559–565. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.059626
Heinze S, Homberg U (2007) Maplike representation of celestial E-vector orientations in the brain of an insect. Science 315:995–997. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1135531
Heinze S, Reppert SM (2011) Sun compass integration of skylight cues in migratory monarch butterflies. Neuron 69:345–358. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.12.025
Heinze S, el Jundi B, Berg BG, Homberg U, Menzel R, Pfeiffer K, Hensgen R, Zittrell F, Dacke M, Warrant E, Pfuhl G, Rybak J, Tedore K (2021) A unified platform to manage, share, and archive morphological and functional data in insect neuroscience. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.65376
Held M, Berz A, Hensgen R, Muenz TS, Scholl C, Rössler W, Homberg U, Pfeiffer K (2016) Microglomerular synaptic complexes in the sky-compass network of the honeybee connect parallel pathways from the anterior optic tubercle to the central complex. Front Behav Neurosci 10:186. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00186
Helfrich-Förster C (2005) Organization of endogenous clocks in insects. Biochem Soc Trans 33:957–961. https://doi.org/10.1042/BST20050957
Helfrich-Förster C, Bertolini E, Menegazzi P (2020) Flies as models for circadian clock adaptation to environmental challenges. Eur J Neurosci 51:166–181. https://doi.org/10.1111/ejn.14180
Hige T, Aso Y, Rubin GM, Turner GC (2015) Plasticity-driven individualization of olfactory coding in mushroom body output neurons. Nature 526:258–262. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature15396
Hoinville T, Wehner R (2018) Optimal multiguidance integration in insect navigation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:2824–2829. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1721668115
Homberg U, Heinze S, Pfeiffer K, Kinoshita M, el Jundi B (2011) Central neural coding of sky polarization in insects. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 366:680–687. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0199
Hourcade B, Muenz TS, Sandoz J-C, Rössler W, Devaud J-M (2010) Long-term memory leads to synaptic reorganization in the mushroom bodies: a memory trace in the insect brain? J Neurosci 30:6461–6465. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0841-10.2010
Hsu CT, Bhandawat V (2016) Organization of descending neurons in Drosophila melanogaster. Sci Rep 6:20259. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep20259
Huber R, Knaden M (2015) Egocentric and geocentric navigation during extremely long foraging paths of desert ants. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 201:609–616. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-015-0998-3
Huber R, Knaden M (2018) Desert ants possess distinct memories for food and nest odors. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 115:10470–10474. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1809433115
Hulse BK, Haberkern H, Franconville R, Turner-Evans DB, Takemura S-Y, Wolff T, Noorman M, Dreher M, Dan C, Parekh R, Hermundstad AM, Rubin GM, Jayaraman V (2021) A connectome of the Drosophila central complex reveals network motifs suitable for flexible navigation and context-dependent action selection. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.66039
Ito K, Shinomiya K, Ito M, Armstrong JD, Boyan G, Hartenstein V, Harzsch S, Heisenberg M, Homberg U, Jenett A, Keshishian H, Restifo LL, Rössler W, Simpson JH, Strausfeld NJ, Strauss R, Vosshall LB (2014) A systematic nomenclature for the insect brain. Neuron 81:755–765. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.12.017
Jayatilaka P, Murray T, Narendra A, Zeil J (2018) The choreography of learning walks in the Australian jack jumper ant Myrmecia croslandi. J Exp Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.185306
Kaiser A, Hensgen R, Tschirner K, Beetz E, Wüstenberg H, Pfaff M, Mota T, Pfeiffer K (2022) A three-dimensional atlas of the honeybee central complex, associated neuropils and peptidergic layers of the central body. J Comp Neurol 530:2416–2438. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.25339
Kamhi JF, Barron AB, Narendra A (2020) Vertical lobes of the mushroom bodies are essential for view-based navigation in Australian Myrmecia ants. Curr Biol 30:3432-3437.e3. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2020.06.030
Kamikouchi A, Inagaki HK, Effertz T, Hendrich O, Fiala A, Göpfert MC, Ito K (2009) The neural basis of Drosophila gravity-sensing and hearing. Nature 458:165–171. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature07810
Kim SS, Rouault H, Druckmann S, Jayaraman V (2017) Ring attractor dynamics in the Drosophila central brain. Science 356:849–853. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aal4835
Kirschner S, Kleineidam CJ, Zube C, Rybak J, Grünewald B, Rössler W (2006) Dual olfactory pathway in the honeybee, Apis mellifera. J Comp Neurol 499:933–952. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21158
Knaden M, Graham P (2016) The sensory ecology of ant navigation: from natural environments to neural mechanisms. Annu Rev Entomol 61:63–76. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-010715-023703
Lai JS-Y, Lo S-J, Dickson BJ, Chiang A-S (2012) Auditory circuit in the Drosophila brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 109:2607–2612. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1117307109
Lambinet V, Hayden ME, Reid C, Gries G (2017) Honey bees possess a polarity-sensitive magnetoreceptor. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 203:1029–1036. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-017-1214-4
Le Möel F, Wystrach A (2020) Opponent processes in visual memories: a model of attraction and repulsion in navigating insects’ mushroom bodies. PLoS Comput Biol 16:e1007631. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1007631
Li F, Lindsey JW, Marin EC, Otto N, Dreher M, Dempsey G, Stark I, Bates AS, Pleijzier MW, Schlegel P, Nern A, Takemura S-Y, Eckstein N, Yang T, Francis A, Braun A, Parekh R, Costa M, Scheffer LK, Aso Y, Jefferis GS, Abbott LF, Litwin-Kumar A, Waddell S, Rubin GM (2020a) The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.62576
Li J, Mahoney BD, Jacob MS, Caron SJC (2020b) Visual input into the Drosophila melanogaster mushroom body. Cell Rep 32:108138. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2020.108138
Müller M, Wehner R (2010) Path integration provides a scaffold for landmark learning in desert ants. Curr Biol 20:1368–1371. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2010.06.035
Murray T, Kócsi Z, Dahmen H, Narendra A, Le Möel F, Wystrach A, Zeil J (2020) The role of attractive and repellent scene memories in ant homing (Myrmecia croslandi). J Exp Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.210021
Namiki S, Kanzaki R (2016) Comparative neuroanatomy of the lateral accessory lobe in the insect brain. Front Physiol 7:244. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2016.00244
Namiki S, Wada S, Kanzaki R (2018a) Descending neurons from the lateral accessory lobe and posterior slope in the brain of the silkmoth Bombyx mori. Sci Rep 8:9663. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-018-27954-5
Namiki S, Dickinson MH, Wong AM, Korff W, Card GM (2018b) The functional organization of descending sensory-motor pathways in Drosophila. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.34272
Nicholson DJ, Judd SP, Cartwright BA, Collett TS (1999) Learning walks and landmark guidance in wood ants (Formica rufa). J Exp Biol 202(Pt 13):1831–1838. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.202.13.1831
Okubo TS, Patella P, D’Alessandro I, Wilson RI (2020) A Neural network for wind-guided compass navigation. Neuron 107:924-940.e18. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2020.06.022
Owald D, Waddell S (2015) Olfactory learning skews mushroom body output pathways to steer behavioral choice in Drosophila. Curr Opin Neurobiol 35:178–184. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2015.10.002
Peng F, Chittka L (2017) A Simple computational model of the bee mushroom body can explain seemingly complex forms of olfactory learning and memory. Curr Biol 27:224–230. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.10.054
Penmetcha B, Ogawa Y, Ribi WA, Narendra A (2019) Ocellar structure of African and Australian desert ants. J Comp Physiol A Neuroethol Sens Neural Behav Physiol 205:699–706. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-019-01357-x
Pfeiffer K, Homberg U (2014) Organization and functional roles of the central complex in the insect brain. Annu Rev Entomol 59:165–184. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-ento-011613-162031
Pisokas I, Rössler W, Webb B, Zeil J, Narendra A (2022) Anesthesia disrupts distance, but not direction, of path integration memory. Curr Biol 32:445-452.e4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2021.11.039
Prisco L, Deimel SH, Yeliseyeva H, Fiala A, Tavosanis G (2021) The anterior paired lateral neuron normalizes odour-evoked activity in the Drosophila mushroom body calyx. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.74172
Ronacher B (2008) Path integration as the basic navigation mechanism of the desert ant Cataglyphis fortis (FOREL, 1902) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecological 11:53–62
Rössler W (2019) Neuroplasticity in desert ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)—importance for the ontogeny of navigation. Myrmecol News 29:1–20. https://doi.org/10.25849/myrmecol.news_029:001
Ryu L, Kim SY, Kim AJ (2022) From photons to behaviors: neural implementations of visual behaviors in Drosophila. Front Neurosci 16:883640. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2022.883640
Scaplen KM, Talay M, Fisher JD, Cohn R, Sorkaç A, Aso Y, Barnea G, Kaun KR (2021) Transsynaptic mapping of Drosophila mushroom body output neurons. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.63379
Scheffer LK, Xu CS, Januszewski M, Lu Z, Takemura S-Y, Hayworth KJ, Huang GB, Shinomiya K, Maitlin-Shepard J, Berg S, Clements J, Hubbard PM, Katz WT, Umayam L, Zhao T, Ackerman D, Blakely T, Bogovic J, Dolafi T, Kainmueller D, Kawase T, Khairy KA, Leavitt L, Li PH, Lindsey L, Neubarth N, Olbris DJ, Otsuna H, Trautman ET, Ito M, Bates AS, Goldammer J, Wolff T, Svirskas R, Schlegel P, Neace E, Knecht CJ, Alvarado CX, Bailey DA, Ballinger S, Borycz JA, Canino BS, Cheatham N, Cook M, Dreher M, Duclos O, Eubanks B, Fairbanks K, Finley S, Forknall N, Francis A, Hopkins GP, Joyce EM, Kim S, Kirk NA, Kovalyak J, Lauchie SA, Lohff A, Maldonado C, Manley EA, McLin S, Mooney C, Ndama M, Ogundeyi O, Okeoma N, Ordish C, Padilla N, Patrick CM, Paterson T, Phillips EE, Phillips EM, Rampally N, Ribeiro C, Robertson MK, Rymer JT, Ryan SM, Sammons M, Scott AK, Scott AL, Shinomiya A, Smith C, Smith K, Smith NL, Sobeski MA, Suleiman A, Swift J, Takemura S, Talebi I, Tarnogorska D, Tenshaw E, Tokhi T, Walsh JJ, Yang T, Horne JA, Li F, Parekh R, Rivlin PK, Jayaraman V, Costa M, Jefferis GS, Ito K, Saalfeld S, George R, Meinertzhagen IA, Rubin GM, Hess HF, Jain V, Plaza SM (2020) A connectome and analysis of the adult Drosophila central brain. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.57443
Schmalz F, el Jundi B, Rössler W, Strube-Bloss M (2022) Categorizing visual information in subpopulations of honeybee mushroom body output neurons. Front Physiol 13:866807. https://doi.org/10.3389/fphys.2022.866807
Schmid-Hempel P, Schmid-Hempel R (1984) Life duration and turnover of foragers in the ant Cataglyphis bicolor (Hymenoptera, Formicidae). Ins Soc 31:345–360. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02223652
Schmitt F, Stieb SM, Wehner R, Rössler W (2016) Experience-related reorganization of giant synapses in the lateral complex: potential role in plasticity of the sky-compass pathway in the desert ant Cataglyphis fortis. Dev Neurobiol 76:390–404. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22322
Schmitt F, Vanselow JT, Schlosser A, Wegener C, Rössler W (2017) Neuropeptides in the desert ant Cataglyphis fortis: mass spectrometric analysis, localization, and age-related changes. J Comp Neurol 525:901–918. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24109
Scholl C, Wang Y, Krischke M, Mueller MJ, Amdam GV, Rössler W (2014) Light exposure leads to reorganization of microglomeruli in the mushroom bodies and influences juvenile hormone levels in the honeybee. Dev Neurobiol 74:1141–1153. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22195
Scholl C, Kübert N, Muenz TS, Rössler W (2015) CaMKII knockdown affects both early and late phases of olfactory long-term memory in the honeybee. J Exp Biol 218:3788–3796. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.124859
Schwarz S, Albert L, Wystrach A, Cheng K (2011) Ocelli contribute to the encoding of celestial compass information in the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. J Exp Biol 214:901–906. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.049262
Seelig JD, Jayaraman V (2015) Neural dynamics for landmark orientation and angular path integration. Nature 521:186–191. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature14446
Sommerlandt FMJ, Rössler W, Spaethe J (2017) Impact of light and alarm pheromone on immediate early gene expression in the European honeybee, Apis mellifera. Entomological Science 20:122–126. https://doi.org/10.1111/ens.12234
Sommerlandt FMJ, Brockmann A, Rössler W, Spaethe J (2019) Immediate early genes in social insects: a tool to identify brain regions involved in complex behaviors and molecular processes underlying neuroplasticity. Cell Mol Life Sci 76:637–651. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00018-018-2948-z
Steck K, Hansson BS, Knaden M (2009) Smells like home: desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis, use olfactory landmarks to pinpoint the nest. Front Zool 6:5. https://doi.org/10.1186/1742-9994-6-5
Steck K, Hansson BS, Knaden M (2011) Desert ants benefit from combining visual and olfactory landmarks. J Exp Biol 214:1307–1312. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.053579
Steinbeck F, Adden A, Graham P (2020) Connecting brain to behaviour: a role for general purpose steering circuits in insect orientation? J Exp Biol. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.212332
Stieb SM, Muenz TS, Wehner R, Rössler W (2010) Visual experience and age affect synaptic organization in the mushroom bodies of the desert ant Cataglyphis fortis. Dev Neurobiol 70:408–423. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20785
Stieb SM, Kelber C, Wehner R, Rössler W (2011) Antennal-lobe organization in desert ants of the genus Cataglyphis. Brain Behav Evol 77:136–146. https://doi.org/10.1159/000326211
Stieb SM, Hellwig A, Wehner R, Rössler W (2012) Visual experience affects both behavioral and neuronal aspects in the individual life history of the desert ant Cataglyphis fortis. Dev Neurobiol 72:729–742. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.20982
Stone T, Webb B, Adden A, Weddig NB, Honkanen A, Templin R, Wcislo W, Scimeca L, Warrant E, Heinze S (2017) An anatomically constrained model for path integration in the bee brain. Curr Biol 27:3069-3085.e11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.08.052
Strausfeld NJ (2012) Arthropod brains: evolution, functional elegance, and historical significance. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Strube-Bloss MF, Rössler W (2018) Multimodal integration and stimulus categorization in putative mushroom body output neurons of the honeybee. R Soc Open Sci 5:171785. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.171785
Strube-Bloss MF, Nawrot MP, Menzel R (2011) Mushroom body output neurons encode odor-reward associations. J Neurosci 31:3129–3140. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2583-10.2011
Stürzl W, Zeil J, Boeddeker N, Hemmi JM (2016) How wasps acquire and use views for homing. Curr Biol 26:470–482. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2015.12.052
Sun X, Yue S, Mangan M (2020) A decentralised neural model explaining optimal integration of navigational strategies in insects. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.54026
Thiagarajan D, Sachse S (2022) Multimodal information processing and associative learning in the insect brain. InSects. https://doi.org/10.3390/insects13040332
Träger U, Wagner R, Bausenwein B, Homberg U (2008) A novel type of microglomerular synaptic complex in the polarization vision pathway of the locust brain. J Comp Neurol 506:288–300. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.21512
Vogt K, Aso Y, Hige T, Knapek S, Ichinose T, Friedrich AB, Turner GC, Rubin GM, Tanimoto H (2016) Direct neural pathways convey distinct visual information to Drosophila mushroom bodies. Elife. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.14009
von Hadeln J, Hensgen R, Bockhorst T, Rosner R, Heidasch R, Pegel U, Quintero Pérez M, Homberg U (2020) Neuroarchitecture of the central complex of the desert locust: tangential neurons. J Comp Neurol 528:906–934. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.24796
Wajnberg E, Acosta-Avalos D, Alves OC, de Oliveira JF, Srygley RB, Esquivel DMS (2010) Magnetoreception in eusocial insects: an update. J R Soc Interface 7(Suppl 2):S207–S225. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2009.0526.focus
Webb B, Wystrach A (2016) Neural mechanisms of insect navigation. Curr Opin Insect Sci 15:27–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cois.2016.02.011
Wehner R (1997) The ant’s celestial compass system: spectral and polarization channels. In: Lehrer M (ed) Orientation and communication in arthropods, 1st edn. Birkhäuser, Basel, pp 145–185
Wehner R (2008) The desert ant’s navigational toolkit: procedural rather than positional knowledge. Navigation 55:101–114. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2161-4296.2008.tb00421.x
Wehner R (2020) Desert navigator: the journey of the ant. Harvard University Press, Cambridge
Wehner R, Lanfranconi B (1981) What do the ants know about the rotation of the sky? Nature 293:731–733. https://doi.org/10.1038/293731a0
Wehner R, Müller M (1993) How do ants acquire their celestial ephemeris function? Naturwissenschaften 80:331–333. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01141909
Wehner R, Rössler W (2013) Bounded plasticity in the desert ant’s navigational tool lit. In: Menzel R (ed) Invertebrate learning and memory, vol 22. Academic Press Elsevier, London, pp 514–529
Wehner R, Meier C, Zollikofer C (2004) The ontogeny of foragwehaviour in desert ants, Cataglyphis bicolor. Ecol Entomol 29:240–250. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0307-6946.2004.00591.x
Wehner R, Hoinville T, Cruse H, Cheng K (2016) Steering intermediate courses: desert ants combine information from various navigational routines. J Comp Physiol 202:459–472. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-016-1094-z
Wittlinger M, Wehner R, Wolf H (2006) The ant odometer: stepping on stilts and stumps. Science 312:1965–1967. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1126912
Wittlinger M, Wehner R, Wolf H (2007) The desert ant odometer: a stride integrator that accounts for stride length and walking speed. J Exp Biol 210:198–207. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.02657
Wystrach A, Schwarz S (2013) Ants use a predictive mechanism to compensate for passive displacements by wind. Curr Biol 23:R1083–R1085. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2013.10.072
Wystrach A, Beugnon G, Cheng K (2012) Ants might use different view-matching strategies on and off the route. J Exp Biol 215:44–55. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.059584
Wystrach A, Schwarz S, Schultheiss P, Baniel A, Cheng K (2014a) Multiple sources of celestial compass information in the Central Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. J Comp Physiol 200:591–601. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0899-x
Wystrach A, Philippides A, Aurejac A, Cheng K, Graham P (2014b) Visual scanning behaviours and their role in the navigation of the Australian desert ant Melophorus bagoti. J Comp Physiol 200:615–626. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-014-0900-8
Wystrach A, Mangan M, Webb B (2015) Optimal cue integration in ants. Proc Biol Sci 282:20151484. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1484
Wystrach A, Le Moël F, Clement L, Schwarz S (2020) A lateralised design for the interaction of visual memories and heading representations in navigating ants. bioRxiv. https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.08.13.249193
Yack JE (2004) The structure and function of auditory chordotonal organs in insects. Microsc Res Tech 63:315–337. https://doi.org/10.1002/jemt.20051
Yilmaz A, Lindenberg A, Albert S, Grübel K, Spaethe J, Rössler W, Groh C (2016) Age-related and light-induced plasticity in opsin gene expression and in primary and secondary visual centers of the nectar-feeding ant Camponotus rufipes. Dev Neurobiol 76:1041–1057. https://doi.org/10.1002/dneu.22374
Yilmaz A, Grübel K, Spaethe J, Rössler W (2019) Distributed plasticity in ant visual pathways following colour learning. Proc Biol Sci 286:20182813. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2018.2813
Zeil J (2012) Visual homing: an insect perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol 22:285–293. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.12.008
Zeil J, Fleischmann PN (2019) The learning walks of ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol News. https://doi.org/10.25849/MYRMECOL.NEWS_029:093
Zeller M, Held M, Bender J, Berz A, Heinloth T, Hellfritz T, Pfeiffer K (2015) Transmedulla neurons in the sky compass network of the honeybee (Apis mellifera) are a possible site of circadian input. PLoS ONE 10:e0143244. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0143244
Ziegler PE, Wehner R (1997) Time-courses of memory decay in vector-based and landmark-based systems of navigation in desert ants, Cataglyphis fortis. J Comp Physiol 181:13–20. https://doi.org/10.1007/s003590050088
Acknowledgements
We thank Rüdiger Wehner for his long-term collaboration in various projects described in this review. We are grateful to Kornelia Grübel and Claudia Groh for the support with neuroanatomical tools, confocal imaging, histochemistry, 3D reconstruction, data analyses, and electron microscopy. We thank all student helpers who contributed to the field work and to confocal reconstructions in various parts of the project. We particularly thank Jens Habenstein for his help with the 3D Cataglyphis brain, the constructive discussions, and his contribution to figure 2. We thank the Tunisian and Greek Governments and the staff of Schinias and Strofylia National Parks in Greece for their help and support over many years.
Funding
Open Access funding enabled and organized by Projekt DEAL. Research of the authors was supported by Grants from the German Research foundation (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, DFG) RO1177/7-1, project number 405620408, and CRC 1047 (project 6, project number 5483697), both to WR, DFG Grant FL1060/1-1, project number 422729997, and a Grant by the Klaus Tschira Foundation gGmbH (GSO/KT 16), both to PF.
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Contributions
Conceptualization: WR, RG, PNF. Drafting of the manuscript: WR. Preparation of figures: WR, RG, PF. Review and editing of manuscript: WR, RG, PNF.
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Conflict of interest
The authors do not have any competing interests.
Additional information
Handling editor: Uwe Homberg.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Rössler, W., Grob, R. & Fleischmann, P.N. The role of learning-walk related multisensory experience in rewiring visual circuits in the desert ant brain. J Comp Physiol A 209, 605–623 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-022-01600-y
Received:
Revised:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00359-022-01600-y