Skip to main content
Log in

The effect of speaker-specific auditory images on reading in Japanese

  • Published:
Current Psychology Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Auditory images for speech are preserved and can be accessed during reading. This research, conducted in Japan, examined whether and to what extent previous findings on the influence of speaker-specific auditory images in reading can be generalized to non-English speakers in a different cultural context. In two studies, Japanese participants were asked to read a text aloud after being informed that the text had been written by either a fast speaker or a slow speaker whose speech they were to listen to. The participants read the episode more slowly when it was attributed to a slow speaker than when it was attributed to a fast speaker. Individual differences in one’s mimicry of the speaker moderated the influence of speaker-specific auditory images in reading. The influence was confirmed only for those who consciously mimicked the speaker. In contrast, situational cues manipulated to generate affiliation with and closeness to the speakers did not influence the participants’ reading times.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Institutional subscriptions

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The mimicry and speaking rate interaction was qualified with speaker’s gender, F(1, 58) = 8.45, p = .005, η2p = .13. As mentioned, the influence of the speakers’ speaking rates was found only in participants consciously mimicking. Whereas the pattern appeared in male conversations, F(1, 58) = 26.29, p < .001, η2p = .31, it became weak in female conversations, F(1, 58) = 3.25, p = .08, η2p = .05.

References

  • Abramson, M., & Goldinger, S. D. (1997). What the reader’s eye tells the mind’s ear: Silent reading activates inner speech. Perception & Psychophysics, 59, 1059–1068.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alexander, J. D., & Nygaard, L. C. (2008). Reading voices and hearing text: Talker-specific auditory imagery in reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 34, 446–459.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Apple, W., Streeter, L. A., & Krauss, R. M. (1979). Effects of pitch and speech rate on personal attributions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 715–727.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22, 577–660.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gray, H. M., Ishii, K., & Ambady, N. (2011). Misery loves company: When sadness increases the desire for social connectedness. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37, 1438–1448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hall, E. T. (1976). Beyond culture. NY: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Halpern, A. R. (1988). Mental scanning in auditory imagery for songs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14, 434–443.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 61–135.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L. (2010). Auditory imagery: Empirical findings. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 302–329.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hubbard, T. L., & Stoeckig, K. (1988). Musical imagery: Generation of tones and chords. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 14(4), 656–667.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Intons-Peterson, M. J. (1980). The role of loudness in auditory imagery. Memory and Cognition, 8, 385–393.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Intons-Peterson, M. J., Russell, W., & Dressel, S. (1992). The role of pitch in auditory imagery. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 18, 233–240.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, K., & Uchida, Y. (2016). Japanese youth marginalization decreases interdependent orientation. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47, 376–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, K., Reyes, J. A., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Spontaneous attention to word content versus emotional tone: Differences among three cultures. Psychological Science, 14, 39–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ishii, K., Kobayashi, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Interdependence modulates the brain response to word-voice incongruity. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5, 307–317.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kitayama, S., & Ishii, K. (2002). Word and voice: Spontaneous attention to emotional utterances in two languages. Cognition and Emotion, 16, 29–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M., & Matt, A. M. (1977). If you speak slowly, do people read your prose slowly? Person-particular speech recoding during reading. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 9, 250–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kurby, C. A., Magliano, J. P., & Rapp, D. N. (2009). Those voices in your head: Activation of auditory images during reading. Cognition, 112, 457–461.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lakin, J. L., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Using nonconscious behavioral mimicry to create affiliation and rapport. Psychological Science, 14, 334–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nygaard, L. C., Burt, S. A., & Queen, J. S. (2000). Surface form typicality and asymmetric transfer in episodic memory for spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 26, 1228–1244.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Palmeri, T. J., Goldinger, S. D., & Pisoni, D. B. (1993). Episodic encoding of voice attributes and recognition memory for spoken words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 19, 309–328.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stel, M., & Vonk, R. (2010). Mimicry in social interaction: Benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. British Journal of Psychology, 101, 311–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stites, M. C., Luke, S. G., & Christianson, K. (2013). The psychologist said quickly, “Dialogue descriptions modulate reading speed!”. Memory and Cognition, 41, 137–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stuart, G. P., & Jones, D. M. (1996). From auditory image to auditory percept: Facilitation through common processes? Memory and Cognition, 24, 296–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tanaka, A., Koizumi, A., Imai, H., Hiramatsu, S., Hiramoto, E., & de Gelder, B. (2010). I feel your voice: Cultural differences in the multisensory perception of emotion. Psychological Science, 21, 1259–1262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Baaren, R. B., Horgan, T. G., Chartrand, T. L., & Dijkmans, M. (2004). The forest, the trees, and the chameleon: Context dependence and mimicry. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 453–459.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wu, S., & Keysar, B. (2007). The effect of culture on perspective taking. Psychological Science, 18, 600–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, B., & Scheepers, C. (2011). Contextual modulation of reading rate for direct versus indirect speech quotations. Cognition, 121, 447–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yao, B., Belin, P., & Scheepers, C. (2011). Silent reading of direct versus indirect speech activates voice-selective areas in the auditory cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 3146–3152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zatorre, R. J., Halpern, A. R., Perry, D. W., Meyer, E., & Evans, A. C. (1996). Hearing in the mind's ear: A PET investigation of musical imagery and perception. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8, 29–46.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Keiko Ishii.

Ethics declarations

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Conflict of Interest

Yuichi Mori declares that he has no conflict of interest. Keiko Ishii declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Electronic supplementary material

ESM 1

(XLSX 12 kb)

ESM 2

(XLSX 12 kb)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Mori, Y., Ishii, K. The effect of speaker-specific auditory images on reading in Japanese. Curr Psychol 39, 2343–2350 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9946-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s12144-018-9946-z

Keywords

Navigation