Skip to main content
Log in

What are the metacognitive costs of young children’s overconfidence?

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Children typically hold very optimistic views of their own skills but so far, only a few studies have investigated possible correlates of the ability to predict performance accurately. Therefore, this study examined the role of individual differences in performance estimation accuracy as a global metacognitive index for different monitoring and control skills (item-level judgments of learning [JOLs] and confidence judgments [CJs]), metacognitive control processes (allocation of study time and control of answers), and executive functions (cognitive flexibility, inhibition, working memory) in 6-year-olds (N = 93). The three groups of under estimators, realists and over estimators differed significantly in their monitoring and control abilities: the under estimators outperformed the over estimators by showing a higher discrimination in CJs between correct and incorrect recognition. Also, the under estimators scored higher on the adequate control of incorrectly recognized items. Regarding the interplay of monitoring and control processes, under estimators spent more time studying items with low JOLs, and relied more systematically on their monitoring when controlling their recognition compared to over estimators. At the same time, the three groups did not differ significantly from each other in their executive functions. Overall, results indicate that differences in performance estimation accuracy are systematically related to other global and item-level metacognitive monitoring and control abilities in children as young as six years of age, while no meaningful association between performance estimation accuracy and executive functions was found.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Allwood, C. M. (2010). The realism in children′s metacognitive judgments of their episodic memory performance. In A. Efklides & P. Misailidi (Eds.), Trends and prospects in metacognition research (pp. 149–169). New York: Springer.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Archibald, S. J., & Kerns, K. A. (1999). Identification and description of new tests of executive functioning in children. Child Neuropsychology, 5, 115–129. doi:10.1076/chin.5.2.115.3167.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., & Miller, P. H. (2010). A developmental perspective on executive function. Child Development, 81, 1641–1660. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01499.x 21077853.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Jones, L. L. (2009). Executive functions after age 5: Changes and correlates. Developmental Review, 29, 180–200. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2009.05.002 20161467.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Best, J. R., Miller, P. H., & Naglieri, J. A. (2011). Relations between executive function and academic achievement from ages 5 to 17 in a large, representative national sample. Learning and Individual Differences, 21, 327–336. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2011.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., & Bering, J. M. (2002). The evolved child applying evolutionary devlopmental psychology to modern schooling. Learning and Individual Differences, 12, 1–27. doi:10.1016/S1041-6080(02)00047-X.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F., Coyle, T. R., & Gaultney, J. F. (1992). Developmental differences in the acquisition and maintenance of an organizational strategy - Evidence for the utilization deficiency hypothesis. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 54, 434–448. doi:10.1016/0022-0965(92)90029-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, L., Sherbenou, R. J., & Johnsen, S. K. (1997). Test of nonverbal intelligence (TONI-3) (3rd ed.). Austin: Pro-Ed.

  • Bryce, D., Whitebread, D., & Szűcs, D. (in press). The relationships among executive functions, metacognitive skills and educational achievement in 5 and 7 year-old children. Metacognition and Learning, doi:10.1007/s11409-014-9120-4.

  • Buratti, S., Allwood, C. M., & Kleitman, S. (2013). First- and second-order metacognitive judgments of semantic memory reports: The influence of personality traits and cognitive styles. Metacognition and Learning, 8, 79–102. doi:10.1007/s11409-013-9096-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clayson, D. E. (2005). Performance overconfidence: Metacognitive effects or misplaced student expectations? Journal of Marketing Education, 27, 122–129. doi:10.1177/0273475304273525.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, M. C., Amso, D., Anderson, L. C., & Diamond, A. (2006). Development of cognitive control and executive functions from 4 to 13 years: Evidence from manipulations of memory, inhibition, and task switching. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2037–2078. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.02.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeMarie, D., Miller, P. H., Ferron, J., & Cunningham, W. R. (2004). Path analysis tests of theoretical models of children′s memory performance. Journal of Cognition and Development, 5, 461–492. doi:10.1207/s15327647jcd0504_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Destan, N., Hembacher, E., Ghetti, S., & Roebers, C. M. (2014). Early metacognitive abilities: The interplay of monitoring and control processes in 5- to 7-year-old children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 126, 213–228.

  • Diamond, A. (2013). Executive functions. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 135–168. doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Connor, L. T. (1997). Age differences in the allocation of study time account for age differences in memory performance. Memory & Cognition, 25, 691–700. doi:10.3758/Bf03211311.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognition. Thousand Oaks: Sage.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. A. (2012). Overconfidence produces underachievement: Inaccurate self evaluations undermine students′ learning and retention. Learning and Instruction, 22, 271–280. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.08.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Thiede, K. W. (2013). Four cornerstones of calibration research: Why understanding students′ judgments can improve their achievement. Learning and Instruction, 24, 58–61. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunning, D., Johnson, K., Ehrlinger, J., & Kruger, J. (2003). Why people fail to recognize their own incompetence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12, 83–87. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.01235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez-Duque, D., Baird, J. A., & Posner, M. I. (2000). Executive attention and metacognitive regulation. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 288–307. doi:10.1006/ccog.2000.0447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, B., & Metcalfe, J. (2014). Overconfidence in children′s multi-trial judgments of learning. Learning and Instruction, 32, 1–9. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2014.01.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H. (1979). Meta-cognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–911. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.906.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., Friedrichs, A. G., & Hoyt, J. D. (1970). Developmental changes in memorization processes. Cognitive Psychology, 1, 324–340. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(70)90019-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hacker, D. J., Bol, L., Horgan, D. D., & Rakow, E. A. (2000). Test prediction and performance in a classroom context. Journal of Educational Psychology, 92, 160–170. doi:10.1037//0022-0663.92.1.160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hembacher, E., & Ghetti, S. (2013). How to bet on a memory: Developmental linkages between subjective recollection and decision making. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 436–452. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2013.03.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hoffmann-Biencourt, A., Lockl, K., Schneider, W., Ackerman, R., & Koriat, A. (2010). Self-paced study time as a cue for recall predictions across school age. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 28, 767–784. doi:10.1348/026151009x479042.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Ensor, R., Wilson, A., & Graham, A. (2010). Tracking executive function across the transition to school: A latent variable approach. Developmental Neuropsychology, 35, 20–36. doi:10.1080/87565640903325691 20390590.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Goldsmith, M. (1996). Monitoring and control processes in the strategic regulation of memory accuracy. Psychological Review, 103, 490–517. doi:10.1037/0033-295x.103.3.490.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, S. S., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Children′s strategic regulation, metacognitive monitoring, and control processes during test taking. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 325–340. doi:10.1348/000709910x485719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krebs, S. S., & Roebers, C. M. (2012). The impact of retrieval processes, age, general achievement level, and test scoring scheme for children′s metacognitive monitoring and controlling. Metacognition and Learning, 7, 75–90. doi:10.1007/s11409-011-9079-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruger, J., & Dunning, D. (1999). Unskilled and unaware of it: How difficulties in recognizing one′s own incompetence lead to inflated self-assessments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77, 1121–1134. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.77.6.1121.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, K., Bull, R., & Ho, R. M. H. (2013). Developmental changes in executive functioning. Child Development, 84, 1933–1953. doi:10.1111/Cdev.12096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lin-Agler, L. M., Moore, D., & Zabrucky, K. M. (2004). Effects of personality on metacognitive self-assessments. College Student Journal, 38(3), 453–461.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Hartwig, M. K., Rawson, K. A., Swan, K., & Cook, D. (2009). Using standards to improve middle school students′ accuracy at evaluating the quality of their recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 307–318. doi:10.1037/a0017599 20025417.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lipko, A. R., Dunlosky, J., Lipowski, S. L., & Merriman, W. E. (2012). Young children are not underconfident with practice: The benefit of ignoring a fallible memory heuristic. Journal of Cognition and Development, 13, 174–188. doi:10.1080/15248372.2011.577760.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2003). Metacognitive monitoring and self-control processes for children′s allocation of study time. Zeitschrift für Paedagogische Psychologie, 17, 173–183. doi:10.1024//1010-0652.17.3.173.

  • Lyons, K. E., & Ghetti, S. (2011). The development of uncertainty monitoring in early childhood. Child Development, 82, 1178–1787. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01649.x 21954871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyons, K. E., & Zelazo, P. D. (2011). Monitoring, metacognition, and executive function: Elucidating the role of self-reflection in the development of self-regulation. In B. J. Benson (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 379–412). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H., & McGuire, M. J. (2002). Metacognition for text: Implications for education. In T. J. Perfect & B. L. Schwartz (Eds.), Applied Metacognition (pp. 39–67). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Marazita, J. M., & Merriman, W. E. (2004). Young children′s judgment of whether they know names for objects: The metalinguistic ability it reflects and the processes it involves. Journal of Memory and Language, 51, 458–472. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2004.06.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J. (2009). Metacognitive judgments and control of study. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 159–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Evidence that judgments of learning are causally related to study choice. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 174–179. doi:10.3758/PBR.15.1.174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2013). Metacognition and control of study choice in children. Metacognition and Learning, 8, 19–46. doi:10.1007/s11409-013-9094-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., & Friedman, N. P. (2012). The nature and organization of individual differences in executive functions: Four general conclusions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 21, 8–14. doi:10.1177/0963721411429458.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex ″frontal lobe″ tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive Psychology, 41, 49–100. doi:10.1006/cogp.1999.0734.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., Dunlosky, K. J., Graf, A., & Narens, L. (1994). Utilization of metacognitive judgments in the allocation of study during multitrial learning. Psychological Science, 5, 207–213. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1994.tb00502.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O., & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and new findings. In G. H. Bower (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 125–173). New York: Academic.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nicholls, J. G. (1978). Development of concepts of effort and ability, perception of academic attainment, and understanding that difficult tasks require more ability. Child Development, 49, 800–814. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1978.tb02383.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pallier, G., Wilkinson, R., Danthiir, V., Kleitman, S., Knezevic, G., Stankov, L., et al. (2002). The role of individual differences in the accuracy of confidence judgments. Journal of General Psychology, 129, 257–299. doi:10.1080/00221300209602099.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S. J., & Gathercole, S. E. (2001). Working memory test battery for children (WMTB-C). London: Psychological Cooperation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plumert, J. M., & Schwebel, D. C. (1997). Social and temperamental influences on children′s overestimation of their physical abilities: Links to accidental injuries. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 67, 317–337. doi:10.1006/jecp.1997.2411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ricken, G., Fritz, A., Schuck, K. D., & Preuß, U. (2007). HAWIVA-III, Hannover-Wechsler-Intelligenztest fuer das Vorschulalter-III. Manual zur Durchfuehrung und Auswertung (3rd ed.). Goettingen, Germany: Huber.

  • Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2009). Children′s strategic regulation of memory accuracy (pp. 253–274). Hauppauge: Nova.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Explicit and implicit confidence judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: An eye-tracking approach. Metacognition and Learning, 5, 229–250. doi:10.1007/s11409-010-9059-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2013). Children′s performance estimation in mathematics and science tests over a school year: A pilot study. Electronic Journal of Research in Educational Psychology, 11(1), 5–24.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roderer, T., & Roebers, C. M. (2014). Can you see me thinking (about my answers)? Using eye-tracking to illuminate developmental differences in monitoring and control skills and their relation to performance. Metacognition and Learning, 9, 1–23. doi:10.1007/s11409-013-9109-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Cimeli, P., Roethlisberger, M., & Neuenschwander, R. (2012). Executive functioning, metacognition, and self-perceived competence in elementary school children: An explorative study on their interrelations and their role for school achievement. Metacognition and Learning, 7, 151–173. doi:10.1007/s11409-012-9089-9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., & Kauer, M. (2009). Motor and cognitive control in a normative sample of 7-year-olds. Developmental Science, 2, 175–181. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00755.x 19120425.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Krebs, S. S., & Roderer, T. (2014). Metacognitive monitoring and control in elementary school children: Their interrelations and their role for test performance. Learning and Individual Differences, 29, 141–149. doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Schmid, C., & Roderer, T. (2009). Metacognitive monitoring and control processes involved in primary school children′s test performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 749–767. doi:10.1348/978185409X429842.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., von der Linden, N., & Howie, P. (2007). Favourable and unfavourable conditions for children′s confidence judgments. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25, 109–134. doi:10.1348/026151006X104392.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roethlisberger, M., Neuenschwander, R., Michel, E., & Roebers, C. M. (2010). Executive functions: Underlying cognitive processes and their correlates in late preschool children. Zeitschrift fuer Entwicklungspsychologie und Paedagogische Psychologie, 42, 99–110. doi:10.1026/0049-8637/A000010.

  • Schaefer, P. S., Williams, C. C., Goodie, A. S., & Campbell, W. K. (2004). Overconfidence and the big five. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 473–480. doi:10.1016/j.jrp.2003.09.010.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291–297. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00044.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Vise, M., Lockl, K., & Nelson, T. O. (2000). Developmental trends in children′s memory monitoring - Evidence from a judgment-of-learning task. Cognitive Development, 15, 115–134. doi:10.1016/S0885-2014(00)00024-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shin, H., Bjorklund, D. F., & Beck, E. F. (2007). The adaptive nature of children′s overestimation in a strategic memory task. Cognitive Development, 22, 197–212. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sinkavich, F. J. (1995). Performance and metamemory: Do students know what they don′t know? Journal of Instructional Psychology, 22(1), 77–87.

    Google Scholar 

  • Son, L. K. (2005). Metacognitive control: Children′s short-term versus long-term study strategies. Journal of General Psychology, 132, 347–363. doi:10.3200/Genp. 132.4.347-364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Son, L. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2000). Metacognitive and control strategies in study-time allocation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 26, 204–221. doi:10.1037//0278-7393.26.1.204.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stipek, D. J., Roberts, T. A., & Sanborn, M. E. (1984). Preschool-age children’s performance expectations for themselves and another child as a function of the incentive value of success and the salience of past performance. Child Development, 55, 1983–1989. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.1984.tb03896.x.

  • Sussan, D., & Son, L. K. (2007). The training of metacognitive monitoring in children. Columbia Undergraduate Science Journal, 2.

  • Szpunar, K. K., Jing, H. G., & Schacter, D. L. (2014). Overcoming overconfidence in learning from video-recorded lectures: Implications of interpolated testing for online education. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 3, 161–164. doi:10.1016/j.jarmac.2014.02.001.

  • Tucker-Drob, E. M. (2009). Differentiation of cognitive abilities across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 45, 1097–1118. doi:10.1037/a0015864 19586182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van Loon, M. H., de Bruin, A. B. H., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2013). Activation of inaccurate prior knowledge affects primary-school students’ metacognitive judgments and calibration. Learning and Instruction, 24, 15–25. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2012.08.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visé, M., & Schneider, W. (2000). Determinanten der Leistungsvorhersage bei Kindergarten- und Grundschulkindern: Zur Bedeutung metakognitiver und motivationaler Einflußfaktoren. Zeitschrift fuer Entwicklungspsychologie und Paedagogische Psychologie, 32, 51–58. doi:10.1026//0049-8637.32.2.51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (2002). The Wechsler Preschool and primary scale of intelligence (WPPSI-III) (3rd ed.). San Antonio: The Psychological Cooperation.

  • Wiebe, S. A., Espy, K. A., & Charak, D. (2008). Using confirmatory factor analysis to understand executive control in preschool children: I. Latent structure. Developmental Psychology, 44, 575–587. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.44.2.575 18331145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This project was partially financed by a grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF- Grant No.100014_126559 provided to Claudia M. Roebers). We would like to thank the participating children and their families, the children’s teachers and the student research assistants who helped with data collection.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Nesrin Destan.

Appendix A

Appendix A

Table 5 Intercorrelations (Spearman-rho) of the study variables

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Destan, N., Roebers, C.M. What are the metacognitive costs of young children’s overconfidence?. Metacognition Learning 10, 347–374 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9133-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-014-9133-z

Keywords

Navigation