Skip to main content
Log in

Explicit and implicit confidence judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: an eye-tracking approach

  • Published:
Metacognition and Learning Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In the present study, primary school children’s ability to give accurate confidence judgments (CJ) was addressed, with a special focus on uncertainty monitoring. In order to investigate the effects of memory retrieval processes on monitoring judgments, item difficulty in a vocabulary learning task (Japanese symbols) was manipulated. Moreover, as a first exploratory step to uncover fast and retrieval bound (implicit) monitoring processes that take place before explicit CJ are openly reported, fixation time allocation during recognition and monitoring was recorded with an eye-tracking device. Results revealed developmental progression in uncertainty (but not in certainty) monitoring between the age of 7 and 9 years. Differences in CJ across levels of item difficulty point to a substantial impact of retrieval processes on 9-yr-olds’ but not on 7-yr-olds’ monitoring. Eye-tracking data revealed an overall bias towards medium and high CJ, and confirmed evidence on developmental progression in monitoring skills.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Other measures of implicit CJ were calculated as well (e.g., the last fixation on a confidence judgment before the explicit judgment was given during recording). These analyses revealed the same or very similar results.

References

  • Arbuthnott, K. D., Kealy, K. L. K., & Ylioja, S. (2008). Judgement of confidence in childhood memories. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 22, 953–978.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aslin, R. N., & McMurray, B. (2004). Automated corneal-reflection eye-tracking in infancy: methodological developments and applications to cognition. Infancy, 6, 155–163.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baillargeon, R. (1987). Object permanence in 3 1/2- and 4 1/2-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 23, 655–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clements, W. A., & Perner, J. (1994). Implicit understanding of belief. Cognitive Development, 9, 377–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta, M. (1998). Frontoparietal cortical networks for directing attention and the eye to visual locations: Identical, independent, or overlapping neural systems? Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 95, 831–838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., & Bjork, R. A. (2008). The integrated nature of metamemory and memory. In J. Dunlosky & R. A. Bjork (Eds.), Handbook of metamemory and memory (pp. 11–28). New York: Psychology.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlosky, J., Hertzog, C., Kennedy, M. R. F., & Thiede, K. W. (2005). The self-monitoring approach for effective learning. International Journal of Cognitive Technology, 10, 4–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Eriksen, B. A., & Eriksen, C. W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16, 143–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Flavell, J. H., & Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In R. V. Kail & J. W. Hagen (Eds.), Perspectives on the development of memory and cognition (pp. 3–34). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hauland, G. (2003). Measuring team situation awareness by means of eye movement data. In Proceedings of HCI International 2003: Vol. 3 (pp. 230–234). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

  • Hayhoe, M. M. (2004). Advances in relating eye movements and cognition. Infancy, 6, 267–274.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayhoe, M. M., Bensinger, D. G., & Ballard, D. H. (1998). Task constraints in visual working memory. Vision Research, 38, 125–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henderson, J. M., & Hollingworth, A. (1998). Eye movements during scene viewing: An overview. In G. Underwood (Ed.), Eye guidance in reading and scene perception (pp. 269–293). Oxford: Elsevier.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Howie, P., & Roebers, C. M. (2007). Developmental progression in the confidence-accuracy relationship in event recall: Insights provided by a calibration perspective. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 871–893.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karatekin, C. (2004). Development of attentional allocation in the dual task paradigm. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 52, 7–21.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karatekin, C. (2007). Eye tracking studies of normative and atypical development. Developmental Review, 27, 283–348.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kelley, C. M., & Jacoby, L. L. (1996). Memory attributions: Remembering, knowing, and feeling of knowing. In L. M. Reder (Ed.), Implicit memory and metacognition (pp. 287–308). Hillsdale: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kinnunen, R., & Vauras, M. (1995). Comprehension monitoring and the level of comprehension in high- and low-achieving primary school children’s reading. Learning and Instruction, 5, 143–165.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kinnunen, R., Vauras, M., & Niemi, P. (1998). Comprehension monitoring in beginning readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 353–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1993). How do we know that we know? The accessibility model of the feeling of knowing. Psychological Review, 100, 609–639.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1995). Dissociating knowing and the feeling of knowing: further evidence for the accessibility model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 311–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1997). Monitoring one’s own knowledge during study: a cue-utilization approach to judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 126, 349–370.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (1998). Metamemory: The feeling of knowing and its vagaries. In M. Sabourin, F. Craik, & M. Robert (Eds.), Advances in psychological science, vol. 2: Biological and cognitive aspects (pp. 461–479). Hove: Psychology Press/Erlbaum (UK) Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (2000). The feeling of knowing: Some metatheoretical implications for consciousness and control. Consciousness and Cognition, 9, 149–171.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (2007). Metacognition and consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 289–325). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A. (2008). Subjective confidence in one’s answers: the consensuality principle. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 945–959.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Ackerman, R. (2010). Choice latency as a cue for children’s subjective confidence in the correctness of their answers. Developmental Science, 13, 441–453.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., & Shitzer-Reichert, R. (2002). Metacognitive judgments and their accuracy: Insights from the processes underlying judgments of learning in children. In M. Izaute, P. Chambres, & P.-J. Marescaux (Eds.), Metacognition: Process, function, and use (pp. 1–17). New York: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Koriat, A., Ackerman, R., Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2009). The memorizing-effort heuristic in judgments of learning: a developmental perspective. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 265–279.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Körkel, J. (1987). Die Entwicklung von Gedächtnis- und Metagedächtnisleistungen in Abhängigkeit von bereichsspezifischen Vorkenntnissen. Frankfurt: Lang.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leslie, A. M., & Keeble, S. (1987). Do six-month-old infants perceive causality? Cognition, 25, 265–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2003). Metakognitive Überwachungs- und Selbstkontrollprozesse bei der Lernzeiteinteilung von Kindern. Zeitschrift für Pädagogische Psychologie, 17, 173–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maki, R. H. (1998). Test predictions over text material. In D. J. Hacker, J. Dunlosky, & A. C. Graesser (Eds.), Metacognition in educational theory and practice (pp. 117–144). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Metcalfe, J., & Finn, B. (2008). Familiarity and retrieval processes in delayed judgments of learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 1084–1097.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nagy, W. E., & Scott, J. A. (2000). Vocabulary processes. In M. L. Kamil, P. B. Mosenthal, P. D. Pearson, & R. Barr (Eds.), Handbook of reading research ((pp, Vol. III, pp. 269–284). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nelson, T. O. & Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A theoretical framework and some new findings. In G.H. Bower (Ed). The Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125–173. New York: Academic Press.

  • Pauen, S. (2002). Evidence for knowledge-based category discrimination in infancy. Child Development, 73, 1016–1033.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pickering, S., & Gathercole, S. (2001). The working memory test battery for children. London: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pillow, B. H. (2002). Children’s and adults’ evaluation of the certainty of deductive inferences, inductive inferences, and guesses. Child Development, 73, 779–792.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pillow, B. H., & Anderson, K. L. (2006). Children’s awareness of their own certainty and understanding of deduction and guessing. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 24, 823–849.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pillow, B. H., Hill, V., Boyce, A., & Stein, C. (2000). Understanding inference as a source of knowledge: children’s ability to evaluate the certainty of deduction, perception, and guessing. Developmental Psychology, 36, 169–179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renner, C. H., & Renner, M. J. (2001). But I thought I knew that: using confidence estimation as a debiasing technique to improve classroom performance. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 15, 23–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rhodes, M. G., & Kelley, C. M. (2005). Executive processes, memory accuracy, and memory monitoring: an aging and individual difference analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 52, 578–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robinson, M. D., Johnson, J. T., & Robertson, D. A. (2000). Process versus content in eyewitness metamemory monitoring. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6, 207–221.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M. (2002). Confidence judgments in children’s and adult’s event recall and suggestibility. Developmental Psychology, 38, 1052–1067.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Howie, P., & Beuscher, E. (2007). Can private reports enhance children’s event recall, lower their suggestibility and foster their metacognitive monitoring compared to face-to-face interviews? Computers in Human Behavior, 23, 749–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roebers, C. M., Schmid, C., & Roderer, T. (2009). Metacognitive monitoring and control processes involved in primary school children’s test performance. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 79, 749–767.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: history and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13, 501–518.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scheiter, K., & Van Gog, T. (2009). Using eye tracking in applied research to study and stimulate the processing of information from multi-representational sources. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 1209–1214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W. (1998). Performance prediction in young children: Effects of skill, metacognition and wishful thinking. Developmental Science, 1, 291–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Lockl, K. (2008). Procedural metacognition in children: Evidence for developmental trends. In J. Dunlosky & B. Bjork (Eds.), A handbook of memory and metamemory (pp. 391–409). New York: Taylor & Francis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., & Pressley, M. (1997). Memory development between two and twenty (2nd ed.). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schraw, G., & Roedel, T. D. (1994). Test difficulty and judgment bias. Memory & Cognition, 22, 63–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spelke, E. S., & Kestenbaum, R. (1986). The origins of the concept of objects. Psychologie Francaise, 31, 67–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W. (1999). The importance of monitoring and self-regulation during multitrial learning. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 6, 662–667.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thiede, K. W., Anderson, M. C. M., & Therriault, D. (2003). Accuracy of metacognitive monitoring affects learning of texts. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 66–73.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van der Stel, M., & Veenman, M. V. J. (2008). Relation between intellectual ability and metacognitive skillfulness as predictors of learning performance of young students performing tasks in different domains. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 128–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J. (2005). The assessment of metacognitive skills: What can be learned from multimethod designs? In C. Artelt & B. Moschner (Eds.), Lernstrategien und Metakognition: Implikationen für Forschung und Praxis (pp. 75–97). Berlin: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Veenman, M. V. J., & Spaans, M. A. (2005). Relation between intellectual and metacognitive skills: age and task differences. Learning and Individual Differences, 15, 159–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Visé, M., & Schneider, W. (2000). Determinants of performance prediction in kindergarten and school children: the importance of metacognitive and motivational factors. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagogische Psychologie, 32, 51–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Von der Linden, N., & Roebers, C. M. (2006). Developmental changes in uncertainty monitoring during an event recall task. Metacognition and Learning, 1, 213–228.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, J., Spencer, K., & Xing, M. (2009). Metacognitive beliefs and strategies in learning Chinese as a foreign language. System, 37, 46–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wynn, K. (1992). Addition and subtraction by human infants. Nature, 358, 749–750.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zimmerman, B. J., & Kitsantas, A. (1999). Acquiring writing revision skill: shifting from process to outcome self-regulatory goals. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 241–250.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas Roderer.

Additional information

The current study was partially financed by a grant of the Swiss National Science Foundation (SNSF—Grant No. 100014–112492 to the second author). We wish to thank Francoise Nor for her help with the data collection and the teachers, children, and their parents for their cooperation and participation. Thanks are also due to the Tobii Technology support team for their technical assistance.

Appendix

Appendix

Easy items – learning trial

figure a

Difficult items – learning trial

figure b

Unanswerable items – recognition trial

figure c

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Roderer, T., Roebers, C.M. Explicit and implicit confidence judgments and developmental differences in metamemory: an eye-tracking approach. Metacognition Learning 5, 229–250 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-010-9059-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11409-010-9059-z

Keywords

Navigation