Skip to main content
Log in

The intuitiveness of the law of large numbers

  • Original Article
  • Published:
ZDM Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper two studies are reported in which two contrasting claims concerning the intuitiveness of the law of large numbers are investigated. While Sedlmeier and Gigerenzer (J Behav Decis Mak 10:33–51, 1997) claim that people have an intuition that conforms to the law of large numbers, but that they can only employ this intuition in specific circumstances, Kahneman and Tversky (Cogn Psychol 3:430–454, 1972) claim that people have an intuition that prohibits them from correctly applying the law of large numbers to certain tasks, making it necessary to reason analytically and inhibit the intuitive response. The dual processing theory of reasoning was used as the theoretical framework to study these two claims, while priming and a working memory load method were used to study the claims in more detail. No evidence was found for the claims of Sedlmeier and Gigerenzer (J Behav Decis Mak 10:33–51, 1997). Various possible explanations for the results are provided and options for further research are suggested.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bar-Hillel, M. (1982). Studies of representativeness. In D. Kahneman, P. Slovic, & A. Tversky (Eds.), Judgement under uncertainty: heuristics and biases (pp. 69–83). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Chiesi, F., Primi, C., & Morsanyi, K. (2011). Developmental changes in probabilistic reasoning: the role of cognitive capacity, instructions, thinking styles, and relevant knowledge. Thinking & Reasoning, 17, 315–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2004). Automatic–heuristic and executive–analytic processing during reasoning: chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1070–1100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Neys, W. (2006). Automatic-heuristic and executive-analytic processing during reasoning: chronometric and dual-task considerations. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 59, 1070–1100. doi:10.1080/02724980543000123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Engel, J., & Sedlmeier, P. (2005). On middle-school students’ comprehension of randomness and chance variability in data. ZDM––The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 37(3), 168–177.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2006). The heuristic–analytic theory of reasoning: extension and evaluation. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13(3), 378–395.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgement and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Curtis-Holmes, J. (2005). Rapid responding increases belief bias: evidence for the dual-process theory of reasoning. Thinking & Reasoning, 11, 382–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Dusoir, A. E. (1977). Proportionality and sample size as factors in intuitive statistical judgment. Acta Psychologica, 41, 129–137.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. S. B. T., & Over, D. E. (1996). Rationality and reasoning. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E. (1987). Intuition in science and mathematics: an educational approach. Dordrecht: Reidel.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischbein, E., & Schnarch, D. (1997). The evolution with age of probabilistic, intuitively based misconceptions. Journal of Research in Mathematics Education, 28, 96–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederick, S. (2005). Cognitive reflection and decision making. The Journal of Economic Perspectives, 19, 25–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Garfield, J. B. (2003). Assessing statistical reasoning. Statistics Education Research Journal, 2(1), 22–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gillard, E., Van Dooren, W., Schaeken, W., & Verschaffel, L. (2009). Proportional reasoning as a heuristic-based process: time pressure and dual task considerations. Experimental Psychology, 56, 92–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D. (2000). A psychological point of view: violations of rational rules as a diagnostic of mental processes. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23, 681–683.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1972). Subjective probability: a judgment of representativeness. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 430–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lem, S., Van Dooren, W., Gillard, E., & Verschaffel, L. (2011). Sample size neglect problems: a critical analysis. Studia Psychologica, 53, 123–135.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Rettinger, D. A., Shah, P., & Hegarty, M. (2001). How are visuospatial working memory, executive functioning, and spatial abilities related? A latent-variable analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Murray, J., Iding, M., Farris, H., & Revlin, R. (1987). Sample-size salience and statistical inference. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society, 25, 367–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pelham, B. W., & Neter, E. (1995). The effect of motivation of judgment depends on the difficulty of the judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(4), 581–594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reagan, R. T. (1989). Variations on a seminal demonstration of people’s insensitivity to sample size. Organzational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 43, 52–57.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rouder, J. N., Speckman, P. L., Sun, D., Morey, R. D., & Iverson, G. (2009). Bayesian t tests for accepting and rejecting the null hypothesis. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16, 225–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schrage, G. (1983). Interpretation of stochastic models. In R. Scholz (Ed.), Decision making under uncertainty (pp. 351–361). Amsterdam: North-Holland.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P. (1998). The distribution matters: two types of sample-size tasks. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(4), 281–301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sedlmeier, P., & Gigerenzer, G. (1997). Intuitions about sample size: the empirical law of large numbers. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 10, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stavy, R., & Babai, R. (2010). Overcoming intuitive interference in mathematics: insights from behavioral, brain imaging and intervention studies. ZDM––The International Journal on Mathematics Education, 42(6), 621–633.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Toplak, M. E., West, R. F., & Stanovich, K. E. (2011). The cognitive reflection test as a predictor of performance on heuristics-and-biases tasks. Memory & Cognition, 39, 1275–1289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Dooren, W., De Bock, D., Depaepe, F., Janssens, D., & Verschaffel, L. (2003). The illusion of linearity: expanding the evidence towards probabilistic reasoning. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 53, 113–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Hoof, J., Lijnen, T., Verschaffel, L., & Van Dooren, W. (2013). Are secondary school students still hampered by the natural number bias? A reaction time study on fraction comparison tasks. Research in Mathematics Education, 15, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Verschueren, N., Schaeken, W., & d’Ydewalle, G. (2004). Everyday conditional reasoning with working memory preload. Proceedings of the Twenty-Sixth Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1399–1404). Mahwah: Erlbaum.

  • Well, A. D., Pollatsek, A., & Boyce, S. J. (1990). Understanding the effects of sample size on the variability of the mean. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 47, 289–312.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Stephanie Lem holds a Post-doctoral fellowship of the Research Foundation-Flanders (FWO). This research was partially supported by Grant GOA/12/010 ‘Number sense: Analysis and Improvement’ of the KU Leuven.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Stephanie Lem.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lem, S. The intuitiveness of the law of large numbers. ZDM Mathematics Education 47, 783–792 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-015-0676-5

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11858-015-0676-5

Keywords

Navigation