Skip to main content
Log in

Mindreading as social expertise

  • Published:
Synthese Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In recent years, a number of approaches to social cognition research have emerged that highlight the importance of embodied interaction for social cognition (Reddy, How infants know minds, 2008; Gallagher, J Conscious Stud 8:83–108, 2001; Fuchs and Jaegher, Phenom Cogn Sci 8:465–486, 2009; Hutto, in Seemans (ed.) Joint attention: new developments in psychology, philosophy of mind and social neuroscience, 2012). Proponents of such ‘interactionist’ approaches emphasize the importance of embodied responses that are engaged in online social interaction, and which, according to interactionists, present an alternative to mindreading as a source of social understanding. We agree that it is important to take embodied interaction seriously, but do not agree that this presents a fundamental challenge to mainstream mindreading approaches. Drawing upon an analogy between embodied interaction and the exercise of expert skills, we advocate a hierarchical view which claims that embodied social responses generally operate in close conjunction with higher-level cognitive processes that play a coordinative role, and which are often sensitive to mental states. Thus, investigation of embodied responses should inform rather than conflict with research on mindreading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Evidence for implicit false belief understanding in children as young as 11 months obviously puts pressure on this argument (Michael 2011; for overviews of this research, see Apperly and Butterfill 2009; Bahrami et al. 2010).

  2. Which depends on how narrowly one understands ‘mindreading’. For discussion, see Gordon (2008).

  3. Although this characterization is obviously a gross oversimplification, it does serve to highlight the contrasting profiles that we are interested in here.

  4. This must be qualified: most of the research has focused on syntax, phonology and semantics, while relatively little work has been done on pragmatics (Cf. Brock et al. 2008).

  5. These results are in contrast with earlier findings reported by Karmiloff-Smith et al. (1995), who found that individuals with WS performed well on a series of theory of mind tests. However, the participants in Karmiloff-Smith’s earlier study ranged in age from 9 to 23 years—well beyond the age at which children generally pass such tasks. In Tager-Flusberg and Sullivan’s study (2000; see also Tager-Flusberg et al. 1997) the success rate for four- to nine-year-old children with WS was only 24–29 %—significantly lower than in the two comparison groups, even though the children with WS had higher verbal mental ages. The more recent findings are also corroborated by a further study in which children with WS were found to have greater difficulties (compared to the same control groups) in a task that involved reasoning about mental states, e.g. when asked to judge whether a character in a story was lying or joking (Sullivan et al. 2003). Moreover, in the same study, when asked to justify their interpretations , they made significantly fewer references to mental states than the children in the other two groups (Cf. Reilly et al. 2004 for similar results).

  6. It might be suggested that the joint attention deficits of individuals with WS show that they do not have fully developed ‘smart’ social-perceptual abilities. Admittedly, such a response cannot be ruled out, but it would be hostage to empirical findings that are not entirely straightforward. Note, first of all, that not all studies have found joint attention deficits in children and adults with WS. Gyori et al. (2004) tested 14 participants with WS (9–22 years) and found that they were almost as good as controls at following a speaker’s gaze when determining the object to which the speaker was referring. Where children with WS seem above all to differ from typically developing children is in the use of, and response to, the pointing gesture (Laing et al. 2002). In part, this may simply reflect the fact that they are much less interested in objects than in people (Laing et al. 2002, p. 239; cf. Mervis et al. 2003). Alternatively, it may reflect a difficulty inhibiting their tendency to look at faces, i.e. the kind of higher-level deficit we are suggesting is needed in order to make use of smart perception. Moreover, it might also be because pointing depends on social representational skills that go beyond social perception (Franco and Butterworth 1996). These findings would thus not necessarily undermine the suggestion that individuals with WS have relatively spared social-perceptual abilities of the sort highlighted by Gallagher. (Thanks to an anonymous referee for pressing this objection).

  7. This suggestion echoes the James-Lange theory of emotions: according to the James-Lange theory, one perceives one’s own bodily changes and thereby experiences an emotion; according to the present suggestion, one experiences one’s own bodily changes and thereby makes a judgment about the other person’s emotions or intentions.

References

  • Ackerman, J. M., Nocera, C. C., & Bargh, J. A. (2010). Incidental haptic sensations influence social judgments and decisions. Science, 328, 1712–1715.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, J. (1982). Acquisition of cognitive skill. Psychological Review, 89(4), 369–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andrews, G., Halford, G., Bunch, K., Bowden, D., & Jones, T. (2003). Concept of mind and relational complexity. Child Development, 74(5), 1476–1499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anisfeld, M. (1991). Neonatal imitation. Developmental Review, 11, 60–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. (2011). Mindreaders: The cognitive basis of “theory of mind”. Hove: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, A. B. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116, 953–970.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Latham, P. E., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., & Frith, C. D. (2010). Optimally interacting minds. Science, 329, 1081–1085.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baldwin, D., & Moses, L. (1994). Early understanding of referential intent and attentional focus: Evidence from language and emotion. In C. Lewis & P. Mitchell (Eds.), Origins of an understanding of mind (pp. 365–373). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Becchio, C., Cavallo, A., Begliomini, C., Sartori, L., Feltrin, G., & Castiello, U. (2012). Social grasping: From mirroring to mentalizing. NeuroImage, 61(1), 240–248.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 701–725.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bernstein, N. (1967). The co-ordination and regulation of movements. London: Pergamon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birch, S., & Bloom, P. (2007). The curse of knowledge in reasoning about false beliefs. Psychological Science, 9, 267–277.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bischof-Köhler, D. (1991). The development of empathy in infants. In M. E. Lamb & H. Keller (Eds.), Infant development: Perspectives from German speaking countries (pp. 245–273). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bogart, K., & Matsumoto, D. (2010a). Facial mimicry is not necessary to recognize emotion: Facial expression recognition by people with Moebius syndrome. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 241–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogart, K., & Matsumoto, D. (2010b). Living with Moebius Syndrome: Adjustment, social competence, and satisfaction with life. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal, 47(2), 134–142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bogart, K. R., Tickle-Degnen, L., & Ambady, N. (2012). Compensatory expressive behavior for facial paralysis: Adaptation to congenital or acquired disability. Rehabilitation Psychology, 57(1), 43–51.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bourgeois, P., & Hess, U. (2008). The impact of social context on mimicry. Biological Psychology, 77(3), 343–352. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.11.008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Briegel, W. (2007). Psychopathology and personality aspects of adults with Moebius sequence. Clinical Genetics, 71(4), 376–377.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Einav, S., & Riby, D. (2008). The other end of the spectrum? Social cognition in Williams syndrome. In T. Striano & V. Reid (Eds.), Social cognition: Development, neuroscience and autism (pp. 281–300). Oxford: Blackwell.

  • Butterfill, S., & Apperly, I. (2013). How to construct a minimal theory of mind. Mind and Language, 28 (in press)

  • Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts (1st ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Carlton, S., & Moses, L. (2001). Individual differences in inhibitory control and children’s theory of mind. Child Development, 72, 1032–1053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chartrand, T., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893–910.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaimov, K., Hernandez, S., KokEnnen, S., Krull, E., & Rives Bogart, K. (2011). Factors impacting social interaction in moebius syndrome. Presented at the Massachussetts Association for Occupational Therapy Conference, Norwood, MA.

  • Cheng, C. M., & Chartrand, T. L. (2003). Self-monitoring without awareness: Using mimicry as a nonconscious affiliation strategy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1170–1179. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Christensen, W. D., Sutton, J., McIlwain, D., & Geeves, A. (forthcoming). Cognitive control in skilled action.

  • Clark, H. H. (1996). Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2009). Natural Pedagogy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(4), 148–153.

  • De Jaegher, H. (2009). Social understanding through direct perception? Yes, by interacting. Consciousness and Cognition 18, 535–542.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485–507. doi:10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9.

    Google Scholar 

  • De Jaegher, H., Di Paolo, E., & Gallagher, S. (2010). Can social interaction constitute social cognition? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 441–447. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.06.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Vignemont, F., & Singer, T. (2006). The empathic brain: How, when and why? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(19), 535–541.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dion, K., Berscheid, E., & Walster, E. (1972). What is beautiful is good. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 24, 285–290.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2007a). Response to McDowell. Inquiry, 50, 371–377, p. 374, referring to Sartre, J. P. (1957) The transcendence of the ego (New York: Noonday Press, 1957), pp. 8–49.

  • Dreyfus, H. L. (2007b). The Return of the Myth of the Mental. Inquiry, 50, 352–365.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dykens, E. M. (2003). Anxiety, fears, and phobias in persons with Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 291–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), Handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., pp. 701–778). New York: Wiley.

  • Ericsson, K. A., & Kintsch, W. (1995). Long-term working memory. Psychological Review, 102, 211–244.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Evans, J. St B. T. (2008). Dual-processing accounts of reasoning, judgment, and social cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 255–278.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Farroni, T., Csibra, G., Simion, F., & Johnson, M. (2002). Eye contact detection at birth. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99, 9602–9605.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fitts, P. M., & Posner, M. I. (1967). Human performance. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.

  • Fogas, J., & Bower, G. (1987). Mood effects on personal perception judgments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39, 175–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franco, F., & Butterworth, G. (1996). Pointing and social awareness: Declaring and pointing in the second year of life. Journal of Child Language, 23(2), 307–336.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frye, D., Zelazo, P., & Palfati, T. (1995). Theory of mind and rule-based reasoning. Cognitive Development, 10, 483–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive Intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8, 465–486.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fusaroli, R., Bahrami, B., Olsen, K., Roepstorff, A., Rees, G., Frith, C., & Tylén, K. (2012). Coming to terms: Quantifying the benefits of linguistic coordination. Psychological Science, 23(8), 931–939.

    Google Scholar 

  • Futó, J., Téglás, E., Csibra, G., & Gergely, G. (2010). Communicative function demonstration induces kind-based artifact representation in preverbal infants. Cognition, 117, 1–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gale, A., Spratt, G., Chapman, A. J., & Smallbone, A. (1975). EEG correlates of eye contact and interpersonal distance. Biological Psychology, 3(4), 237–245.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2001). The practice of mind: Theory, simulation, or primary interaction? Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8, 83–108.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S. (2008a). Inference or interaction: Social cognition without precursors. Philosophical Explorations: An International Journal for the Philosophy of Mind and Action, 11(3), 163–174. doi:10.1080/13869790802239227.

  • Gallagher, S. (2008b). Direct perception in the intersubjective context. Consciousness and Cognition, 17, 535–543.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, S., & Zahavi, D. (2008). The phenomenological mind. London: Routledge.

  • Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 8–11.

    Google Scholar 

  • Geangu, A., Bengu, O., Stahl, D., & Striano, T. (2010). Contagious crying beyond the first days of life. Infant Behavioural Development, 33(3), 279–288.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gergely, G., Bekkering, H., & Király, I. (2002). Rational imitation in preverbal infants. Nature, 415, 755.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gordon, R. (2008). Beyond mindreading. Philosophical explorations, 11, 219–222.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Greene, J., Sommerville, R., Nystrom, L., Darley, J., & Cohen, J. (2001). An fMRI investigation of emotional engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 2105–2108.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gyori, M., Lukacs, A., & Pleh, C. (2004). Towards the understanding of the neurogenesis of social cognition: Evidence from impaired populations. Journal of Cultural and Evolutionary Psychology, 2, 261–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Haruno, M., Wolpert, D. M., & Kawato, M. (2003). Hierarchical MOSAIC for movement generation. In International congress series, MCCL (pp. 575–590). Amsterdam: Elsevier. http://linkinghub.elsevier.com/retrieve/pii/S0531513103001900.

  • Hatfield, E., Cacioppo, J. T., & Rapson, R. L. (1994). Emotional contagion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann, E., Call, J., Hare, B., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Humans evolved specialized skills of social cognition: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Science, 317(5843), 1360–1366.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hess, U., & Bourgeois, P. (2010). You smile-I smile: Emotion expression in social interaction. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 514–520. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.11.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2001). Causes and consequences of imitation. Trends in Cognitive Science, 5(6), 253–261.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hietanen, J., Leppanen, J., Peltola, M., Linna-aho, K., & Ruuhiala, H. (2008). Seeing direct and averted gaze activates the approach-avoidance motivational brain systems. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2423–2430.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (2002). The cradle of thought. London: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hood, B., Willen, J., & Driver, J. (1998). Adults’ eyes trigger shifts of visual attention in human infants. Psychological Science, 9(2), 131–134.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutto, D. (2012). Elementary mind minding, enactivist style. In A. Seemans (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind and social neuroscience (pp. 307–342). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A., Klima, E., Bellugi, U., Grant, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Is there a social module? Language, face processing and theory of mind in individuals with Williams syndrome. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 196–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klein-Tasman, B., Phillips, K. D., Lord, C. E., Mervis, C. B., & Gallo, F. (2009). Overlap WITH the autism spectrum in young children with Williams Syndrome. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 30, 289–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Krueger, J., & Michael, J. (2012). Gestural coupling and social cognition: Moebius Syndrome as a case study. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 81. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00081.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kugiumutzakis, G. (1999). Genesis and development of early infant mimesis to facial and vocal models. In J. Nadel & G. Butterworth (Eds.), 1999: Imitation in infancy. Cambridge studies in cognitive perceptual development (pp. 36–59). New York: Cambridge University Press.

  • Laing, E., Butterworth, G., Ansari, D., Gsödl, M., Longhi, E., Panagiotaki, G., et al. (2002). Atypical development of language and social communication in toddlers with Williams syndrome. Developmental Science, 5, 233–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–131). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Laws, G., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2004). Pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in Williams syndrome: A comparison with Down’s syndrome and specific language impairment. International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 39, 45–64.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leavitt, J. L. (1979). Cognitive demands of skating and stickhandling in Ice Hockey. Canadian Journal of Applied Sport Sciences, 4, 46–55. Accessed June 16, 2012, from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/498401.

  • Leslie, A., Friedman, O., & German, T. (2004). Core mechanisms in theory of mind. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(12), 528–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liepelt, R., & Brass, M. (2010). Top-down modulation of motor priming by belief about animacy. Experimental Psychology, 57, 221–227.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Logan, G. D. (1985). Skill and automaticity: Relations, implications, and future directions. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 39, 367–386. doi:10.1037/h0080066.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacKay, D. G. (1982). The problems of flexibility, fluency, and speed-accuracy trade-off in skilled behavior. Psychological Review, 89(5), 483–506. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.89.5.483.

  • McHugo, G. J., Lanzetta, J. T., & Bush, L. K. (1991). The effect of attitudes on emotional reactions to expressive displays of political leaders. Journal Nonverbal Behav, 15, 19–41.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N. (1995). Understanding the intentions of others: Re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children. Developmental Psychology, 31, 838–850.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A., & Moore, K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mervis, C. B., Morris, C. A., Klein, T., Bonita, P., Bertrand, J., Kwitny, S., et al. (2003). Attentional characteristics of infants and toddlers with Williams syndrome during triadic interactions. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 243–268.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyerson, M. D. (2001). Resiliency and success in adults with Moebius syndrome. The Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Journal: Official Publication of the American Cleft Palate-Craniofacial Association, 38(3), 231–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (2011). Interaction and mindreading. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 2(3), 559–578.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Moll, H., & Tomasello, M. (2004). 12- and 18-month-old infants follow gaze to spaces behind barriers. Developmental Science, 7, F1–F9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Müller, B., Kühn, S., van Baaren, R., Dotsch, R., Brass, M., & Dijksterhuis, A. (2011). Perspective-taking eliminated differences in co-representations of out-group members’ actions. Experimental Brain Research, 21(3–4), 423–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nichols, K., & Champness, B. (1971). Eye gaze and the GSR. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 7, 623–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., Dawson, G., & Munson, J. (2002). Early recognition of 1-year-old infants with autism spectrum disorder versus mental retardation. Development and Psychopathology, 14, 239–251.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pacherie, E. (2005). Perceiving intentions. In J. Sàágua (Ed.), A Explicaç ao da Interpretaç ao Humana (pp. 401–414). Lisbon: Ediç oes Colibri.

  • Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169–225.

    Google Scholar 

  • Prinz, J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford: OUP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ratcliffe, M. (2007). Rethinking commonsense psychology: A critique of folk psychology, theory of mind and simulation. Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2003). On being an object of attention: Implications for self-other-consciousness. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(9), 397–402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2008). How infants know minds. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Pres.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reddy, V. (2012). A gaze at grips with me. In A. Seemans (Ed.), Joint attention: New developments in psychology, philosophy of mind and social neuroscience (pp. 237–258). Cambridge: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, J., Losh, M., Bellugi, U., & Wulfeck, B. (2004). “Frog, where are you?” Narratives in children with specific language impairment, early focal brain injury, and Williams syndrome. Brain and Language, 88, 229–247.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenbaum, D. A., Cohen, R. G., Jax, S. A., Weiss, D. J., & van der Wel, R. (2007). The problem of serial order in behavior: Lashley’s legacy. Human Movement Science, 26, 525–554. doi:10.1016/j.humov.2007.04.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sagi, A., & Hoffman, M. L. (1976). Empathic distress in newborns. Developmental Psychology, 12, 175–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schnall, S., Haidt, J., & Clore, G. (2005). Irrelevant disgust makes moral judgment more severe, for those who listen to their bodies. Unpublished manuscript, University of Virginia, Charlottesville, VA.

  • Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2008). Gaze-following in human infants depends on communicative Signals. Developmental Science, 18(9), 678–671.

    Google Scholar 

  • Senju, A., Southgate, V., White, S., & Frith, U. (2009). Mindblind eyes: An absence of spontaneous theory of mind in Asperger Syndrome. Science, 325(5942), 883–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sohn, Y. W., & Doane, S. M. (2004). Memory processes of flight situation awareness: Interactive roles of working memory capacity, long-term working memory, and expertise. Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society, 46, 461–475.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spek, A. A., Scholte, E. M., & Van Berckelaer-Onnes, I. A. (2010). Theory of mind in adults with HFA and Asperger Syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40, 280–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stern, D. (1985/1998). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York: Basic Books.

  • Stojanovik, V. (2006). Social interaction deficits and conversational inadequacy in Williams syndrome. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 19, 157–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sullivan, K., Winner, E., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2003). Can adolescents with Williams syndrome tell the difference between lies and jokes? Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 85–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sutton, J., Macilwain, D., Christensen, W., & Geeves, A. (2011). Applying intelligence to the reflexes: Embodied skills and habits between Dreyfus and Descartes. The Journal of the British Society for Phenomenology, 42, 78–101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Sullivan, K. (2000). A componential view of theory of mind: Evidence from Williams syndrome. Cognition, 76, 59–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., Sullivan, K., & Boshart, J. (1997). Executive functions and performance on false belief tasks. Developmental Neuropsychology, 13, 487–493.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, J. A., & Ivry, R. B. (2012). The role of strategies in motor learning. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1251, 1–12. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06430.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teufel, C., Alexis, D. M., Todd, H., Lawrance-Owen, A. J., Clayton, N. S., & Davis, G. (2009). Social cognition modulates the sensory coding of observed gaze-direction. Current Biology, 19, 1274–1277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Teufel, C., Fletcher, P. C., & Davis, G. (2010). Seeing other minds: Attributed mental states influence perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14, 376–382.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomasello, M., Carpenter, M., Call, J., Behne, T., & Moll, H. (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28, 675–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Topal, J., Gergely, G., Miklose, A., Erdohegyi, E., & Csibra, G. (2008). Infants’ perseverative search errors are induced by pragmatic misinterpretation. Science, 321, 1831–1834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Trevarthen, C. (1979). Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waag, W. L., & Bell, H. H. (2009). Situation assessment and decision making in skilled fighter pilots. In G. A. Klein (Ed.), Caroline, E Zsambock (pp. 247–254). Naturalistic decision making. New York: Routledge.

  • Weeks, S. J., & Hobson, R. P. (1992). The salience of facial expression for autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 28, 137–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wulf, G. (2007). Attention and motor skill learning. Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yoon, J. M. D., Johnson, M. H., & Csibra, G. (2008). Communication-induced memory biases in preverbal infants. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 13690–13695.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., & Radke-Yarrow, M. (1982). The development of altruism: Alternative research strategies. In N. Eisenberg (Ed.), The development of prosocial behavior (pp. 109–137). New York: Academic Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., & King, R. A. (1979). Child-rearing and children’s prosocial initiations toward victims of distress. Child Development, 50, 319–330.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zahn-Waxler, C., Radke-Yarrow, M., Wagner, E., & Chapman, M. (1992). Development of concern for others. Developmental Psychology, 28(1), 126–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to John Michael.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Michael, J., Christensen, W. & Overgaard, S. Mindreading as social expertise. Synthese 191, 817–840 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-013-0295-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-013-0295-z

Keywords

Navigation