Skip to main content

Advertisement

Log in

Components of reading comprehension in adolescent first-language and second-language students from low-track schools

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Based on the Simple View of Reading, the present study investigates whether component skills of reading differ between adolescents from low-track schools speaking German as a first (L1) versus as a second language (L2). In our study, 479 9th-grade students completed the Programme for International Student Assessment reading test as well as measures of working memory, reading fluency, reading vocabulary, morpho-syntactic skills, phonological awareness, and listening comprehension. Results indicate that L2 students lagged behind their L1 peers on most measures. After controlling for students’ socioeconomic status, only L2 students with Turkish as a first language continued to show weaknesses in linguistic skills. Linguistic comprehension as well as reading fluency were associated with reading comprehension in all groups of students. We observed only a few group differences in the relationships between component skills of reading and reading comprehension. The results suggest that reading fluency is important for reading comprehension of L1 and L2 readers from low-track schools and that socioeconomic background partially accounts for group differences in component skills of reading.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aaron, P. G., Joshi, R. M., Gooden, R., & Bentum, K. E. (2008). Diagnosis and treatment of reading disabilities based on the component model of reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41, 67–84. doi:10.1177/0022219407310838.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Adams, A.-M., Bourke, L., & Willis, M. (1999). Working memory and spoken language comprehension in young children. International Journal of Psychology, 34, 364–373. doi:10.1080/002075999399701.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • August, D., & Shanahan, T. (Eds.). (2006). Developing literacy in second-language learners: Report of the national literacy panel on language-minority children and youth. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Backus, A. (2005). Turkish as an immigrant language in Europe. In J. F. Kroll & A. M. B. De Groot (Eds.), Handbook of bilingualism: Psycholinguistic approaches (pp. 689–723). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Clarendon Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baumgartner, H., & Steenkamp, J.-B. E. M. (1998). Multi-group latent variable models for varying numbers of items and factors with cross-national and longitudinal applications. Marketing Letters, 9, 21–35. doi:10.1023/A:1007911903032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Becker, R. & Beck, M. (2011). Migration, Sprachförderung und soziale Integration. Eine Evaluation der Sprachförderung von Berliner Schulkindern mit Migrationshintergrund anhand von ELEMENT-Panel-Daten [Migration, language promotion, and social integration. An evaluation of language promotion of immigrant students in Berlin using the ELEMENT panel data]. In R. Becker (Ed.), Integration durch Bildung. Bildungserwerb von jungen Migranten in Deutschland (pp. 121–138). Wiesbaden: VS Verlag.

  • Bialystok, E. (1979). Explicit and implicit judgements of L2 grammaticality. Language Learning, 29, 81–103. doi:10.1111/j.1467-1770.1979.tb01053.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E. (2001). Bilingualism in development. Language, literacy & cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Bialystok, E., Luk, G., & Kwan, E. (2005). Bilingualism, biliteracy, and learning to read: Interactions among languages and writing systems. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 43–61. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr0901_4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buly, M. R., & Valencia, S. W. (2002). Below the bar: Profiles of students who fail state reading. Educational Evaluation and Policy Analysis, 24, 219–239. doi:10.3102/01623737024003219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cain, K., Oakhill, J., & Bryant, P. (2004). Children’s reading comprehension ability: Concurrent prediction by working memory, verbal activity, and component skills. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 31–42. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.96.1.31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Crosson, A. C., & Lesaux, N. K. (2010). Revisiting assumptions about the relationship of fluent reading to comprehension: Spanish-speakers’ text-reading fluency in English. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 23, 475–494. doi:10.1007/s11145-009-9168-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Scarborough, H. S. (2006). Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 277–299. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1003_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2003). Language proficiency and reading ability in first- and second-language learners. Reading Research Quarterly, 38, 78–103. doi:10.1598/RRQ.38.1.4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1995). Stages of development in learning to read words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 116–125. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9817.1995.tb00077.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fiske, S. T., Lin, M., & Neuberg, S. (1999). The continuum model. In S. Chaiken & Y. Trope (Eds.), Dual process theories in social psychology (pp. 254–321). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., & Cain, K. (2011). The simple view of reading: Is it valid for different types of alphabetic orthographies? Educational Psychology Review, 23, 553–576. doi:10.1007/s10648-011-9175-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Florit, E., Roch, M., & Levorato, M. C. (2014). Listening text comprehension in preschoolers: A longitudinal study on the role of semantic components. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 27, 793–817. doi:10.1007/s11145-013-9464-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, V. C. M. (2002). Monolingual and bilingual acquisition: Learning different treatments of that-trace phenomena in English and Spanish. In D. K. Oller & R. E. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 220–252). Clevedon, UK: Multilingual Matters.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A. D. (1989). Evaluation of the role of phonological STM in the development of vocabulary in children: A longitudinal study. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 200–213. doi:10.1016/0749-596X(89)90044-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Geva, E., & Farnia, F. (2012). Developmental changes in the nature of language proficiency and reading fluency paint a more complex view of reading comprehension in ELL and EL1. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1819–1845. doi:10.1007/s11145-011-9333-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Glock, S., & Krolak-Schwerdt, S. (2013). Does nationality matter? The impact of stereotypical expectations on student teachers’ judgments. Social Psychology of Education, 16, 111–127. doi:10.1007/s11218-012-9197-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gogolin, I. (2005). Bilingual education—the German experience and debate. In Arbeitsstelle Interkulturelle Konflikte und gesellschaftliche Integration (Ed.), The effectiveness of bilingual school programs for immigrant children (pp. 133–145). Berlin: Wissenschaftszentrum Berlin für Sozialforschung (WZB).

    Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., & Mueller, J. (2009). Are first- and second-language factors related in predicting second-language reading comprehension? A study of spanish-speaking children acquiring English as a second language from first to second grade. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 330–344. doi:10.1037/a0014320.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B., & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability. Remedial & Special Education, 7, 6–10. doi:10.1177/074193258600700104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experiences of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heller, K. A., & Perleth, C. (2000). Kognitiver Fähigkeitstest für 4. bis 12. Klassen, Revision: KFT 4-12+R [Test of cognitive abilities for grades 4 to 12]. Weinheim: Beltz.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoover, W. A., & Gough, P. B. (1990). The simple view of reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2, 127–160. doi:10.1007/BF00401799.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, L.-T., & Bentler, P. M. (1999). Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Structural Equation Modeling, 6, 1–55. doi:10.1080/10705519909540118.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jenkins, J. R., Fuchs, L. S., van den Broek, P., Espin, C., & Deno, S. L. (2003). Sources of individual differences in reading comprehension and reading fluency. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 719–729. doi:10.1037/0022-0663.95.4.719.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jussim, L., & Harber, K. D. (2005). Teacher expectations and self-fulfilling prophecies: Knowns and unknowns, resolved and unresolved controversies. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 9, 131–155. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0902_3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kahraman, B., & Knoblich, G. (2000). Stechen statt sprechen: Valenz und Aktivierbarkeit von Stereotypen über Türken [Pricking instead of talking. Valence and activability of stereotypes about Turks]. Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 31, 31–43. doi:10.1024//0044-3514.31.1.31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Socioeconomic status, English proficiency, and late-emerging reading difficulties. Educational Researcher, 39, 484–486. doi:10.3102/0013189X10378400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kim, S., & Hagtvet, K. A. (2003). The impact of misspecified item parceling on representing latent variables in covariance structure modeling: A simulation study. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 10, 101–127. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM1001_5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., & Savage, R. S. (2008). Can the simple view deal with the complexities of reading? Literacy, 42, 75–82. doi:10.1111/j.1741-4369.2008.00487.x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klieme, E., Artelt, C., Hartig, J., Jude, N., Köller, O., Prenzel, M., et al. (Eds.). (2010). PISA 2009—Bilanz nach einem Jahrzehnt [PISA 2009—balance after a decade]. Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., & Kieffer, M. J. (2010). Exploring sources of reading comprehension difficulties among language minority learners and their classmates in early adolescence. American Educational Research Journal, 47, 596–632. doi:10.3102/0002831209355469.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lesaux, N. K., Lipka, O., & Siegel, L. S. (2006). Investigating cognitive and linguistic abilities that influence the reading comprehension skills of children from diverse linguistic backgrounds. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 19, 99–131. doi:10.1007/s11145-005-4713-6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Little, T. D., Cunningham, W. A., Shahar, G., & Widaman, K. F. (2002). To parcel or not to parcel: Exploring the question, weighing the merits. Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 9, 151–173. doi:10.1207/S15328007SEM0902_1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Meyer, M. S., & Felton, R. H. (1999). Repeated reading to enhance fluency: Old approaches and new directions. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 283–306. doi:10.1007/s11881-999-0027-8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Muthén, L. K., & Muthén, B. O. (1998–2012). MPlus Version 7.11. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén & Muthén.

  • Nelson, T. D. (2002). The psychology of prejudice. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Netten, A., Droop, M., & Verhoeven, L. (2011). Predictors of reading literacy for first and second language learners. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 24, 413–425. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9234-2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Noble, K. G., Farah, M. J., & McCandliss, B. D. (2006). Socioeconomic background modulates cognition–achievement relationships in reading. Cognitive Development, 21, 349–368. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.01.007.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2010). PISA 2009 results: Overcoming social background—equity in learning opportunities and outcomes (Vol. II). Paris: OECD Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • OECD. (2012). PISA 2009 Technical Report. Paris: OECD Publishing.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Olsthoorn, N. M., Andringa, S., & Hulstijn, J. H. (2014). Visual and auditory digit-span performance in native and nonnative speakers. International Journal of Bilingualism, 18, 663–673. doi:10.1177/1367006912466314.

  • Ready, D. D., & Wright, D. L. (2011). Accuracy and inaccuracy in teachers’ perceptions of young children’s cognitive abilities. The role of child background and classroom context. American Educational Research Journal, 48, 335–360. doi:10.3102/0002831210374874.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Riches, C., & Genesee, F. (2006). Literacy Crosslinguistic and crossmodal issues. In F. Genesee, K. Lindholm-Leary, W. M. Saunders, & D. Christian (Eds.), Educating English language learners. A synthesis of research evidence (pp. 64–109). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R. (2006). Reading comprehension is not always the product of nonsense word decoding and linguistic comprehension: Evidence from teenagers who are extremely poor readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 143–164. doi:10.1207/s1532799xssr1002_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, W., Schlagmüller, M., & Ennemoser, M. (2007). Lesegeschwindigkeits- und Verständnistest für die Klassen 6-12 (LGVT 6-12) [Test of reading fluency and accuracy]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (2008). On the anglocentricities of current reading research and practice: The perils of overreliance on an “outlier” orthography. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 584–615. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.4.584.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., & Ryan, E. B. (1988). Development of grammatical-sensitivity, phonological, and short-term memory skills in normally achieving and learning disabled children. Developmental Psychology, 24, 28–37. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.24.1.28.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Söhn, J. (2005). Zweisprachiger Schulunterricht für Migrantenkinder. Ergebnisse der Evaluationsforschung zu seinen Auswirkungen auf Zweitspracherwerb und Schulerfolg [Bilingual education for immigrant students. Results from evaluation research concerning effects on second-language acquisition and educational outcomes]. Berlin: Wissenschaftszentrum für Sozialforschung.

  • Sprietsma, M. (2013). Discrimination in grading: Experimental evidence from primary school teachers. Empirical Economics, 45, 523–538. doi:10.1007/s00181-012-0609-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stanat, P., Rauch, D., & Segeritz, M. (2010). Schülerinnen und Schüler mit Migrationshintergrund [Immigrant students]. In E. Klieme, C. Artelt, J. Hartig, N. Jude, O. Köller, M. Prenzel, W. Schneider, & P. Stanat (Eds.), PISA 2009: Bilanz nach einem Jahrzehnt (pp. 200–230). Münster: Waxmann.

    Google Scholar 

  • Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs of the Länder in the Federal Republic of Germany (KMK). (2013). The education system in the Federal Republic of Germany 2011/2012. Bonn: Secretariat of the Standing Conference of the Ministers of Education and Cultural Affairs of the Länder in the Federal Republic of Germany.

  • Verhoeven, L., & van Leeuwe, J. (2012). The simple view of second language reading throughout the primary grades. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 25, 1805–1818. doi:10.1007/s11145-011-9346-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weiß, R. H. (2006). Grundintelligenztest Skala 2, CFT 20 - Revision (CFT 20-R) [Test of general cognitive abilities - Scale 2]. Göttingen: Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wiley, H. I., & Deno, S. L. (2005). Oral reading and maze measures as predictors of success for English learners on a state standards assessment. Remedial and Special Education, 26, 207–214. doi:10.1177/07419325050260040301.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Katzir-Cohen, T. (2001). Reading fluency and its intervention. Scientific Studies of Reading, 5, 211–239. doi:10.1207/S1532799XSSR0503_2.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Alexandra Marx.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Marx, A., Stanat, P., Roick, T. et al. Components of reading comprehension in adolescent first-language and second-language students from low-track schools. Read Writ 28, 891–914 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9554-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-015-9554-3

Keywords

Navigation