Skip to main content
Log in

Brief Report: Cognitive Control of Social and Nonsocial Visual Attention in Autism

  • Brief Report
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prosaccade and antisaccade errors in the context of social and nonsocial stimuli were investigated in youth with autism spectrum disorder (ASD; n = 19) a matched control sample (n = 19), and a small sample of youth with obsessive compulsive disorder (n = 9). Groups did not differ in error rates in the prosaccade condition for any stimulus category. In the antisaccade condition, the ASD group demonstrated more errors than the control group for nonsocial stimuli related to circumscribed interests, but not for other nonsocial stimuli or for social stimuli. Additionally, antisaccade error rates were predictive of core ASD symptom severity. Results indicate that the cognitive control of visual attention in ASD is impaired specifically in the context of nonsocial stimuli related to circumscribed interests.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

References

  • Agam, Y., Joseph, R. M., Barton, J. J. S., & Manoach, D. S. (2010). Reduced cognitive control of response inhibition by the anterior cingulate cortex in autism spectrum disorders. NeuroImage, 52(1), 336–347. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.010.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boraston, Z., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2007). The application of eye-tracking technology in the study of autism. The Journal of Physiology, 581(3), 893–898. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2007.133587.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Boyd, B. A., Conroy, M. A., Mancil, G. R., Nakao, T., & Alter, P. J. (2007). Effects of circumscribed interests on the social behaviors of children with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(8), 1550–1561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Chawarska, K., Klin, A., Paul, R., Macari, S., & Volkmar, F. (2009). A prospective study of toddlers with ASD: Short-term diagnostic and cognitive outcomes. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 50(10), 1235–1245. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02101.x.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., & Gruber, C. P. (2002). The Social Responsiveness Scale. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cuthbert, B. N., & Insel, T. R. (2013). Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: The seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Medicine, 11, 126.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., & Bodfish, J. W. (2009). Autism is characterized by dorsal anterior cingulate hyperactivation during social target detection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. doi:10.1093/scan/nsp017.

    PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dichter, G. S., Felder, J. N., Green, S. R., Rittenberg, A. M., Sasson, N. J., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Reward circuitry function in autism spectrum disorders. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7, 160–172. doi:10.1093/scan/nsq095.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47(1), 248–257. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.016.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gotham, K., Risi, S., Dawson, G., Tager-Flusberg, H., Joseph, R., Carter, A., et al. (2008). A replication of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) revised algorithms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(6), 642–651.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Grelotti, D. J., Klin, A. J., Gauthier, I., Skudlarski, P., Cohen, D. J., Gore, J. C., et al. (2005). fMRI activation of the fusiform gyrus and amygdala to cartoon characters but not to faces in a boy with autism. Neuropsychologia, 43(3), 373–385. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.06.015.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Howell, D. C., Rogier, M., Yzerbyt, V., & Bestgen, Y. (1998). Statistical methods in human sciences. New York: Wadsworth.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutton, S. B. (2008). Cognitive control of saccadic eye movements. Brain and Cognition, 68(3), 327–340. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2008.08.021.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jacob, S., Landeros-Weisenberger, A., & Leckman, J. F. (2009). Autism spectrum and obsessive-compulsive disorders: OC behaviors, phenotypes and genetics. Autism Research, 2(6), 293–311.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Jemel, B., Mottron, L., & Dawson, M. (2006). Impaired face processing in autism: Fact or artifact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 91–106. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0050-5.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, W., & Klin, A. (2013). Attention to eyes is present but in decline in 2–6-month-old infants later diagnosed with autism. Nature, 504(7480), 427–431. doi:10.1038/nature12715.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Katarzyna, C., Fred, V., & Ami, K. (2010). Limited attentional bias for faces in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(2), 178. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.194.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, J., Birmaher, B., Brent, D., Rao, U. M. A., Flynn, C., Moreci, P., et al. (1997). Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(7), 980–988.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, N. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intellifence Test (2nd ed.). Bloomington, MN: Pearson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59(9), 809. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lopez, B. R., Lincoln, A. J., Ozonoff, S., & Lai, Z. (2005). Examining the relationship between executive functions and restricted, repetitive symptoms of autistic disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(4), 445–460. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-5035-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., DiLavore, P., Risi, S., Gotham, K., & Bishop, S. (2012). Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS-2) (2nd ed.). Torrance, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Doll, S. K., Hegedus, S. J., Minshew, N. J., & Sweeney, J. A. (2007). Maturation of executive function in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 61(4), 474–481. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.02.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luna, B., Garver, K. E., Urban, T. A., Lazar, N. A., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Maturation of cognitive processes from late childhood to adulthood. Child Development, 75(5), 1357–1372. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00745.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Manoach, D. S., Lindgren, K. A., & Barton, J. J. S. (2004). Deficient saccadic inhibition in Asperger’s disorder and the social-emotional processing disorder. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 75(12), 1719–1726. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2003.025981.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McMahon, C. M., & Henderson, H. A. (2014). Error-monitoring in response to social stimuli in individuals with higher-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Science. doi:10.1111/desc.12220.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mosconi, M. W., Kay, M., D’Cruz, A.-M., Seidenfeld, A., Guter, S., Stanford, L. D., & Sweeney, J. A. (2009). Impaired inhibitory control is associated with higher-order repetitive behaviors in autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Medicine, 39(9), 1559–1566. doi:10.1017/S0033291708004984.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mostert-Kerckhoffs, M. A. L., Staal, W. G., Houben, R. H., & de Jonge, M. V. (2015). Stop and change: Inhibition and flexibility skills are related to repetitive behavior in children and young adults with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(10), 3148–3158. doi:10.1007/s10803-015-2473-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sabatino, A., Rittenberg, A., Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L., Bodfish, J. W., & Dichter, G. S. (2013). Functional neuroimaging of social and nonsocial cognitive control in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 43(12), 2903–2913. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1837-4.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2012). Affective responses by adults with autism are reduced to social images but elevated to images related to circumscribed interests. PLoS ONE, 7(8), e42457. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0042457.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., Elison, J. T., Turner-Brown, L. M., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Brief report: Circumscribed attention in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(2), 242–247. doi:10.1007/s10803-010-1038-3.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., & Touchstone, E. W. (2014). Visual attention to competing social and object images by preschool children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 44(3), 584–592. doi:10.1007/s10803-013-1910-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sasson, N. J., Turner-Brown, L. M., Holtzclaw, T. N., Lam, K. S. L., & Bodfish, J. W. (2008). Children with autism demonstrate circumscribed attention during passive viewing of complex social and nonsocial picture arrays. Autism Research. doi:10.1002/aur.4.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Scahill, L., Riddle, M. A., McSwiggin-Hardin, M., Ort, S. I., King, R. A., Goodman, W. K., et al. (1997). Children’s Yale-Brown obsessive compulsive scale: reliability and validity. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36(6), 844–852. doi:10.1097/00004583-199706000-00023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shin, N. Y., Lee, T. Y., Kim, E., & Kwon, J. S. (2014). Cognitive functioning in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A meta-analysis. Psychological medicine, 44(6), 1121–1130.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • South, M., Ozonoff, S., & McMahon, W. M. (2005). Repetitive behavior profiles in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35(2), 145–158. doi:10.1007/s10803-004-1992-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tottenham, N., Tanaka, J. W., Leon, A. C., McCarry, T., Nurse, M., Hare, T. A., et al. (2009). The NimStim set of facial expressions: Judgments from untrained research participants. Psychiatry Research, 168(3), 242–249. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.05.006.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Turner, M. A. (1999). Generating novel ideas: Fluency performance in high-functioning and learning disabled individuals with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40(2), 189–201. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00432.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Turner-Brown, L. M., Lam, K. S. L., Holtzclaw, T. N., Dichter, G. S., & Bodfish, J. W. (2011). Phenomenology and measurement of circumscribed interests in autism spectrum disorders. Autism, 15(4), 437–456. doi:10.1177/1362361310386507.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Turner-Brown, L. M., Perry, T. D., Dichter, G. S., Bodfish, J. W., & Penn, D. L. (2008). Brief report: Feasibility of social cognition and interaction training for adults with high functioning autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(9), 1777–1784. doi:10.1007/s10803-008-0545-y.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, K. K., Miller, S., Hannah, E., Kovac, M., Damiano, C. R., Sabatino-DiCrisco, A., et al. (2015). Increased reward value of non-social stimuli in children and adolescents with autism. Frontiers in Psychology. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01026.

    PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to extend our sincere gratitude to the families who participated in this study. This research was supported by MH081285, MH073402, HD079124, by a UNC Jessie Ball DuPont Dissertation Fellowship to MK and by a UNC Graduate School Summer Research Fellowship to ASD.

Author Contributions

The first author, ASD, worked closely with GD regarding the conception, design, analysis, and interpretation of the data. SJM, EH, and MK made contributed substantially with data acquistion. Authors made significant contributions to drafting or revising the article or contributed to the intellectual content. This study was a portion of the dissertation of ASD under the supervision of GD.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Antoinette Sabatino DiCriscio.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

DiCriscio, A.S., Miller, S.J., Hanna, E.K. et al. Brief Report: Cognitive Control of Social and Nonsocial Visual Attention in Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 2797–2805 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2804-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-016-2804-7

Keywords

Navigation