Skip to main content
Log in

From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two studies investigated whether typically developing children (TD) and children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) were able to decide whether two characters were communicating or not on the basis of point-light displays. Point-lights portrayed actors engaged or not in a social interaction. In study 1, TD children (4–10 years old; n = 36) grasped social intentions from body language, with a notable improvement around 7/8. In study 2, children with ASD (6–12 years old; n = 12) could categorize the point-light displays at above-chance levels, but performed less efficiently, especially for the social interaction displays, than TD children (matched to chronological and non-verbal mental age, 6–12 years old; n = 24). An action representation deficit is discussed in relation to a social representation deficit and it is suggested that these deficits might be linked to altered maturational process of the mirror system in ASD.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV), 4th edn. Washington, DC.

  • Asendorpf, J. B., & Baudonniere, P. M. (1993). Self-awareness and other-awareness—mirror self-recognition and synchronic imitation among unfamiliar peers. Developmental Psychology, 29, 88–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A. P. (2009). Impaired recognition of emotions from body movements is associated with elevated motion coherence thresholds in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3023–3029.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Atkinson, A. P., Dittrich, W. H., Gemmell, A. J., & Young, A. W. (2004). Emotion perception from dynamic and static body expressions in point-light and full-light displays. Perception, 33, 717–746.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bernier, R., Dawson, G., Webb, S., & Murias, M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain and Cognition, 64, 228–237.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertenthal, B. I., Proffitt, D. R., & Kramer, S. J. (1987). Perception of biomechanical motions by infants: Implementation of various processing constraints. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 13, 577–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: A “complex’’ issue. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grezes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2005). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1243–1249.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Nazarian, B., Anton, J. L., & Schmitz, C. (2011). Recruitment of both the mirror and the mentalizing networks when observing social interactions depicted by point-lights: A neuroimaging study. PLoS ONE, 6(1), e15749.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cross, E. S., Hamilton, A. F. D., Kraemer, D. J. M., Kelley, W. M., & Grafton, S. T. (2009). Dissociable substrates for body motion and physical experience in the human action observation network. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1383–1392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dakin, S., & Frith, U. (2005). Vagaries of visual perception in autism. Neuron, 48, 497–507.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Decety, J., & Grezes, J. (2006). The power of simulation: Imagining one’s own and other’s behavior. Brain Research, 1079, 4–14.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dehaene-Lambertz, G., & Baillet, S. (1998). A phonological representation in the infant brain. NeuroReport, 9, 1885–1888.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dittrich, W. H., Troscianko, T., Lea, S., & Morgan, D. (1996). Perception of emotion from dynamic point-light displays represented in dance. Perception, 25, 727–738.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Esposito, G., & Venuti, P. (2008). Analysis of toddlers’ gait after six months of independent walking to identify autism: A preliminary study. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 106, 259–269.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Falck-Ytter, T., Gredeback, G., & von Hofsten, C. (2006). Infants predict other people’s action goals. Nature Neuroscience, 9, 878–879.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Forssberg, H., Eliasson, A. C., Kinoshita, H., Johansson, R. S., & Westling, G. (1991). Development of human precision grip-I—basic coordination of force. Experimental Brain Research, 85, 451–457.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, R., & McDaniel, C. (1982). The perception of biological motion by human infants. Science, 218, 486–487.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Freitag, C. M., Konrad, C., Haberlen, M., Kleser, C., von Gontard, A., Reith, W., et al. (2008). Perception of biological motion in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 46, 1480–1494.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U., & Happe, F. (1994). Autism—beyond theory of mind. Cognition, 50, 115–132.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gallese, V. (2006). Intentional attunement: A neurophysiological perspective on social cognition and its disruption in autism. Brain Research, 1079, 15–24.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F. G. E., & Booth, R. D. L. (2008). The power of the positive: Revisiting weak coherence in autism spectrum disorders. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 61, 50–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Happe, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2010a). Mesmerising mirror neurons. NeuroImage, 51, 789–791.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heyes, C. (2010b). Where do mirror neurons come from? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 34, 575–583.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hubert, B., Wicker, B., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., Duverger, H., Da Fonseca, D., et al. (2007). Brief report: Recognition of emotional and non-emotional biological motion in individuals with autistic spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1386–1392.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jeannerod, M. (2001). Neural simulation of action: A unifying mechanism for motor cognition. NeuroImage, 14, S103–S109.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johansson, G. (1973). Visual perception of biological motion and a model for its analysis. Perception and Psychophysics, 14, 201–211.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, S. A., Blaha, L. M., Houpt, J. W., & Townsend, J. T. (2010). Systems factorial technology provides new insights on global-local information processing in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 54, 53–72.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (1983). Kaufman assessment battery for children (K-ABC). MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kilner, J. M. (2009). Dissociable functional roles of the human action-observation network (Commentary on E. S. Cross et al.). European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 1382.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lepage, J. F., & Theoret, H. (2007). The mirror neuron system: Grasping others’ actions from birth? Developmental Science, 10, 513–523.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lombardo, M. V., Chakrabarti, B., Bullmore, E. T., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). Specialization of right temporo-parietal junction for mentalizing and its relation to social impairments in autism. NeuroImage, 56, 1832–1838.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Lecouteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised—a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Materna, S., Dicke, P. W., & Thier, P. (2008). The posterior superior temporal sulcus is involved in social communication not specific for the eyes. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2759–2765.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAleer, P., Kay, J., Pollick, F., & Rutherford, M. (2011). Intention perception in high functioning people with autism spectrum disorders using animacy displays derived from human actions. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41, 1053–1063.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198(4312), 75–78.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, D. G., Hobson, R., & Lee, A. (1997). Components of person perception: An investigation with autistic, non-autistic retarded and typically developing children and adolescents. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 15, 401–423.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morton, J., & Johnson, M. H. (1991). Conspec and conlern—a 2-process theory of infant face recognition. Psychological Review, 98, 164–181.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, P., Brady, N., Fitzgerald, M., & Troje, N. F. (2009). No evidence for impaired perception of biological motion in adults with autistic spectrum disorders. Neuropsychologia, 47, 3225–3235.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nystrom, P. (2008). The infant mirror neuron system studied with high density EEG. Social Neuroscience, 3, 334–347.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190–198.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2007). The simulating social mind: The role of the mirror neuron system and simulation in the social and communicative deficits of autism spectrum disorders. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 310–327.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., & Ramachandran, V. S. (2008). Preliminary evidence for deficits in multisensory integration in autism spectrum disorders: The mirror neuron hypothesis. Social Neuroscience, 3, 348–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Onishi, K. H., & Baillargeon, R. (2005). Do 15-month-old infants understand false beliefs? Science, 308, 255–258.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parron, C., Da Fonseca, D., Santos, A., Moore, D. G., Monfardini, E., & Deruelle, C. (2008). Recognition of biological motion in children with autistic spectrum disorders. Autism, 12, 261–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pavlova, M., Krageloh-Mann, I., Sokolov, A., & Birbaumer, N. (2001). Recognition of point-light biological motion displays by young children. Perception, 30, 925–933.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., & Gibson, L. Y. (2008). Investigating the functional integrity of the dorsal visual pathway in autism and dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 46, 2593–2596.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pelphrey, K. A., Morris, J. P., Michelich, C. R., Allison, T., & McCarthy, G. (2005). Functional anatomy of biological motion perception in posterior temporal cortex: An FMRI study of eye, mouth and hand movements. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1866–1876.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Dobler, V., Bell, S., & Davis, G. (2006). The microgenesis of global perception in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36, 107–116.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pollick, F. E., Paterson, H. M., Bruderlin, A., & Sanford, A. J. (2001). Perceiving affect from arm movement. Cognition, 82, B51–B61.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Provost, B., Lopez, B. R., & Heimerl, S. (2007). A comparison of motor delays in young children: Autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and developmental concerns. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 321–328.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., & Gallese, V. (2001). Neurophysiological mechanisms underlying the understanding and imitation of action. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 661–670.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2010). The functional role of the parieto-frontal mirror circuit: Interpretations and misinterpretations. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11, 264–274.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rochat, P. (2007). Intentional action arises from early reciprocal exchanges. Acta Psychologica, 124, 8–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, C., & Assaiante, C. (2008). Postural control in children with autism spectrum disorder. In M. Hadders-Algra & E. Brogren Carlberg (Eds.), Posture: A key issue in developmental disorders (pp. 253–269). London: Mac Keith Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, C., Martin, N., & Assaiante, C. (2002). Building anticipatory postural adjustment during childhood: A kinematic and electromyographic analysis of unloading in children from 4 to 8 years of age. Experimental Brain Research, 142, 354–364.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmitz, C., Martineau, J., Barthelemy, C., & Assaiante, C. (2003). Motor control and children with autism: Deficit of anticipatory function? Neuroscience Letters, 348, 17–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Simion, F., Regolin, L., & Bulf, H. (2008). A predisposition for biological motion in the newborn baby. Proceedings of the National academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 105, 809–813.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sommerville, J. A., Woodward, A. L., & Needham, A. (2005). Action experience alters 3-month-old infants’ perception of others’ actions. Cognition, 96, B1–B11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Southgate, V., Senju, A., & Csibra, G. (2007). Action anticipation through attribution of false belief by 2-year-olds. Psychological Science, 18, 587–592.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, C., Laine, F., Rodriguez, M., & Gepner, B. (2007). Slowing down presentation of facial movements and vocal sounds enhances facial expression recognition and induces facial-vocal imitation in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 1469–1484.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • van Elk, M., van Schie, H. T., Hunnius, S., Vesper, C., & Bekkering, H. (2008). You’ll never crawl alone: Neurophysiological evidence for experience-dependent motor resonance in infancy. NeuroImage, 43, 808–814.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vandenbroucke, M. W. G., Scholte, H. S., van Engeland, H., Lamme, V. A. F., & Kemner, C. (2008). Coherent versus component motion perception in autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38, 941–949.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vollm, B. A., Taylor, A. N. W., Richardson, P., Corcoran, R., Stirling, J., McKie, S., et al. (2006). Neuronal correlates of theory of mind and empathy: A functional magnetic resonance imaging study in a nonverbal task. NeuroImage, 29, 90–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • von Hofsten, C. (2007). Action in development. Developmental Science, 10, 54–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wang, L. X., Mottron, L., Berthiaume, C., & Dawson, M. (2007). Local bias and local-to-global interference without global deficit: A robust finding in autism under various conditions of attention, exposure time, and visual angle. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24, 550–574.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2006). Altruistic helping in human infants and young chimpanzees. Science, 311, 1301–1303.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1990). Wechsler preschool and primary scale of intelligence -revised (WPPSI-R): Short form vocabulary and block design. New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1996). Manual for intelligence scale for children (3rd ed.). New York: Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H. G., Whiten, A., Suddendorf, T., & Perrett, D. I. (2001). Imitation, mirror neurons and autism. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 287–295.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zivotofsky, A., & Hausdorff, J. (2007). The sensory feedback mechanisms enabling couples to walk synchronously: An initial investigation. Journal of NeuroEngineering and Rehabilitation, 4, 28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

Laurie Centelles has been awarded a doctoral fellowship from the Orange Foundation. The authors wish to thank Marianne Vaugoyeau for assistance with stimuli build-up.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Christina Schmitz.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Centelles, L., Assaiante, C., Etchegoyhen, K. et al. From Action to Interaction: Exploring the Contribution of Body Motion Cues to Social Understanding in Typical Development and in Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 43, 1140–1150 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-012-1655-0

Keywords

Navigation