Skip to main content
Log in

Slowing Down Presentation of Facial Movements and Vocal Sounds Enhances Facial Expression Recognition and Induces Facial–Vocal Imitation in Children with Autism

  • ORIGINAL PAPER
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the effects of slowing down presentation of facial expressions and their corresponding vocal sounds on facial expression recognition and facial and/or vocal imitation in children with autism. Twelve autistic children and twenty-four normal control children were presented with emotional and non-emotional facial expressions on CD-Rom, under audio or silent conditions, and under dynamic visual conditions (slowly, very slowly, at normal speed) plus a static control. Overall, children with autism showed lower performance in expression recognition and more induced facial–vocal imitation than controls. In the autistic group, facial expression recognition and induced facial–vocal imitation were significantly enhanced in slow conditions. Findings may give new perspectives for understanding and intervention for verbal and emotional perceptive and communicative impairments in autistic populations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1.
Fig. 2.
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. The TVAP (Active and Passive Vocabulary Test) is a french vocabulary test. It is made up of 30 sets of five pictures. These pictures represent either objects or actions. This test is divided in two subtests (Active and PassiveVocabulary subtests). In the Passive vocabulary subtest, the experimenter asks the child to point to one of the five pictures. This subtest assesses verbal comprehension level. In the Active Vocabulary subtest, the experimenter asks the child to give a definition of one of the five pictures. This subtest assesses verbal expression level. In our study, children with autism were matched with typically developing children using the passive Vocabulary subtest only.

References

  • American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (DSM-IV). 4th ed. Washington DC: APA.

    Google Scholar 

  • Babiloni, C., Bares, M., Vecchio, F., Brazdil, M., Jurak, P., Moretti, D. V., Ubaldi, A., Rossini, P. M., & Rektor, I. (2004). Synchronization of gamma oscillations increases functional connectivity of human hippocampus and inferior-middle temporal cortex during repetitive visuomotor events. European Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 3088–3098.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bailey, A., Luthert, P., Dean, A., Harding, B., Janota, I., Montgomery, M., Rutter, M., & Lantos, P. (1998). A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain, 121, 889–905.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Campbell, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A., & Grant, J. (1995). Are children with autism blind to the mentalistic significance of the eyes? British Journal of Devevelopmental Psychology, 13, 379–398.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S., Ring, H., Bullmore, E., Wheelwright, S., Ashwin, C., & Williams, S. (2000). The amygdala theory of autism. Neuroscience Biobehavioral Review, 24, 355–364.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bassili, J. N. (1979). Emotion recognition: the role of facial movement and the relative importance of upper and lower areas of the face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 37, 2049–2058.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Belmonte, M. K., Allen, G., Beckel-Mitchener, A., Boulanger, L. M., Carper, R. A., & Webb, S. J. (2004). Autism and abnormal development of brain connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 9228–9231.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2003). Motion perception in autism: a complex issue? Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 15, 218–225.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blake, R., Turner, L. M., Smoski, M. J., Pozdol, S. L., & Stone, W. L. (2003). Visual recognition of biological motion is impaired in children with autism. Psychological Science, 14, 151–157.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Borgers, C., & Kopell, N. (2003). Synchronization in networks of excitatory and inhibitory neurons with sparse, random connectivity. Neural Computers, 15, 509–538.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boucher, J., & Lewis, V. (1992). Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 33, 843–859.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Breakspear, M. (2004). “Dynamic” connectivity in neural systems: theoretical and empirical considerations. Neuroinformatics, 2, 205–226.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brock, J., Brown, C. C., Boucher, J., & Rippon, G. (2002). The temporal binding deficit hypothesis of autism. Developmental Psychopathology, 14, 209–224.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brown, C., Gruber, T., Boucher, J., Rippon, G., & Brock, J. (2005). Gamma abnormalities during perception of illusory figures in autism. Cortex, 41, 364–376.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Castelli, F., Frith, C., Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2002). Autism, Asperger syndrome and brain mechanisms for the attribution of mental states to animated shapes. Brain, 125, 1839–1849.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Celani, G., Battacchi, M. W., & Arcidiacono, L. (1999). The understanding of the emotional meaning of facial expressions in people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 57–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, I. L. (1994). An artificial neural network analogue of learning in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 36, 5–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E. (1997). Brainstem, cerebellar and limbic neuroanatomical abnormalities in autism. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 7, 269–278.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., Saitoh, O., Yeung-Courchesne, R., Press, G. A., Lincoln, A. J., Haas, R. H., & Schreibman, L. (1994). Abnormality of cerebellar vermian lobules VI and VII in patients with infantile autism: identification of hypoplastic and hyperplastic subgroups with MR imaging. American Journal of Roentgenology, 162, 123–130.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Courchesne, E., & Pierce, K. (2005). Why the frontal cortex in autism might be talking only to itself: local over-connectivity but long-distance disonnection. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 15, 225–230.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Critchley, H. D., Daly, E. M., Bullmore, E. T., Williams, S. C., Van Amelsvoort, T., Robertson, D. M., Rowe, A., Phillips, M., Mcalonan, G., Howlin, P., & Murphy, D. G. (2000). The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when people with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain, 123, 2203–2212.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dalton, K. M., Nacewicz, B. M., Johnstone, T., Schaefer, H. S., Gernsbacher, M. A., Goldsmith, H. H., Alexander, A. L., & Davidson, R. J. (2005). Gaze fixation and the neural circuitry of face processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 8, 519–526.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davies, S., Bishop, D., Manstead, A. S. R., & Tantam, D. (1994). Face perception in children with autism and Asperger’s syndrome. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 1033–1057.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Webb, S. J., & Mc Partland, J. (2005). Understanding the nature of face processing impairment in autism: insights from behavioral and electrophysiological studies. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 403–424.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • De Gelder, B., Vroomen, J., & Van der Heide, L. (1991). Face recognition and lip-reading in autism. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 3, 69–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deltour, J. J., & Hupkens, D. (1981). Test de Vocabulaire Actif Passif, TVAP. Issy-les-Moulineaux: Éditions EAP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 199–210.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Fagot, J. (2006). Processing of compound visual stimuli by children with autism and Asperger syndrome. International Journal of Psychology, 41, 97–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Doya, K. (2000). Complementary roles of basal ganglia and cerebellum on learning and motor control. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 10, 732–739.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frith, U. (1989). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Basic Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., Mestre, D., Masson, G., & de Schonen, S. (1995). Postural effects of motion vision in young autistic children. NeuroReport, 6, 1211–1214.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., de Gelder, B., & de Schonen, S. (1996). Face processing in autistics: evidence for a generalized deficit? Child Neuropsychology, 2, 123–139.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B. (1997). Face recognition and visual-motion perception in autistic children. Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation, Université Aix-Marseille II.

  • Gepner, B. (2001). «Malvoyance» du mouvement dans l’autisme infantile? Une nouvelle approche neuropsychopathologique développementale. Psychiatrie de l’Enfant, 1, 77–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., Deruelle, C., & Grynfeltt, S. (2001). Motion and emotion: a novel approach to the study of face processing by autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31, 37–45.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., & Massion, J. (directed by) (2002). L’autisme: une pathologie du codage temporel? Travaux Interdisciplinaires du Laboratoire Parole et Langage, 21, 177–218.

  • Gepner, B., & Mestre, D. (2002a). Brief Report: Postural reactivity to fast visual motion differentiates autistic from children with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 32, 231–238.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., & Mestre, D. (2002b). Rapid visual-motion integration deficit in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6, 455.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B. (2004). Autism, movement and facial processing. American Journal of Psychiatry, 161, 1719.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B. (2005). Malvoyance du mouvement dans l’autisme : de la clinique à la recherche et à la rééducation. In A. Berthoz, C. Andres, C. Barthélémy, J. Massion, & B. Rogé (Eds.), Autisme, cerveau et développement: de la recherche à la pratique (pp. 205–226). Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gepner, B., Lainé, F., & Tardif, C. (2005). E-Motion mis-sight and other temporal processing disorders in autism. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 23, 104–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gervais, H., Belin, P., Boddaert, N., Leboyer, M., Coez, A., Sfaello, I., Barthélémy, C., Brunelle, F., Samson, Y., & Zilbovicius, M. (2004). Abnormal cortical voice processing in autism. Nature Neuroscience, 7, 801–802.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Grandin, T. (1995). Thinking in pictures and other reports from my life with autism. New York: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Grézes, J., & de Gelder, B. (2005). Contagion motrice et émotionnelle. In A. Berthoz, C. Andres, C. Barthélémy, J. Massion, & B. Rogé (Eds.), Autisme, cerveau et développement: de la recherche à la pratique (pp. 295–319). Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habib M., Espesser R., Rey V., Giraud K., Bruas P., & Gres C. (1999). Training dyslexics with acoustically modified speech : evidence of improved phonological performance. Brain and Cognition, 40, 143–146.

    Google Scholar 

  • Habib, M. (2006). Troubles associés et comorbidités dans la dyslexie: de l’observation clinique à la compréhension des mécanismes. In Expertise collective sur les Troubles de Développement du Langage oral et écrit, Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale. Editions INSERM: Paris (in press).

  • Hadjikhani, N., Joseph, R. M., Snyder, J., Chabris, C. F., Clark, J., Steele, S., McGrath, L., Vangel, M., Aharon, I., Feczko, E., Harris, G. J., & Tager-Flusberg, H. (2004). Activation of the fusiform gyrus when individuals with autism spectrum disorder view faces. Neuroimage, 22, 1141–1150.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. (1999). Autism: Cognitive deficit or cognitive style? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 216–222.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hertzig, M. E., Snow, M.E, & Sherman, M. (1989). Affect and cognition in autism. Journal of the Amercican Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28, 195–199.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P. (1986a). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 321–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson R. P. (1986b). The autistic child’s appraisal of expressions of emotion: a further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 27, 671–680.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, R. P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988a). What’s in a face? The case of autism. British Journal of Psychology, 79, 441–453.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hobson, P., Ouston, J., & Lee, A. (1988b). Emotion recognition in autism: coordinating faces and voices. Psychological Medicine, 18, 911–923.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, C., Russell, J., & Robbins, T. W. (1994). Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia, 32, 477–492.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, J. R., & Melyn, M. (2005). EEG and seizures in autistic children and adolescents: further findings with therapeutic implications. Clinical EEG and Neuroscience, 36, 15–20.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hutt, C., & Ounsted, C. (1966). The biological significance of gaze avoidance with particular reference to the syndrome of infantile autism. Behavioral Sciences, 11, 346–356.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hutt, C., Hutt, S. J., Lee, D., & Ounsted, C. (1964). Arousal and childhood autism. Nature, 4961, 908–909.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ito M, (1984). The cerebellum and neural control. New York: Raven Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jamain, S., Quach, H., Betancur, C., Rastam, M., Colineaux, C., Gillberg, I. C., Soderstrom, H., Giros, B., Leboyer, M., Gillberg, C., & Bourgeron, T. et al. (2003). Mutations of the X-linked genes encoding neuroligins NLGN3 and NLGN4 are associated with autism. Nature Genetics, 34, 27–29.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson M. T., & Ebner T. J. (2000.) Processing of multiple kinematic signals in the cerebellum and motor cortices. Brain Research Review, 33, 155–168.

    Google Scholar 

  • Just, M. A., Cherkassky, V. L., Keller, T. A., & Minshew, N. J. (2004). Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain, 127, 1811–1821.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kamachi, M., Bruce, V., Mukaida, S., Gyoba, J., Yoshikawa, S., & Akamatsu, S. (2001). Dynamic properties influence the perception of facial expressions. Perception, 30, 875–887.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kanner, L. (1943). Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Nervous Child, 2, 217–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kemper, T. L., & Bauman, M. L. (1993). The contribution of neuropathologic studies to understanding of autism. Neurological Clinics, 11, 175–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klin, A., Sparrow, S. S., de Bildt, A., Cicchetti, D. V., Cohen, D. J., & Volkmar, F. R. (1999). A normed study of face recognition in autism and related disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 29, 497–507.

    Google Scholar 

  • Langdell, T. (1978). Recognition of faces: an approach to the study of auism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 19, 255–268.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, M., & Hubel, D. (1988). Segregation of form, color, movement and depth: anatomy, physiology and perception. Science, 240, 740–749.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Pearson, D. A., Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Chen, R. (1994). Imitation and expression of facial affect in autism. Development and Psychopathology, 6, 433–444.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Tunali-Kotoski, B., Chen, R., Brelsford, K. A., Ortegon, J., & Pearson, D. A. (1995). Intermodal perception of affect in persons with autism or Down syndrome. Development and Psychopathology, 7, 409–418.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martineau, J., Roux, S., Adrien, J. L., Garreau, B., Barthélémy, C., & Lelord, G. (1992). Electrophysiological evidence of different abilities to form cross-modal associations in children with autistic behaviors. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 82, 60–66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. (1996). L’imitation, reflet de l’esprit préverbal. Enfance, 1, 16–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial and manual gestures by human neonates. Science, 198, 75–78.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Meltzoff, A. N., & Moore, M. K. (1993). Why faces are special to infants. On connecting the attraction of faces and infants’ ability for imitation and cross-modal processing. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. Jusczyk, P. MacNeilage et, & J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: speech and face processing in the first year of life (pp. 211–215). The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mestre, D., Rondan, C., Masson, G., Castet, E., Deruelle, C., & Gepner, B. (2002). Evaluation de la vision du mouvement chez des enfants autistes au moyen du nystagmus opto-cinétique. Travaux Interdisciplinaires du Laboratoire Parole et Langage, 21, 192–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Middleton F. A., & Strick P. L. (2000). Basal ganglia and cerebellum loops: motor and cognitive circuits. Brain Research Review, 31, 236–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., & Campbell, R. (2005). Motion perception and autistic spectrum disorder: a review. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition, 23, 3–36.

    Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 255–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Burack, J. A., Iarocci, G., Belleville, S., & Enns, J. T. (2003). Locally oriented perception with intact global processing among adolescents with high-functioning autism: evidence from multiple paradigms. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 904–913.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J. (1989). Communicating by imitation: a developmental and comparative approach to transitory social competence. In B. H. Schneider et al. (Eds.), Social competence in developmental perspective (pp. 131–144). The Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J. (1992). Imitation et Communication. Un abord comparatif de l’enfant prélangagier et de l’enfant autiste. In J. Hochmann, & P. Ferrari (Eds.), Imitation, identification chez l’enfant autiste (pp. 79–104). Paris: Bayard Editions.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nadel, J. (2005). Imitation et autisme. In: A. Berthoz, C. Andres, C. Barthélémy, J. Massion, & B. Rogé, (Eds.), Autisme, cerveau et développement: de la recherche à la pratique (pp. 205–226). Paris: Odile Jacob.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nishitani, N., Avikainen, S., & Hari, R. (2004). Abnormal imitation-related cortical activation sequences in Asperger’s syndrome. Annals of Neurology, 55, 558–562.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V. S., & Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Research Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 190–198.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Maybery, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: a possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43, 1044–1053.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Percha, B., Dzakpasu, R., Zochowski, M., & Parent, J. (2005). Transition from local to global phase synchrony in small world neural network and its possible implications for epilepsy. Physical Review E Statistical Nonlinear Soft Matter Physics, 72, 031909.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, K., Muller, R. A., Ambrose, J., Allen, G., & Courchesne, E. (2001). Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: evidence from functional MRI. Brain, 124, 2059–2073.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pierce, K., Haist, F., Sedaghat, F., & Courchesne, E. (2004). The brain response to personally familiar faces in autism: findings of fusiform activity and beyond. Brain, 127, 2703–2716.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Plaisted, K., Swettenham, J., & Rees, L. (1999). Children with autism show local precedence in a divided attention task and global precedence in a selective attention task. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 40, 733–742.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pollick, F. E., Hill, H., Calder, A., & Paterson, H. (2003). Recognising facial expression from spatially and temporally modified movements. Perception, 32, 813–826.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F., Rosen, S., Dakin, S. C., Day, B. L., Castellote, J. M., White, S., & Frith, U. (2003). Theories of developmental dyslexia: insights from a multiple case study of dyslexic adults. Brain, 126, 841–865.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rapin, I. (2002). The autistic spectrum disorders. The New England Journal of Medicine, 347, 302–303.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. (1981). Progressive matrices. Issy-les-Moulineaux: Editions EAP.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rinehart, N. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Moss, S. A., Brereton, A. V., & Toge, B. J. (2000). Atypical interference of local detail on global processing in high-functioning autism and Asperger’s disorder. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41, 769–778.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rogers, S. J., Hepburn, S. L., Stackhouse, T., & Wehner, E. (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44, 763–781.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sauvage D. (1988). Autisme du nourrisson et du jeune enfant. Paris: Masson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schopler, E., Reichler, R. J, de Vellis, R. F., & Daly, K. (1980). Toward objective classification of childhood autism: Childhood Autism Rating Scale (CARS). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,10, 91–103.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schultz, R. T., Gauthier, I., Klin, A., Fulbright, R., Anderson, A., Volkmar, F. R., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C., Cohen, D. J., & Gore, J. C. (2000). Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 331–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shah, A., & Frith, U. (1993). Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design task? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34, 1351–1364.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: a critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116, 259–273.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, J., O’Brien, J., Riggs, K., Braddick, O., Atkinson, J., & Wattam-Bell, J. (2000). Motion processing in autism: evidence for a dorsal stream deficiency. NeuroReport, 11, 2765–2767.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spencer, K. M., Nestor, P. G., Perlmutter, R., Niznikiewicz, M. A., Klump, M. C., Frumin, M., Shenton, M. E., & McCarley, R. W. (2004). Neural synchrony indexes disordered perception and cognition in schizophrenia. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 101, 17288–17293.

    Google Scholar 

  • Symond, M. P., Harris, A. W., Gordon, E., & Williams, L. M. (2005). “Gamma synchrony” in first-episode schizophrenia: a disorder of temporal connectivity? American Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 459–465.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Takarae, Y., Minshew, N. J., Luna, B., Krisky, C. M., & Sweeney, J. A. (2004). Pursuit eye movement deficits in autism. Brain, 127, 2584–2594.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1976). Rapid auditory processing in normal and disordered language development. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research, 19, 561–594.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1973). Defects of non-verbal auditory perception in children with developmental aphasia. Nature, 241, 468–469.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., & Piercy, M. (1974). Developmental aphasia: rate of auditory processing and selective impairment of consonant perception. Neuropsychologia, 12, 83–93.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P., Miller, S. L., Bedi, G., Byma, G., Wang, X., Nagarajan, S. S., Schreiner, C., Jenkins, W. M., & Merzenich, M. M. (1996). Language comprehension in language-learning impaired children improved with acoustically modified speech. Science, 271, 81–83.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, C., & Gepner, B. (2003). L’Autisme. Paris: Nathan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tardif, C., Thomas, K., Gepner, B., & Rey, V. (2002). Contribution à l’évaluation du système phonologique explicite chez des enfants autistes. Parole, 21, 35–72.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teitelbaum, P., Teitelbaum, O., Nye, J., Fryman, J., & Maurer, R. (1998). Movement analysis in infancy may be useful for early diagnosis of autism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 95, 13982–13987.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teunisse, J. P., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Face processing in adolescents with autistic disorder: the inversion and composite effect. Brain and Cognition, 52, 285–294.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Theoret, H., Halligan, E., Kobayashi, M., Fregni, F., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2005). Impaired motor facilitation during action observation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Current Biology, 15, 84–85.

    Google Scholar 

  • Timme, M., Wolf, F., & Geisel, T. (2004). Topological speed limits to network synchronization. Physical Review Letter, 92, 074101.

    Google Scholar 

  • Varela, F., Lachaux, J. P., Rodriguez, E., & Martinerie, J. (2001). The Brainweb: phase synchronization and large-scale integration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2, 229–239.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Villalobos, M. E., Mizuno, A., Dahl, B. C., Kemmotsu, N., & Muller, R. A. (2005). Reduced functional connectivity between V1 and inferior frontal cortex associated with visuomotor performance in autism. Neuroimage, 15, 916–925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vinter, A. (1986). The role of movement in eliciting early imitations. Child Development, 57, 66–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., Sparrow, S. S., Rende, R. C., & Cohen, D. J. (1989). Facial perception in autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 30, 591–598.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Volkmar, F. R., & Pauls, D. (2003). Autism (Seminar). The Lancet, 362, 1133–1141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Waterhouse, L., Fein, D., & Modahl, C. (1996). Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. Psychological Review, 103, 457–489.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Welsh, J. P., Ahn, E. S., & Placantonakis, D. G. (2005). Is autism due to brain desynchronization ? International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 253–263.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wickelgren, I. (2005). Autistic brains out of synch ? Science, 308, 1856–1858.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wicker, B., Fonlupt, P., Hubert, B., Gepner, B., Tardif, C., & Deruelle, C. (submitted). Abnormal effective connectivity during explicit emotional processing in autism.

  • Williams, D. (1992). Nobody nowhere. London: Doubleday.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H., Massaro, D. W., Peel, N. J., Bosseler, A., & Suddendorf, T. (2004a). Visual–auditory integration during speech imitation in autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 25, 559–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Williams, J. H., Whiten, A., & Singh, T. (2004b). A systematic review of action imitation in autistic spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 285–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1992). The ICD-10 classification of mental and behavioural disorders (ICD-10). Geneva: WHO.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zazzo, R. (1957). Le problème de l’imitation chez le nouveau-né. Enfance, 2, 135–142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zihl, J., Von Cramon, D., & Mai, N. (1983). Selective disturbance of movement vision after bilateral brain damage. Brain, 106, 313–340.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zwaigenbaum, L., Bryson, S., Rogers, T., Roberts, W., Brian, J., & Szatmari, P. (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23, 143–152.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We wish to thank very sincerely all the parents who accepted the participation of their children in this study. We also thank all the children who participated in this study and hope that the results obtained will contribute to their rehabilitation in the future. We are grateful to David Béchu, Damien Penalba and Pierrick Leborgne, from the SCAM (Service Commun Audio-visuel et Multimédia, University of Provence), for their valuable technical assistance in the realization of facial and vocal stimuli.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Bruno Gepner.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Tardif, C., Lainé, F., Rodriguez, M. et al. Slowing Down Presentation of Facial Movements and Vocal Sounds Enhances Facial Expression Recognition and Induces Facial–Vocal Imitation in Children with Autism. J Autism Dev Disord 37, 1469–1484 (2007). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0223-x

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-006-0223-x

Keywords

Navigation