Skip to main content
Log in

Ready, set, point: The effects of alertness on prism adaptation in healthy adults

  • Research Article
  • Published:
Experimental Brain Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Prism adaptation (PA) is a promising rehabilitation technique for visuo-spatial neglect, an attention disorder that is characterized by spatial attention deficits (i.e., deficits in orienting). PA involves visuo-motor adaptation to rightward shifting prism goggles. Following goggle removal, this adaptation results in leftward shifts in visuo-motor aiming and amelioration of spatial neglect. Even though some studies clearly support the beneficial effects of PA for spatial neglect, not all studies find benefits, thus it remains unclear how PA effects could be improved. Taking advantage of the known interactions between orienting and alerting reported in the attention literature (i.e., alerting enhances orienting function; e.g., Ishigami and Klein in J Individ Differ 30:220–237. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.30.4.220, 2009, in J Neurosci Methods 190:117–28. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.019, 2010; Robertson et al. in Nature 395:169–72. doi:10.1038/25993, 1998), we examined the effects of alerting tones on PA with healthy young and older adults. We found that the effects of alerting on PA with young adults were negative, while there was a positive effect with older adults, specifically on a visuo-motor outcome task. Thus, enhancement of PA effects by alerting may be age specific and task specific. Therefore, we can conclude that while the impact of alerting tones is not always positive, further research in patients with neglect may be warranted.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. There were 144 trials for participants we tested earlier (n = 11). Data from the first two blocks were analyzed to be consistent with the participants we tested later.

References

  • Appelros P, Karlsson GM, Seiger A, Nydevik I (2002) Neglect and anosognosia after first-ever stroke: incidence and relationship to disability. J Rehabil Med 34:3215–3220

    Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomeo P, Chokron S (2002) Orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 26:217–234

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bartolomeo P, Thiebaut de Schotten M, Chica AB (2012) Brain networks of visuospatial attention and their disruption in visual neglect. Front Hum Neurosci 6:110. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2012.00110

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berberovic N, Pisella L, Morris AP, Mattingley JB (2004) Prismatic adaptation reduces biased temporal order judgements in spatial neglect. Cogn Neurosci Neuropsychol 15:1199–1204. doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000125780.50515.7a

    Google Scholar 

  • Bultitude JH, Rafal RD, List A (2009) Prism adaptation reverses the local processing bias in patients with right temporo-parietal junction lesions. Brain 132:1669–1677. doi:10.1093/brain/awp096

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Butler BC, Eskes GA, Klein RM (2010) Measuring the components of attention. Can J Ex Psychol 64:323–328

    Google Scholar 

  • Callejas A, Lupiàñez J, Funes MJ, Tudela P (2005) Modulations among the alerting, orienting and executive control networks. Exp Brain Res 167:27–37. doi:10.1007/s00221-005-2365-z

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta M, Shulman GL (2011) Spatial neglect and attention networks. Annu Rev Neurosci 34:569–599. doi:10.1146/annurev-neuro-061010-113731

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Lewis C, Snyder AZ, Sapir A (2005) Neural basis and recovery of spatial attention deficits in spatial neglect. Nat Neurosci 8:1603–1610

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull JT, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM (1996) A fronto-parietal network for rapid visual information processing: a PET study of sustained attention and working memory. Neuropsychologia 34:1085–1095

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Coull JT, Frith CD, Büchel C, Nobre AC (2000) Orienting attention in time: behavioural and neuroanatomical distinction between exogenous and endogenous shifts. Neuropsychologia 38:808–819

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Danckert J, Ferber S (2006) Revisiting unilateral neglect. Neuropsychologia 44:987–1006

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • DeGutis JM, Van Vleet TM (2010) Tonic and phasic alertness training: a novel behavioral therapy to improve spatial and non-spatial attention in patients with hemispatial neglect. Front Hum Neurosci 4:1–17. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2010.00060

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner M (2002) Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci 14:340–347

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fan J, McCandliss BD, Fossella J, Flombaum JI, Posner MI (2005) The activation of attentional networks. Neuroimage 26:471–479. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.02.004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Halligan PW, Marshall JC (1998) Visuospatial neglect: the ultimate deconstruction? Brain Cogn 37:419–438. doi:10.1006/brcg.1998.1006

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Heilman KM (1979) Neglect and related disorders. In: Heilman KM, Valenstein E (eds) Clinical neuropsychology. Oxford University Press, New York, pp 268–307

    Google Scholar 

  • Hjaltason H, Tegnér R, Tham K, Levander M, Ericson K (1996) Sustained attention and awareness of disability in chronic neglect. Neuropsychologia 34:1229–1233

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishigami Y, Klein RM (2009) Are individual differences in absentmindedness correlated with individual differences in attention? J Individ Differ 30:220–237. doi:10.1027/1614-0001.30.4.220

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ishigami Y, Klein RM (2010) Repeated measurement of the components of attention using two versions of the Attention Network Test (ANT): stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. J Neurosci Methods 190:117–128. doi:10.1016/j.jneumeth.2010.04.019

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishigami Y, Klein RM (2011) Repeated measurement of the components of attention of older adults using the two versions of the attention network test: stability, isolability, robustness, and reliability. Front Aging Neurosci 3:17. doi:10.3389/fnagi.2011.00017

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ishigami Y, Lawrence MA, Klein RM, Eskes G (2012) Attention networks and their interactions in healthy aging. Paper accepted for Our Future is Aging: current research on knowledge, practice and policy, Halifax, NS, Canada (November 21–23, 2012)

  • Karnath HO, Fruhmann Berger M, Küker W, Rorden C (2004) The anatomy of spatial neglect based on voxelwise statistical analysis: a study of 140 patients. Cereb Cortex 14:1164–1172. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhh076

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luauté J, Halligan P, Rode G, Jacquin-Courtois S, Boisson D (2006a) Prism adaptation first among equals in alleviating left neglect: a review. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24:409–418

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Luauté J, Michel C, Rode G, Pisella L, Jacquin-Courtois S, Costes N, Cotton F, le Bars D, Boisson D, Halligan P, Rossetti Y (2006b) Functional anatomy of the therapeutic effects of prism adaptation on left neglect. Neurology 66:1859–1867. doi:10.1212/01.wnl.0000219614.33171.01

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mattingley JB, Husain M, Rorden C, Kennard C, Driver J (1998) Motor role of human inferior parietal lobe revealed in unilateral neglect patients. Nature 392:179–182

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris AP, Kritikos A, Berberovic N, Pisella L, Chambers CD, Mattingley JB (2004) Special section prism adaptation and spatial attention: a study of visual search in normal’s and patients with unilateral neglect. Cortex 40:703–721

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morton SM, Bastian AJ (2004) Prism adaptation during walking generalizes to reaching and requires the cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 92:2497–2509. doi:10.1152/jn.00129.2004

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paolucci S, Antonucci G, Grasso MG, Pizzamiglio L (2001) The role of unilateral spatial neglect in rehabilitation of right brain–damaged ischemic stroke patients: a matched comparison. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 82:743–749. doi:10.1053/apmr.2001.23191

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pisella L, Rode G, Farne A, Tilikete C, Rossetti Y (2006) Prism adaptation in the rehabilitation of patients with visuo-spatial cognitive disorders. Curr Opin Neurol 19:1–10

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI (2008) Measuring alertness. Ann N Y Acad Sci 1129:193–199. doi:10.1196/annals.1417.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Peterson SE (1990) The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci 13:25–42. doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner M, Walker A, Friedrich J, Rafal R (1984) Effects of parietal injury on covert orienting of attention. J Neurosci 4:1863–1874

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner MI, Inhoff AW, Friedrich FJ, Cohen A (1987) Isolating attentional systems: a cognitive-anatomical analysis. Psychobiol 15:107–121

    Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Wallace B (1988) Components of prism adaptation in terminal and concurrent exposure: organization of the eye-hand coordination loop. Percept Psychophys 44:59–68

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Wallace B (2006) Prism adaptation and unilateral neglect: review and analysis. Neuropsychologia 44:1–20

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Redding GM, Rossetti Y, Wallace B (2005) Applications of prism adaptation: a tutorial in theory and method. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 29:431–444

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, Tegnér R, Tham K, Lo A, Nimmo-Smith I (1995) Sustained attention training for unilateral neglect: theoretical and rehabilitation implications. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 17:416–430. doi:10.1080/01688639508405133

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, Manly T, Beschin N, Daini R, Haeske-Dewick H, Hömberg V, Jehkonen M, Pizzamiglio G, Shiel A, Weber E (1997) Auditory sustained attention is a marker of unilateral spatial neglect. Neuropsychologia 35:1527–1532

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Robertson IH, Mattingley JB, Rorden C, Driver J (1998) Phasic alerting of neglect patients overcomes their spatial deficit in visual awareness. Nature 395:169–172. doi:10.1038/25993

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rode G, Klos T, Courtois-Jacquin S, Rossetti Y, Pisella L (2006) Neglect and prism adaptation: a new therapeutic tool for spatial cognition disorders. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24:347–356

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rossetti Y, Rode G, Pisella L, Farné A, Li L, Boisson D, Perenin MT (1998) Prism adaptation to a rightward optical deviation rehabilitates left hemispatial neglect. Nature 395:166–169. doi:10.1038/25988

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rousseaux M, Bernati T, Saj A, Kozlowski O (2006) Ineffectiveness of prism adaptation on spatial neglect signs. Stroke 37:542–543. doi:10.1161/01.STR.0000198877.09270.e8

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Siéroff E, Decaix C, Chokron S, Bartolomeo P (2007) Impaired orienting of attention in left unilateral neglect: a componential analysis. Neuropsychology 21:94–113. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.21.1.94

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Striemer CL, Danckert JA (2010) Through a prism darkly: re-evaluating prisms and neglect. Trends Cogn Sci 14:308–316. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sturm W, Willmes K (2001) On the functional neuroanatomy of intrinsic and phasic alertness. Neuroimage 14:S76–S84. doi:10.1006/nimg.2001.0839

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sturm W, Longoni F, Weis S, Specht K, Herzog H, Vohn R, Thimm M, Willmes K (2004) Functional reorganisation in patients with right hemisphere stroke after training of alertness: a longitudinal PET and fMRI study in eight cases. Neuropsychologia 42:434–450. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.09.001

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sturm W, Thimm M, Küst J, Karbe H, Fink GR (2006) Alertness-training in neglect: behavioral and imaging results. Restor Neurol Neurosci 24:371–384

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thimm M, Fink GR, Küst J, Karbe H, Sturm W (2006) Impact of alertness training on spatial neglect: a behavioural and fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 44:1230–1246. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.008

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vallar G, Perani D (1986) The anatomy of unilateral neglect after right-hemisphere stroke lesions: a clinical/CT-scan correlation study in man. Neuropsychologia 24:609–622. doi:10.1016/0028-3932(86)90001-1

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Van Rossum G, Drake FL (2001) Python Reference Manual. PythonLabs, Virginia, USA. http://www.python.org

  • Van Vleet TM, Robertson LC (2006) Cross-modal interactions in time and space: auditory influence on visual attention in hemispatial neglect. J Cogn Neurosci 18:1368–1379. doi:10.1162/jocn.2006.18.8.1368

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vossel S, Weiss PH, Eschenbeck P, Fink GR (2012) Anosognosia, neglect, extinction and lesion site predict impairment of daily living after right-hemispheric stroke. Cortex 49:1782–1789. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2012.12.011

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wilson FC, Manly T (2003) Sustained attention training and errorless learning facilitates self-care functioning in chronic ipsilesional neglect following severe traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychol Rehabil 13:537–548. doi:10.1080/09602010343000200

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by a Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) operating grant (to Dr. Gail Eskes) and by the Atlantic Canada Opportunities Agency (ACOA, through the Atlantic Innovation Fund program to Dr. Gail Eskes). Franziska Kintzel was supported by the Trans-Atlantic Neuroscience Teaching Network (TANTEN), in cooperation with Maastricht University (The Netherlands) and Dalhousie University. Franziska Kintzel thanks Dr. Eric Vuurman (Maastricht University) for the support and supervision during the conduct of the project. Dr. Yoko Ishigami was supported by a postdoctoral funding from ACOA. We thank Joshua Salmon for his valuable assistance in programming of the tasks.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Franziska Kintzel.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kintzel, F., Ishigami, Y. & Eskes, G.A. Ready, set, point: The effects of alertness on prism adaptation in healthy adults. Exp Brain Res 233, 1441–1454 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4218-8

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-015-4218-8

Keywords

Navigation