Skip to main content
Log in

Altered Gesture and Speech Production in ASD Detract from In-Person Communicative Quality

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study disentangled the influences of language and social processing on communication in autism spectrum disorder (ASD) by examining whether gesture and speech production differs as a function of social context. The results indicate that, unlike other adolescents, adolescents with ASD did not increase their coherency and engagement in the presence of a visible listener, and that greater coherency and engagement were related to lesser social and communicative impairments. Additionally, the results indicated that adolescents with ASD produced sparser speech and fewer gestures conveying supplementary information, and that both of these effects increased in the presence of a visible listener. Together, these findings suggest that interpersonal communication deficits in ASD are driven more strongly by social processing than language processing.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Lexical affiliates were identified for all gesture types (iconic, metaphorical, beat, deictic), which reduced interrater agreement compared to other similar studies (e.g., de Marchena and Eigsti 2010) where not all gesture types were scored.

  2. This value was lower than others because there were unlimited possibilities, rather than a fixed scale or alternatives to choose between. In order to be scored as in agreement, both coders’ response were required to be identical.

  3. Text analysis measures were used as a metric of speech complexity because there are no comparable automated measures specifically designed for speech analysis.

References

  • Alibali, M. W., & Don, L. S. (2001). Children’s gestures are meant to be seen. Gesture, 1, 113–127. doi:10.1075/gest.1.2.02ali.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169–188. doi:10.1006/jmla.2000.2752.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (5th ed.). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Astington, J. W., & Baird, J. A. (2005). Why language matters for theory of mind (Vol. xii). New York: Oxford University Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Attwood, A., Frith, U., & Hermelin, B. (1988). The understanding and use of interpersonal gestures by autistic and Down’s syndrome children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 18, 241–257. doi:10.1007/BF02211950.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baltaxe, C. A. (1977). Pragmatic deficits in the language of autistic adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 2, 176–180. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/2.4.176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baron-Cohen, S. (1997). Mindblindness: An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bavelas, J. B., Gerwing, J., Sutton, C., & Prevost, D. (2008). Gesturing on the telephone: Independent effects of dialogue and visibility. Journal of Memory and Language, 58, 495–520. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.02.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Camaioni, L., Perucchini, P., Muratori, F., & Milone, A. (1997). Brief report: A longitudinal examination of the communicative gestures deficit in young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 715–725. doi:10.1023/A:1025858917000.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Charman, T., Drew, A., Baird, C., & Baird, G. (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30, 213–236. doi:10.1017/S0305000902005482.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Colgan, S. E., Lanter, E., McComish, C., Watson, L. R., Crais, E. R., & Baranek, G. T. (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 307–319. doi:10.1080/09297040600701360.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cook, S. W., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113, 98–104. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.006.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dawson, G., Toth, K., Abbott, R., Osterling, J., Munson, J., Estes, A., & Liaw, J. (2004). Early social attention impairments in autism: Social orienting, joint attention, and attention to distress. Developmental Psychology, 40, 271.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I. M. (2010). Conversational gestures in autism spectrum disorders: Asynchrony but not decreased frequency. Autism Research, 3, 311–322. doi:10.1002/aur.159.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Marchena, A., & Eigsti, I. M. (2015). The art of common ground: Emergence of a complex pragmatic language skill in adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Child Language. doi:10.1017/S0305000915000070.

  • Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (1998). The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. The American Journal of Psychology, 111, 43–62. doi:10.2307/1423536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12, 516–522. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00395.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F. G. E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101–119. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(93)90026-R.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Huerta, M., Bishop, S. L., Duncan, A., Hus, V., & Lord, C. (2014). Application of DSM-5 criteria for autism spectrum disorder to three samples of children with DSM-IV diagnoses of pervasive developmental disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry,. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.12020276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kincaid, J. P., Fishburne, R. P, Jr, Rogers, R. L., & Chissom, B. S. (1975). Derivation of new readability formulas for navy enlisted personnel (Research Branch Report 8-75). TN: Millington.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal?: Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 16–32. doi:10.1016/S0749-596X(02)00505-3.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lake, J. K., Humphreys, K. R., & Cardy, S. (2011). Listener vs. speaker-oriented aspects of speech: Studying the disfluencies of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18, 135–140. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0037-x.

  • Landa, R. (2000). Social language use in Asperger syndrome. In A. Klin, F. Volkmar, & S. Sparrow (Eds.), Asperger syndrome (pp. 125–155). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landis, J. R., & Koch, G. G. (1977). The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landry, S. H., & Loveland, K. A. (1989). The effect of social context on the functional communication skills of autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 19, 283–299. doi:10.1007/BF02211847.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H, Jr, Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30, 205–223. doi:10.1023/A:1005592401947.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Rutter, M., & Couteur, A. (1994). Autism diagnostic interview-revised: A revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 659–685. doi:10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., & Landry, S. H. (1986). Joint attention and language in autism and developmental language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 16, 335–349. doi:10.1007/BF01531663.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Loveland, K. A., Landry, S. H., Hughes, S. O., Hall, S. K., & McEvoy, R. E. (1988). Speech acts and the pragmatic deficits of autism. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 31, 593–604. doi:10.1044/jshr.3104.593.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Martin, I., & McDonald, S. (2004). An exploration of causes of non-literal language problems in individuals with Asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34, 311–328. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000029553.52889.15.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., & Peppe, S. (2003). Prosody in autism spectrum disorders: A critical review. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 38, 325–350. doi:10.1080/1368282031000154204.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCann, J., Peppé, S., Gibbon, F. E., O’Hare, A., & Rutherford, M. (2007). Prosody and its relationship to language in school-aged children with high-functioning autism. International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 42, 682–702. doi:10.1080/13682820601170102.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Mol, L., Krahmer, E., Maes, A., & Swerts, M. (2012). Adaptation in gesture: Converging hands or converging minds? Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 249–264. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2011.07.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morrel-Samuels, P., & Krauss, R. M. (1992). Word familiarity predicts temporal asynchrony of hand gestures and speech. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 615–622. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.18.3.615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mundy, P., Sigman, M., & Kasari, C. (1990). A longitudinal study of joint attention and language development in autistic children. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 20, 115–128. doi:10.1007/BF02206861.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K., Lakusta, L., Schroer, E., Minshew, N., & Luna, B. (2011). Deficits in adults with autism spectrum disorders when processing multiple objects in dynamic scenes. Autism Research, 4, 132–142. doi:10.1002/aur.179.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hearn, K., Schroer, E., Minshew, N., & Luna, B. (2010). Lack of developmental improvement on a face memory task during adolescence in autism. Neuropsychologia, 48, 3955–3960. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.024.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Osterling, J., & Dawson, G. (1994). Early recognition of children with autism: A study of first birthday home videotapes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 24, 247–257. doi:10.1007/BF02172225.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Shriberg, L. D., McSweeny, J., Cicchetti, D., Klin, A., & Volkmar, F. (2005). Brief report: Relations between prosodic performance and communication and socialization ratings in high functioning speakers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 35, 861–869. doi:10.1007/s10803-005-0031-8.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Shriberg, L. D. (2001). Speech and prosody characteristics of adolescents and adults with high-functioning autism and Asperger syndrome. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 1097–1115. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/087).

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Silverman, L. B., Bennetto, L., Campana, E., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2010). Speech-and-gesture integration in high functioning autism. Cognition, 115, 380–393. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2010.01.002.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Stone, W. L., Ousley, O. Y., Yoder, P. J., Hogan, K. L., & Hepburn, S. L. (1997). Nonverbal communication in two-and three-year-old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27, 677–696.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Swerts, M., Wichmann, A., & Beun, R.-J. (1996). Filled pauses as markers of discourse structure. In Proceedings of the fourth international conference on spoken language, 1996. ICSLP 96. (Vol. 2, pp. 1033–1036). IEEE.

  • Tartaro, A., & Cassell, J. (2008). Playing with virtual peers: Bootstrapping contingent discourse in children with autism. In Proceedings of the 8th international conference on international conference for the learning sciences (Vol. 2, pp. 382–389). Retrieved from http://dl.acm.org/citation.cfm?id=1599919.

  • Tomasello, M. (2009). Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Harvard: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ure, J. (1971). Lexical density and register differentiation. In G. E. Perren & J. L. M. Trim (Eds.), Applications of linguistics (pp. 443–452). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler abbreviated scale of intelligence. Psychological Corporation.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by an institutional training fellowship “Training in Transformative Discovery in Psychiatry” (MH016804-31; Robert A. Sweet, PI) to L.M.M. and a NARSAD Young Investigator Award from the Brain and Behavior Research Foundation to A.S.G. It was done at the University of Pittsburgh, supported by NIMH K01MH081191 (PI: O’Hearn), and NIH HD055748 (PI: Minshew) from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. The authors thank Andrew S. Lynn for assistance with data collection and Ashlie Caputo and Marco Pilotta for assistance with data coding, as well as the participants and their families.

Author Contributions

L.M.M., K.O'H., B.L., & A.S.G. designed research; L.M.M. and K. O'H. performed research; L.M.M. and A.S.G. analyzed data; L.M.M. & A.S.G. wrote the paper.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Laura M. Morett.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Morett, L.M., O’Hearn, K., Luna, B. et al. Altered Gesture and Speech Production in ASD Detract from In-Person Communicative Quality. J Autism Dev Disord 46, 998–1012 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2645-9

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-015-2645-9

Keywords

Navigation