Skip to main content
Log in

Toward an Analysis of Emotions as Products of Motivating Operations

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The study of motivation within behavior analysis has historically pertained to the investigation of organism-environment interactions that alter the efficacy of events as reinforcers or aversive stimuli. A number of authors have noted similarities between the motivational properties of events that are classified as motivating operations (MOs) and the sorts of events typically held to occasion emotions. We will propose that emotions may be most usefully conceptualized within the framework of the MO concept, and that conceptualizing emotions in this way may serve to stimulate research in this important area. In so doing, we will provide a history of the behavior analytic treatment of emotions and offer an account of emotions as products of MOs using several examples from the literature. We will conclude by suggesting several areas in which the present account may generate research.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Michael (1993, p. 197) adopted Skinner’s analysis of emotions described here and noted that although Skinner did not specifically identify the reinforcer-establishing properties of environmental events associated with emotions, the implication was clear.

References

  • Anderson, C. M., Hawkins, R. P., Freeman, K. A., & Scotti, J. R. (2000). Private events: Do they belong in a science of human behavior? The Behavior Analyst, 23, 1–10.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Augustson, E. M., & Dougher, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 181–191.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., & Hake, D. F. (1969). Positive conditioned suppression: Conditioned suppression using positive reinforcers as the unconditioned stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 167–173.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 191–204.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & McLaughlin, R. (1965). The opportunity for aggression as an operant reinforcer during aversive stimulation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 171–180.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Baum, W. M. (2011). Behaviorism, private events, and the molar view of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 185–200.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blackman, D. (1977). Conditioned suppression and the effects of classical conditioning on operant behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 340–363). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Carr, E. G., McLaughlin, D. M., Giacobbe-Grieco, T., & Smith, C. E. (2003). Using mood ratings and mood induction in assessment and intervention for severe problem behavior. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 32–55.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Catania, A. C. (2007). Learning (4th ed.). Cornwall-on-Hudson: Sloan Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cherek, D. R., Thompson, T., & Heistad, G. T. (1973). Responding maintained by the opportunity to attack during an interval food reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 113–123.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cole, J. M., & Parker, B. K. (1971). Schedule-induced aggression: Access to an attackable target bird as a positive reinforcer. Psychonomic Science, 12, 33–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Critchfield, T. S. (2011). Translational contributions of the experimental analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 3–17.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dove, L. D. (1971). Reinforcing properties of situations that permit schedule-induced attack. (Doctoral dissertation). Florida State University. Ann Arbor, Michigan: University Microfilms, No. 72-9196.

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (1954). Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review, 61, 34–46.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 311–333.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (1994). A behavior-analytic account of depression and a case report using acceptance-based procedures. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 321–334.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (2000). Establishing operations, cognition, and emotion. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 11–24.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dymond, S., Schlund, M. W., Roche, B., Whelan, R., Richards, J., & Davies, C. (2011). Inferred threat and safety: Symbolic generalization of human avoidance learning. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 49, 614–621.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. (1941). Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 390–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Falk, J. L. (1971). The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 6, 577–588.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fantino, E. (1973). Emotion. In J. A. Nevin & G. S. Reynolds (Eds.), The study of behavior: Learning, motivation, emotion, and instinct (pp. 281–322). Glenview: Scott, Foresman and Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, L. W., & Peterson, G. L. (1977). Schedule-induced aggression in humans and animals: A comparative parametric review. Aggressive Behavior, 3, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Friman, P. C., Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1998a). Why behavior analysts should study emotion: The example of anxiety. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 151–176.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friman, P. C., Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1998b). Behavior analysis of private events is possible, progressive, and nondualistic: A response to Lamal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 707–708.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gentry, W. D. (1968). Fixed-ratio schedule-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 813–817.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1974). Toward a constructional approach to social problems: Ethical and constitutional issues raised by applied behavior analysis. Behaviorism, 2, 1–84.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Goldiamond, I. (1979). Emotions and emotional behavior: A consequential analysis and treatment. Audiotape, Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy. New York: BMA Audio Cassettes Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). Noncontingent presentation of attention and alternative stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 229–237.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2009). Overcoming the pseudo-problem of private events in the analysis of behavior. Behavior and Philosophy, 37, 39–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review, 76, 46–69.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421–430.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., & Michael, J. (2000). Current research on the influence of establishing operations on behavior in applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 411–418.

  • Kantor, J. R. (1953). The logic of modern science. Indianapolis: IN: Principia Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, J. F., & Hake, D. F. (1970). An extinction-induced increase in aggressive response with humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 154–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klatt, K. P., & Morris, E. K. (2001). The Premack principle, response deprivation, and establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 173–180.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lamal, P. A. (1998). Advancing backwards. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 705–706.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Layng, T. V. J. (2006). Emotions and emotional behavior: A constructional approach to understanding some social benefits of aggression. Brazilian Journal of Behavior Analysis, 2, 155–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Layng, T. V. J. (2009). The search for an effective clinical behavior analysis: The nonlinear thinking of Israel Goldiamond. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 163–184.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lyon, D. O. (1968). Conditioned suppression: Operant variables and aversive control. Psychological Record, 18, 317–338.

    Google Scholar 

  • Malkesman, O., Scattoni, M. L., Paredes, D., Tragon, T., Pearson, B., Shaltiel, G., Chen, G., Crawley, J. N., & Manji, H. K. (2010). The female urine sniffing test: A novel approach for assessing reward-seeking behavior in rodents. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 864–871.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Malott, R. W. (2008). Principles of behavior (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Martin, G., & Pear, J. (1999). Behavior modification: What it is and how to do it. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGill, P. (1999). Establishing operations: Implications for the assessment, treatment, and prevention of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 393–418.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative stimuli and motivative functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, N. E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 89–101.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Moore, J. (2011). A review of Baum’s review of Conceptual Foundations of Radical Behaviorism. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 127–140.

    Article  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mowrer, O. H., & Lamoreaux, R. R. (1942). Avoidance conditioning and signal duration—a study of secondary motivation and reward. Psychological Monographs, 54(5) (Whole No. 247).

  • Poling, A. (2010). Looking to the future: Will behavior analysis survive and prosper? The Behavior Analyst, 33, 6–17.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1965). Inhibition of avoidance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59, 406–412.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, R. A., & Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74, 151–182.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Richards, R. W., & Rilling, M. (1972). Aversive aspects of a fixed-interval schedule of food reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 405–411.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rygula, R., Abumaria, N., Flugge, G., Fuchs, E., Ruther, E., & Havemann-Reinecke, U. (2005). Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: Impact of chronic social stress. Behavioural Brain Research, 162, 127–134.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance behavior. In P. H. Hoch & J. Zubin (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 70–99). New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1953). Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive warning signal. Science, 118, 157–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? The Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.

    Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton: Copley Publishing Group.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.

    Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A. W. (1988). Skinner’s theory and the emotion-behavior relationship: Incipient change with major implications. American Psychologist, 43, 747–748.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Staats, A. W., & Eifert, G. H. (1990). The paradigmatic behaviorism theory of emotions: Basis for unification. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 539–566.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sundberg, M. L. (2004). A behavioral analysis of motivation and its relation to mand training. In L. W. Williams (Ed.), Developmental disabilities: Etiology, assessment, intervention, and integration (pp. 199–220). Reno: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ulrich, R. E., Hutchinson, R. R., & Azrin, N. H. (1965). Pain-elicited aggression. The Psychological Record, 15, 111–126.

    Google Scholar 

  • Valdovinos, M. G., & Kennedy, C. H. (2004). A behavior-analytic conceptualization of the side effects of psychotropic medication. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 231–238.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vyse, S. (2013). Changing course. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 123–135.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wray, A. M., Freund, R. A., & Dougher, M. J. (2009). A behavior-analytic account of cognitive bias in clinical populations. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 29–49.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Zacharko, R. M., Bowers, W. J., & Anisman, H. (1984). Responding for brain stimulation: Stress and desmethylimipramine. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 8, 601–606.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Matthew Lewon.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Lewon, M., Hayes, L.J. Toward an Analysis of Emotions as Products of Motivating Operations. Psychol Rec 64, 813–825 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0046-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-014-0046-7

Keywords

Navigation