Advertisement

The Psychological Record

, Volume 64, Issue 4, pp 813–825 | Cite as

Toward an Analysis of Emotions as Products of Motivating Operations

  • Matthew Lewon
  • Linda J. Hayes
Original Article

Abstract

The study of motivation within behavior analysis has historically pertained to the investigation of organism-environment interactions that alter the efficacy of events as reinforcers or aversive stimuli. A number of authors have noted similarities between the motivational properties of events that are classified as motivating operations (MOs) and the sorts of events typically held to occasion emotions. We will propose that emotions may be most usefully conceptualized within the framework of the MO concept, and that conceptualizing emotions in this way may serve to stimulate research in this important area. In so doing, we will provide a history of the behavior analytic treatment of emotions and offer an account of emotions as products of MOs using several examples from the literature. We will conclude by suggesting several areas in which the present account may generate research.

Keywords

Emotion Motivation Motivating operations Establishing operations 

References

  1. Anderson, C. M., Hawkins, R. P., Freeman, K. A., & Scotti, J. R. (2000). Private events: Do they belong in a science of human behavior? The Behavior Analyst, 23, 1–10.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  2. Augustson, E. M., & Dougher, M. J. (1997). The transfer of avoidance evoking functions through stimulus equivalence classes. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 28, 181–191.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  3. Azrin, N. H., & Hake, D. F. (1969). Positive conditioned suppression: Conditioned suppression using positive reinforcers as the unconditioned stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12, 167–173.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  4. Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & Hake, D. F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 191–204.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  5. Azrin, N. H., Hutchinson, R. R., & McLaughlin, R. (1965). The opportunity for aggression as an operant reinforcer during aversive stimulation. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 8, 171–180.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  6. Baum, W. M. (2011). Behaviorism, private events, and the molar view of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 185–200.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  7. Blackman, D. (1977). Conditioned suppression and the effects of classical conditioning on operant behavior. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 340–363). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
  8. Carr, E. G., McLaughlin, D. M., Giacobbe-Grieco, T., & Smith, C. E. (2003). Using mood ratings and mood induction in assessment and intervention for severe problem behavior. American Journal on Mental Retardation, 108, 32–55.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  9. Catania, A. C. (2007). Learning (4th ed.). Cornwall-on-Hudson: Sloan Publishing.Google Scholar
  10. Cherek, D. R., Thompson, T., & Heistad, G. T. (1973). Responding maintained by the opportunity to attack during an interval food reinforcement schedule. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 19, 113–123.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  11. Cole, J. M., & Parker, B. K. (1971). Schedule-induced aggression: Access to an attackable target bird as a positive reinforcer. Psychonomic Science, 12, 33–35.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  12. Cooper, J. O., Heron, T. E., & Heward, W. L. (2007). Applied behavior analysis (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Merrill Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  13. Critchfield, T. S. (2011). Translational contributions of the experimental analysis of behavior. The Behavior Analyst, 34, 3–17.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  14. Dove, L. D. (1971). Reinforcing properties of situations that permit schedule-induced attack. (Doctoral dissertation). Florida State University. Ann Arbor, Michigan: University Microfilms, No. 72-9196.Google Scholar
  15. Dinsmoor, J. A. (1954). Punishment: I. The avoidance hypothesis. Psychological Review, 61, 34–46.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  16. Dinsmoor, J. A. (2001). Stimuli inevitably generated by behavior that avoids electric shock are inherently reinforcing. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 75, 311–333.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  17. Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (1994). A behavior-analytic account of depression and a case report using acceptance-based procedures. The Behavior Analyst, 17, 321–334.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  18. Dougher, M. J., & Hackbert, L. (2000). Establishing operations, cognition, and emotion. The Behavior Analyst, 23, 11–24.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  19. Dymond, S., Schlund, M. W., Roche, B., Whelan, R., Richards, J., & Davies, C. (2011). Inferred threat and safety: Symbolic generalization of human avoidance learning. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 49, 614–621.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  20. Estes, W. K., & Skinner, B. F. (1941). Some quantitative properties of anxiety. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 29, 390–400.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  21. Falk, J. L. (1971). The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiology & Behavior, 6, 577–588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  22. Fantino, E. (1973). Emotion. In J. A. Nevin & G. S. Reynolds (Eds.), The study of behavior: Learning, motivation, emotion, and instinct (pp. 281–322). Glenview: Scott, Foresman and Company.Google Scholar
  23. Fischer, S. M., Iwata, B. A., & Mazaleski, J. L. (1997). Noncontingent delivery of arbitrary reinforcers as treatment for self-injurious behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 239–249.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  24. Frederiksen, L. W., & Peterson, G. L. (1977). Schedule-induced aggression in humans and animals: A comparative parametric review. Aggressive Behavior, 3, 57–75.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  25. Friman, P. C., Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1998a). Why behavior analysts should study emotion: The example of anxiety. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 151–176.Google Scholar
  26. Friman, P. C., Hayes, S. C., & Wilson, K. G. (1998b). Behavior analysis of private events is possible, progressive, and nondualistic: A response to Lamal. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 707–708.PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  27. Gentry, W. D. (1968). Fixed-ratio schedule-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 11, 813–817.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  28. Goldiamond, I. (1974). Toward a constructional approach to social problems: Ethical and constitutional issues raised by applied behavior analysis. Behaviorism, 2, 1–84.PubMedGoogle Scholar
  29. Goldiamond, I. (1979). Emotions and emotional behavior: A consequential analysis and treatment. Audiotape, Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy. New York: BMA Audio Cassettes Publisher.Google Scholar
  30. Hanley, G. P., Piazza, C. C., & Fisher, W. W. (1997). Noncontingent presentation of attention and alternative stimuli in the treatment of attention-maintained destructive behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 30, 229–237.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  31. Hayes, L. J., & Fryling, M. J. (2009). Overcoming the pseudo-problem of private events in the analysis of behavior. Behavior and Philosophy, 37, 39–57.Google Scholar
  32. Herrnstein, R. J. (1969). Method and theory in the study of avoidance. Psychological Review, 76, 46–69.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  33. Herrnstein, R. J., & Hineline, P. N. (1966). Negative reinforcement as shock-frequency reduction. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 421–430.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  34. Iwata, B. A., Smith, R. G., & Michael, J. (2000). Current research on the influence of establishing operations on behavior in applied settings. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 33, 411–418.Google Scholar
  35. Kantor, J. R. (1953). The logic of modern science. Indianapolis: IN: Principia Press.Google Scholar
  36. Keller, F. S., & Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). Principles of psychology. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  37. Kelly, J. F., & Hake, D. F. (1970). An extinction-induced increase in aggressive response with humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 154–164.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  38. Klatt, K. P., & Morris, E. K. (2001). The Premack principle, response deprivation, and establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 24, 173–180.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  39. Lamal, P. A. (1998). Advancing backwards. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 31, 705–706.PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  40. Laraway, S., Snycerski, S., Michael, J., & Poling, A. (2003). Motivating operations and terms to describe them: Some further refinements. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 36, 407–414.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  41. Layng, T. V. J. (2006). Emotions and emotional behavior: A constructional approach to understanding some social benefits of aggression. Brazilian Journal of Behavior Analysis, 2, 155–170.Google Scholar
  42. Layng, T. V. J. (2009). The search for an effective clinical behavior analysis: The nonlinear thinking of Israel Goldiamond. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 163–184.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  43. Lyon, D. O. (1968). Conditioned suppression: Operant variables and aversive control. Psychological Record, 18, 317–338.Google Scholar
  44. Malkesman, O., Scattoni, M. L., Paredes, D., Tragon, T., Pearson, B., Shaltiel, G., Chen, G., Crawley, J. N., & Manji, H. K. (2010). The female urine sniffing test: A novel approach for assessing reward-seeking behavior in rodents. Biological Psychiatry, 67, 864–871.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  45. Malott, R. W. (2008). Principles of behavior (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River: Pearson Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  46. Martin, G., & Pear, J. (1999). Behavior modification: What it is and how to do it. Upper Saddle River: Prentice Hall.Google Scholar
  47. McGill, P. (1999). Establishing operations: Implications for the assessment, treatment, and prevention of problem behavior. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 32, 393–418.PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  48. Michael, J. (1982). Distinguishing between discriminative stimuli and motivative functions of stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 37, 149–155.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  49. Michael, J. (1993). Establishing operations. The Behavior Analyst, 16, 191–206.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  50. Miller, N. E. (1948). Studies of fear as an acquirable drive: I. Fear as motivation and fear-reduction as reinforcement in the learning of new responses. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 38, 89–101.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  51. Moore, J. (2011). A review of Baum’s review of Conceptual Foundations of Radical Behaviorism. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 95, 127–140.PubMedCentralCrossRefGoogle Scholar
  52. Mowrer, O. H. (1939). A stimulus-response analysis of anxiety and its role as a reinforcing agent. Psychological Review, 46, 553–565.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  53. Mowrer, O. H., & Lamoreaux, R. R. (1942). Avoidance conditioning and signal duration—a study of secondary motivation and reward. Psychological Monographs, 54(5) (Whole No. 247).Google Scholar
  54. Poling, A. (2010). Looking to the future: Will behavior analysis survive and prosper? The Behavior Analyst, 33, 6–17.Google Scholar
  55. Rescorla, R. A., & LoLordo, V. M. (1965). Inhibition of avoidance behavior. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 59, 406–412.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  56. Rescorla, R. A., & Solomon, R. L. (1967). Two-process learning theory: Relationships between Pavlovian and instrumental learning. Psychological Review, 74, 151–182.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  57. Richards, R. W., & Rilling, M. (1972). Aversive aspects of a fixed-interval schedule of food reinforcement. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 17, 405–411.PubMedCentralCrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  58. Rygula, R., Abumaria, N., Flugge, G., Fuchs, E., Ruther, E., & Havemann-Reinecke, U. (2005). Anhedonia and motivational deficits in rats: Impact of chronic social stress. Behavioural Brain Research, 162, 127–134.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  59. Schoenfeld, W. N. (1950). An experimental approach to anxiety, escape, and avoidance behavior. In P. H. Hoch & J. Zubin (Eds.), Anxiety (pp. 70–99). New York: Grune & Stratton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  60. Sidman, M. (1953). Avoidance conditioning with brief shock and no exteroceptive warning signal. Science, 118, 157–158.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  61. Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Boston: Authors Cooperative.Google Scholar
  62. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The behavior of organisms: An experimental analysis. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
  63. Skinner, B. F. (1950). Are theories of learning necessary? The Psychological Review, 57, 193–216.CrossRefPubMedGoogle Scholar
  64. Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
  65. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. Acton: Copley Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  66. Skinner, B. F. (1974). About behaviorism. New York: Alfred A. Knopf.Google Scholar
  67. Staats, A. W. (1988). Skinner’s theory and the emotion-behavior relationship: Incipient change with major implications. American Psychologist, 43, 747–748.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  68. Staats, A. W., & Eifert, G. H. (1990). The paradigmatic behaviorism theory of emotions: Basis for unification. Clinical Psychology Review, 10, 539–566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
  69. Sundberg, M. L. (2004). A behavioral analysis of motivation and its relation to mand training. In L. W. Williams (Ed.), Developmental disabilities: Etiology, assessment, intervention, and integration (pp. 199–220). Reno: Context Press.Google Scholar
  70. Ulrich, R. E., Hutchinson, R. R., & Azrin, N. H. (1965). Pain-elicited aggression. The Psychological Record, 15, 111–126.Google Scholar
  71. Valdovinos, M. G., & Kennedy, C. H. (2004). A behavior-analytic conceptualization of the side effects of psychotropic medication. The Behavior Analyst, 27, 231–238.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  72. Vyse, S. (2013). Changing course. The Behavior Analyst, 36, 123–135.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  73. Wray, A. M., Freund, R. A., & Dougher, M. J. (2009). A behavior-analytic account of cognitive bias in clinical populations. The Behavior Analyst, 32, 29–49.PubMedCentralPubMedGoogle Scholar
  74. Zacharko, R. M., Bowers, W. J., & Anisman, H. (1984). Responding for brain stimulation: Stress and desmethylimipramine. Progress in Neuropsychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 8, 601–606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Copyright information

© Association of Behavior Analysis International 2014

Authors and Affiliations

  1. 1.Department of Psychology, MS 296University of Nevada, RenoRenoUSA

Personalised recommendations