Skip to main content
Log in

Different RAN components relate to reading at different points in time

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present 10-year longitudinal study examined how rapid automatized naming (RAN) components—articulation time and pause time—relate to reading fluency. Seventy-five Greek-speaking children were followed from Grade 1 to Grade 10 and were assessed five times (in Grades 1, 2, 4, 6, and 10) on RAN (digits and objects) and reading fluency (word reading efficiency, phonemic decoding efficiency, and text reading fluency). The results indicated first that a substantial amount of variance in reading fluency was accounted for by what articulation time and pause time shared. In addition, the unique contribution of pause time to reading fluency decreased across time and the unique contribution of articulation time increased across time. Viewed in conjunction with the developmental changes in the RAN components, our findings suggest that the RAN tasks are processed and executed in different ways across time.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Anderson, S. W., Podwall, F. N., & Jaffe, J. (1984). Timing analysis of coding and articulation processes in dyslexia. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 433, 71–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. A. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. (2005). Individual differences in RAN and reading: A response timing analysis. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 73–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cobbold, S., Passenger, T., & Terrell, C. (2003). Serial naming speed and the component elements of speech time and pause time: Relationships with the development of word-level reading in children aged four to five years. Journal of Research in Reading, 26, 165–176.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F. (2011). What discrete and serial rapid automatized naming (RAN) can reveal about reading. Scientific Studies of Reading, 15, 314–337.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (2005). Learning to read words: Theory, findings, and issues. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 167–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Papadopoulos, T. C., Fella, A., & Parrila, R. (2012). Rapid naming speed components and reading development in a consistent orthography. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Learning, 10, 199–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2009). RAN components and reading development from Grade 3 to Grade 5: What underlies the relationship? Scientific Studies of Reading, 13, 508–534.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G. K., Parrila, R., Kirby, J., & Stephenson, K. (2008a). Rapid naming components and their relationship with phonological awareness, orthographic knowledge, speed of processing and different reading outcomes. Scientific Studies of Reading, 12, 325–350.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Liao, C.-H. (2008b). Rapid naming speed reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Reading & Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 21, 885–903.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jones, M. W., Obregón, M., Kelly, M. L., & Branigan, H. P. (2008). Elucidating the component processes involved in dyslexic and non-dyslexic reading fluency: An eye-tracking study. Cognition, 109, 389–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2008). Development of word reading fluency and spelling in a consistent orthography: An 8-year follow-up. Journal of Educational Psychology, 100, 150–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lervåg, A., & Hulme, C. (2009). Rapid naming (RAN) taps a basic constraint on the development of early reading fluency. Psychological Science, 20, 1040–1048.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Li, J. J., Cutting, L. E., Ryan, M., Zilioli, M., Denckla, M. B., & Mahone, E. M. (2009). Response variability in rapid automatized naming predicts reading comprehension. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 31, 877–888.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatized naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, G., & Swank, P. (2002). Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 158–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Obregón, M. (1994). Exploring naming timing patterns by dyslexic and normal readers on the serial RAN task (Unpublished master’s thesis). Tufts University, Boston, USA.

  • Papadopoulos, T. C., Georgiou, G. K., & Kendeou, P. (2009). Investigating the double-deficit hypothesis in Greek: Findings from a longitudinal study. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 42, 528–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos T. C., & Kendeou, P. (2012, July). Cognitive and linguistic predictors of fluency and reading comprehension in adolescence. Paper presented at the 19th annual conference of the Society for the Scientific Studies of Reading in Montreal, Canada.

  • Pedhazur, E. G. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed.). Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.

    Google Scholar 

  • Powell, D., Stainthorp, R., Stuart, M., Garwood, H., & Quinlan, P. (2007). An experimental comparison between rival theories of rapid automatized naming performance and its relationship to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 98, 46–68.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., Altani, A., & Georgiou, G. (2013). Development of serial processing in reading and rapid naming. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 116, 914–929.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Protopapas, A., & Vlahou, E. (2009). A comparative quantitative analysis of Greek orthographic transparency. Behavior Research Methods, 41, 991–1008.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ramus, F. (2003). Developmental dyslexia: Specific phonological deficit or general sensorimotor dysfunction? Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 13, 212–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roman, A. A., Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R. K., Wade-Wooley, L., & Deacon, H. (2009). Toward a comprehensive view of the skills involved in word reading in Grades 4, 6, and 8. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 102, 96–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., Pillay, V., & Melidona, S. (2007). Deconstructing rapid automatized naming: Component processes and the prediction of reading difficulties. Learning and Individual Differences, 17, 129–146.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. (2006). Theoretical framework for beginning reading in different orthographies. In M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 441–462). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snyder, L. S., & Downey, D. M. (1995). Serial rapid naming skills in children with reading disabilities. Annals of Dyslexia, 45, 31–50.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn and Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth- grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J. H., & Spelberg, B. J. H. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors, and pictured objects, and word-reading speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 25–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yan, M., Pan, J., Laubrock, J., Kliegl, R., & Shu, H. (2013). Parafoveal processing in rapid automatized naming: A comparison between Chinese normal and dyslexic children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 115, 579–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported in part by Grants from the Faculty of Education, University of Alberta to G. Georgiou and the Cyprus Research Promotion Foundation to T. Papadopoulos. The authors would like to thank the principals, the teachers, and the children for their participation in this study.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to George K. Georgiou.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Georgiou, G.K., Papadopoulos, T.C. & Kaizer, E.L. Different RAN components relate to reading at different points in time. Read Writ 27, 1379–1394 (2014). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9496-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-014-9496-1

Keywords

Navigation