Skip to main content
Log in

Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The objective of this study was to examine how rapid naming speed is related to reading ability across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Forty English-speaking Canadian children, 40 Greek-speaking Cypriot children, and 40 Chinese-speaking Taiwanese children were administered RAN, reading accuracy, and reading fluency tasks in grade 4. The results revealed that across languages there were no statistically significant differences in the correlations between RAN and reading. However, a subsequent analysis of the RAN components—articulation and pause time—revealed that different RAN components may be responsible for the RAN-reading relationship across languages. Implications for existing theories relating RAN to reading are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badian, N. (2001). Phonological and orthographic processing: Their roles in reading prediction. Annals of Dyslexia, 51, 179–202.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barker, T. A., Torgesen, J. K., & Wagner, R. K. (1992). The role of orthographic processing skills on five different reading tasks. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 335–345.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bjaalid, I.-K., Hoien, T., & Lundberg, I. (1996). The contribution of orthographic and phonological processes to word reading in young Norwegian readers. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 189–198.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76, 610–622.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1995). Tracing symbol naming speed’s unique contributions to reading disability over time. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 7, 189–216.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., Golden, J. O., Kennedy, A., & Young, A. (1994). Limits upon orthographic knowledge due to processes indexed by naming speed. In V. W. Berninger (Ed.), The varieties of orthographic knowledge: Theoretical and developmental issues (pp. 173–218). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 51, 195–219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G., & Wolf, M. (1993). Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 5, 69–85.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bowey, J. A., McGuigan, M., & Ruschena, A. (2005). On the association between serial naming speed for letters and digits and word-reading skill: Towards a developmental account. Journal of Research in Reading, 28, 400–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cheung, H., McBride-Chang, C., & Chow, W.-Y. (2006). Reading in Chinese. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 421–440). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chow, B. W.-Y., McBride-Chang, C., & Burgess, S. (2005). Phonological processing skills and early reading abilities in Hong Kong Chinese kindergarteners learning to read English as a second language. Journal of Educational Psychology, 97, 81–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J., & Cohen, P. (1983). Applied multiple regression/correlation analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L. (2003). Modeling the relationship between growth in rapid naming speed and growth in decoding skill in first-grade children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 225–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cutting, L. E., & Denckla, M. B. (2001). The relationship of rapid serial naming and word reading in normally developing readers: An exploratory model. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 673–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Jong, P. F., & van der Leij, A. (1999). Specific contributions of phonological abilities to early reading acquisition: Results from a Dutch latent variable longitudinal study. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 450–476.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Filippo, G., Brizzolara, D., Chilosi, A., De Luca, M., Judica, A., Pecini, C., et al. (2005). Rapid naming, not cancellation speed or articulation rate, predicts reading in an orthographically regular language (Italian). Child Neuropsychology, 11, 349–361.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N., Natsume, M., Stavropoulou, K., Hoxhallari, L., van Daal, V., Polyzoe, N., et al. (2004). The effects of orthographic depth on learning to read alphabetic, syllabic, and logographic scripts. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 438–468.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Kirby, J. (2006). Rapid naming speed components and early reading acquisition. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 199–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., & Papadopoulos, T. (2005, August). Predictors of word decoding and reading fluency in English and Greek: A cross-linguistic comparison. Paper presentation at the 11th biennial conference of EARLI, Nicosia, Cyprus.

  • Glass, G. V., & Hopkins, K. D. (1984). Statistical methods in education and psychology (2nd ed.). Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Giannouli, V. (1999). Learning to read and spell in Greek: The importance of letter knowledge and morphological awareness. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 51–70). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Harris, M., & Hatano, G. (1999). Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hatzigeorgiou, N., Gavrilidou, M., Piperidis, S., Carayannis, G., Papakostopoulou, A., Spiliotopoulou, A., et al. (2000). Design and implementation of the online ILSP corpus. Proceedings of the second international conference of language resources and evaluation (LREC) (vol. 3, pp. 1737–1740). Athens, Greece.

  • Ho, C. S.-H., & Bryant, P. (1997). Learning to read Chinese beyond the logographic phase. Reading Research Quarterly, 32, 276–289.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D. W., Chung, K., Lee, S.-H., & Tsang, S.-M. (2007). In search of subtypes of Chinese developmental dyslexia. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 97, 61–83.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., Chan, D., Tsang, S.-M., & Lee, S.-H. (2000). The Hong Kong test of specific learning difficulties in reading and writing. Hong Kong: Hong Kong Specific learning Difficulties Research Team.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ho, C. S.-H., & Lai, D. N.-C. (1999). Naming speed deficits and phonological memory deficits in Chinese developmental dyslexia. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 173–186.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Holopainen, L., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Predicting delay in reading achievement in a highly transparent language. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 401–413.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hu, C.-F., & Catts, H. W. (1998). The role of phonological processing in early reading ability: What we can learn from Chinese. Scientific Studies of Reading, 2, 55–79.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Huang, H.-S. (2001). Graded Chinese character recognition test. Taipei: Psychology Publication.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joshi, M., & Aaron, P. G. (2006). Handbook of orthography and literacy. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kirby, J. R., Parrila, R., & Pfeiffer, S. (2003). Naming speed and phonological awareness as predictors of reading development. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 453–464.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kobayashi, M. S., Haynes, C. W., Macaruso, P., Hook, P. E., & Kato, J. (2005). Effects of mora deletion, nonword repetition, rapid naming, and visual search performance on beginning reading in Japanese. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 105–128.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Landerl, K., & Wimmer, H. (2000). Deficits in phoneme segmentation are not the core problem of dyslexia: Evidence from German and English children. Applied Psycholinguistics, 21, 243–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lepola, J., Poskiparta, E., Laakkonen, E., & Niemi, P. (2005). Development of and relationship between phonological and motivational processes and naming speed in predicting word recognition in grade 1. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 367–399.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Liao, C.-H., Georgiou, G., & Parrila, R. (in press). Rapid naming speed and Chinese character recognition. Reading and Writing. Available online at http://www.springerlink.com/content/100333/?Content+Status=Accepted

  • Manis, F. R., Doi, L. M., & Bhadha, B. (2000). Naming speed, phonological awareness, and orthographic knowledge in second graders. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 325–333.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Seidenberg, M. S., & Doi, L. M. (1999). See Dick RAN: Rapid naming and the longitudinal prediction of reading subskills in first and second graders. Scientific Studies of Reading, 3, 129–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V., & Wimmer, H. (2002). Phoneme awareness and pathways into literacy: A comparison of German and American children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 15, 653–682.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayringer, H., Wimmer, W., & Landerl, K. (1998). Phonological skills and literacy acquisition in German. In P. Reitsma & L. Verhoeven (Eds.), Problems and interventions in literacy development (pp. 147–161). Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Cho, J.-R., Liu, H., Wagner, R., Shu, H., Zhou, A., et al. (2005). Changing models across cultures: Associations of phonological awareness and morphological structure awareness with vocabulary and word recognition in second graders from Beijing, Hong Kong, Korea, and the United States. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 92, 140–160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Ho, C. S.-H. (2005). Predictors of beginning reading in Chinese and English: A 2-year longitudinal study of Chinese kindergartners. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9, 117–144.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Kail, R. V. (2002). Cross-cultural similarities in the predictors of reading acquisition. Child Development, 73, 1392–1407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Manis, F. R. (1996). Structural invariance in the associations of naming speed, phonological awareness, and verbal reasoning in good and poor readers: A test of the double-deficit hypothesis. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 8, 323–339.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., Shu, H., Zhou, A., Wat, C. P., & Wagner, R. K. (2003). Morphological awareness uniquely predicts young children’s Chinese character recognition. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 743–751.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McBride-Chang, C., & Zhong, Y. (2003). A longitudinal study of effects of phonological processing, visual skills, and speed of processing on Chinese character acquisition among Hong Kong Chinese kindergartners. In C. McBride-Chang & H.-C. Chen (Eds.), Reading development in Chinese children (pp. 37–49). Westport: Praeger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Naglieri, J. A., & Das, J. P. (1997). Das-Naglieri cognitive assessment system. Itasca: Houghton Mifflin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, G., Foorman, B. R., Francis, D. J., & Carlson, C. D. (2001). Measures of information processing in rapid automatized naming (RAN) and their relation to reading. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 78, 359–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Neuhaus, G., & Swank, P. (2002). Understanding the relations between RAN letter subtest components and word reading in first-grade students. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 35, 158–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nikolopoulos, D., Goulandris, N., Hulme, C., & Snowling, M. J. (2006). The cognitive bases of learning to read and spell in Greek: Evidence from a longitudinal study. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 94, 1–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Papadopoulos, T. C. (2001). Phonological and cognitive correlates of word-reading acquisition under two different instructional approaches. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 16, 549–567.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Parrila, R., Kirby, J. R., & McQuarrie, L. (2004). Articulation rate, naming speed, verbal short-term memory, and phonological awareness: Longitudinal predictors of early reading development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 3–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Patel, T., Snowling, M. J., & de Jong, P. F. (2004). A cross-linguistic comparison of children learning to read in English and Dutch. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 785–797.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pedhazur, E. G. (1982). Multiple regression in behavioral research: Explanation and prediction (2nd ed.). New York: CBS College Publishing.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2003). The interaction between phonological processing, syntactic awareness, and naming speed in the reading and spelling performance of first-grade children. Brain and Cognition, 53, 287–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Plaza, M., & Cohen, H. (2007). The contribution of phonological awareness and visual attention in early reading and spelling. Dyslexia, 13, 67–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rahbari, N., Sénéchal, M., & Arab-Mohdaddam, N. (2007). The role of orthographic and phonological processing skills in the reading and spelling of monolingual Persian children. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 20, 511–533.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Savage, R., & Frederickson, N. (2005). Evidence of a highly specific relationship between rapid automatic naming and text-reading speed. Brain and Language, 93, 152–159.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Predicting the future achievement of second graders with reading disabilities: Contributions of phonemic awareness, verbal memory, rapid naming, and IQ. Annals of Dyslexia, 48, 115–136.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schatschneider, C., Fletcher, J. M., Francis, D. J., Carlson, C. D., & Foorman, B. R. (2004). Kindergarten prediction of reading skills: A longitudinal comparative analysis. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96, 265–282.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Boston: Allyn & Bacon.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tan, L. H., Spinks, J. A., Eden, G. F., Perfetti, C. A., & Siok, W. T. (2005). Reading depends on writing, in Chinese. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 102, 8781–8785.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1994). Longitudinal studies of phonological processing and reading. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 27, 276–286.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., & Rashotte, C. A. (1999). Test of word reading efficiency. Austin: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J. K., Wagner, R. K., Rashotte, C. A., Burgess, S., & Hecht, S. (1997). Contributions of phonological awareness and rapid automatized naming ability to growth of word-reading skills in second- to fifth-grade children. Scientific Studies of Reading, 1, 161–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • van den Bos, K. P., Zijlstra, B. J. H., & Spelberg, B. J. H. (2002). Life-span data on continuous-naming speeds of numbers, letters, colors, and pictured objects, and word-reading speed. Scientific Studies of Reading, 6, 25–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychological Bulletin, 101, 192–212.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children-third edition. San Antonio: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Goswami, U. (1994). The influence of orthographic consistency on reading development: Word recognition, in English and German children. Cognition, 51, 91–103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., Landerl, C., & Frith, U. (1999). Learning to read German: Normal and impaired acquisition. In M. Harris & G. Hatano (Eds.), Learning to read and write: A cross-linguistic perspective (pp. 34–50). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H., & Mayringer, H. (2001). Is the reading-rate problem of German children caused by slow visual processes? In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency, and the brain (pp. 93–102). Timonium: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Bowers, P. G. (1999). The double-deficit hypothesis for the developmental dyslexias. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 415–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bowers, P. G., & Biddle, K. (2000). Naming-speed processes, timing, and reading: A conceptual review. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 33, 387–407.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., & Denckla, M. B. (2005). Rapid automatized naming and rapid alternating stimulus tests (RAN/RAS). Austin: PRO-ED.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woodcock, R. W. (1998). Woodcock reading mastery test-revised-normative update examiner’s manual. Circle Pines: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to George K. Georgiou.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Georgiou, G.K., Parrila, R. & Liao, CH. Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Read Writ 21, 885–903 (2008). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9096-4

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-007-9096-4

Keywords

Navigation