Skip to main content
Log in

The role of letters and syllables in typical and dysfluent reading in a transparent orthography

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The role of letters and syllables in typical and dysfluent second grade reading in Finnish, a transparent orthography, was assessed by lexical decision and naming tasks. Typical readers did not show reliable word length effects in lexical decision, suggesting establishment of parallel letter processing. However, there were small effects of word syllable structure in both tasks suggesting the presence of some sublexical processing also. Dysfluent readers showed large word length effects in both tasks indicating decoding at the letter-phoneme level. When lexical access was required in a lexical decision task, dyslexics additionally chunked the letters into syllables. Response duration measure revealed that dysfluent readers even sounded out the words in phoneme-by-phoneme fashion, depending on the task difficulty. This letter-by-letter decoding is enabled by the transparent orthography and promoted by Finnish reading education.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Àlvarez, C. J., Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2004). Are syllables phonological units in visual word recognition? Language and Cognitive Processes, 19, 427–452.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ans, B., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (1998). A connectionist multiple-trace memory model for polysyllabic word reading. Psychological Review, 105, 678–723.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M. (2006). Learning to read: The effect of orthography. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 531–550). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aro, M., & Wimmer, H. (2003). Learning to read: English in comparison to six more regular orthographies. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 621–635.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Baayen, R. H., Feldman, L. B., & Schreuder, R. (2006). Morphological influences on the recognition of monosyllabic monomorphemic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 55, 290–313.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D. A., Boland, J. E., & Shields, L. W. (1989). Priming in pronunciation: Beyond pattern recognition and onset latency. Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 14–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Balota, D., Cortese, M., Sergent-Marshall, S., Spieler, D., & Yap, M. (2004). Visual word recognition for single-syllable words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 133, 283–316.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barca, L., Burani, C., & Arduino, L. S. (2002). Word naming times and psycholinguistic norms for Italian nouns. Behavior Research Methods, 34, 424–434.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bergmann, J., & Wimmer, H. (2008). A dual-route perspective on poor reading in a regular orthography: Evidence from phonological and orthographic lexical decisions. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 653–676.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bertram, R., & Hyönä, J. (2003). The length of a complex word modifies the role of morphological structure: Evidence from eye movements when reading short and long Finnish compounds. Journal of Memory and Language, 48, 615–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., Ferrand, L., Grainger, J., & Perea, M. (2005). Sequential effects of phonological priming in visual word recognition. Psychological Science, 16, 585–589.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carreiras, M., & Perea, M. (2007). Naming pseudowords in Spanish: Effects of syllable frequency. Brain and Language, 90, 393–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M. (2004). Are there lexicons? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 57, 1153–1171.

    Google Scholar 

  • Coltheart, M., Rastle, K., Perry, C., Langdon, R., & Ziegler, J. (2001). DRC: A dual route cascaded model of visual word recognition and reading aloud. Psychological Review, 108, 204–256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Barca, L., Burani, C., & Zoccolotti, P. (2008). The effect of word length and other sublexical, lexical, and semantic variables on developmental reading deficits. Cognitive & Behavioral Neurology, 21, 227–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Borrelli, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2002). Reading words and pseudowords: An eye movement study of developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 80, 617–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Burani, C., Paizi, D., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2010). Letter and letter-string processing in developmental dyslexia. Cortex, 46, 1272–1283.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (1999). Eye movement patterns in linguistic and non-linguistic tasks in developmental surface dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 37, 1407–1420.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Di Filippo, G. D., De Luca, M., Judica, A., Spinelli, D., & Zoccolotti, P. (2006). Lexicality and stimulus length effects in Italian dyslexics: Role of the overadditivity effect. Child Neuropsychology, 12, 141–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, L. (2000). Reading aloud polysyllabic words and pseudowords: The syllabic length effect reexamined. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7, 142–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ferrand, L., & New, B. (2003). Syllabic length effects in visual word recognition and naming. Acta Psychologica, 113, 167–183.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frederiksen, J. R., & Kroll, J. F. (1976). Spelling and sound: Approaches to the internal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 361–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (1998). Toward a strong phonological theory of visual word recognition: True issues and false trails. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 71–99.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gagl, B., Hawelka, S., & Wimmer, H. (2010). Acquisition of efficient visual word processing: Evidence from eye movement and naming latencies. Poster session presented at the 17th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Berlin, Germany.

  • Goswami, U. (1995). Phonological development and reading by analogy: What is analogy, and what is not? Journal of Research in Reading, 18, 139–145.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Häikiö, T., Hyönä, J., & Bertram, R. (2010). The role of syllables in word recognition. Poster session presented at the 17th Annual Meeting of the Society for the Scientific Study of Reading, Berlin, Germany.

  • Hall, R. A. (1974). Ordered rules for Italian syllabification. Italica, 51, 305–307.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hawelka, S., Gagl, B., & Wimmer, H. (2010). A dual-route perspective on eye movements of dyslexic readers. Cognition, 115, 367–379.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Häyrinen, T., Serenius-Sirve, S., & Korkman, M. (1999). Lukilasse. Psykologien Kustannus, Helsinki[Lukilasse-test battery for screening reading, spelling, and arithmetics].

  • Hutzler, F., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. M. (2005). Effects of syllable-frequency in lexical decision and naming: An eye-movement study. Brain and Language, 92, 138–152.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hutzler, F., & Wimmer, H. (2004). Eye movements of dyslexic children when reading in a regular orthography. Brain and Language, 89, 235–242.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hyönä, J., Bertram, R., & Pollatsek, A. (2004). Are long compound words identified serially via their constituents? Evidence from an eye-movement-contingent display change study. Memory & Cognition, 32, 523–532.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jared, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). Naming multisyllabic words. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 16, 92–105.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Juphard, A., Carbonnel, S., & Valdois, S. (2004). Length effect in reading and lexical decision: Evidence from skilled readers and a developmental dyslexic participant. Brain and Cognition, 55, 332–340.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Karlson, F. (1999). Suomen peruskielioppi [Finnish: An Essential Grammar.]. London, England: Routledge.

  • Katz, L., & Frost, R. (1992). The reading process is different for different orthographies: The orthographic depth hypothesis. In R. Frost & L. Katz (Eds.), Advances in psychology: Orthography, phonology, morphology, and meaning (Vol. 94, pp. 67–84). Oxford, England: North-Holland.

  • Kawamoto, A. H., Kello, C. T., Jones, R., & Bame, K. (1998). Initial phoneme versus whole-word criterion to initiate pronunciation: Evidence based on response latency and initial phoneme duration. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning Memory and Cognition, 24, 862–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kotimaisten kielten tutkimuskeskus. [Research Institute for the Languages of Finland]. (2007). Kirjoitetun suomen kielen sanojen taajuuksia. [Frequencies of written Finnish words]. Retrieved from http://kaino.kotus.fi/sanat/taajuuslista/parole.php.

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K. (2007). The beginning phases of reading literacy instruction in Finland. In P. Linnakylä & I. Arffman (Eds.), Finnish reading literacy. When quality and equity meet (pp. 155–174). Jyväskylä, Finland: University of Jyväskylä, Institute for Educational Research.

  • Lerkkanen, M.-K., Niemi, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Poskiparta, M., Siekkinen, M., & Nurmi, J.-E. (2006). The first steps study (Alkuportaat, ongoing). Finland: University of Jyväskylä.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Language development and literacy skills in late-talking toddlers with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Annals of Dyslexia, 55, 166–192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2006). The effect of word length on lexical decision in dyslexic and normal reading children. Brain and Language, 98, 140–149.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martens, V. E. G., & de Jong, P. F. (2008). Effects of repeated reading on the length effect in word and pseudoword reading. Journal of Research in Reading, 31, 40–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • New, B., Ferrand, L., Pallier, C., & Brysbaert, M. (2006). Reexamining the word length effect in visual word recognition: New evidence from the English Lexicon Project. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 13, 45–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orsolini, M., Fanari, R., Cerrachio, S., & Famiglietti, L. (2009). Phonological and lexical reading in Italian children with dyslexia. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 22, 933–954.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pagliuca, G., & Monaghan, P. (2010). Discovering large grain sizes in a transparent orthography: Insights from a connectionist model of Italian word naming’. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 22, 813–835.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perry, C., Ziegler, J. C., & Zorzi, M. (2010). Beyond single syllables: Large-scale modeling of reading aloud with the Connectionist Dual Process (CDP ++) model. Cognitive Psychology, 61, 106–151.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugh, K. R., Frost, S. J., Sandak, R., Landi, N., Rueckl, J. G., Constable, R. T., et al. (2008). Effects of stimulus difficulty and repetition on printed word identification: An fmri comparison of nonimpaired and reading-disabled adolescent cohorts. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1146–1160.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J., & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: A review. Reading Research Quarterly, 27, 29–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rastle, K., Harrington, J., Coltheart, M., & Palethorpe, S. (2000). Reading aloud begins when the computation of phonology is complete. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 26, 1178–1191.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & Plaut, D. C. (1998). Evaluating word-reading models at the item level: Matching the grain of theory and data. Psychological Science, 9, 234–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003). Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1995). Phonological recoding and self-teaching: Sine qua non of reading acquisition. Cognition, 55, 151–218.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spinelli, D., De Luca, M., Di Filippo, G., Mancini, M., Martelli, M., & Zoccolotti, P. (2005). Length effect in word naming in reading: Role of reading experience and reading deficit in Italian readers. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27, 217–235.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spoehr, K. T., & Smith, E. E. (1973). The role of orthographic and phonotactic rules in perceiving letter patterns. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 104, 21–34.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stenneken, P., Conrad, M., & Jacobs, A. (2007). Processing of syllables in production and recognition tasks. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 36, 65–78.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. V., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): What we have learned in the past four decades. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Weekes, B. S. (1997). Differential effects of number of letters on word and nonword naming latency. The quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 50A, 439–456.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1993). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system. Applied Psycholinguistics, 14, 1–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1996a). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia: Evidence from children learning to read German. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 61, 80–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (1996b). The early manifestation of developmental dyslexia: Evidence from German children. Reading and Writing, 8, 171–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Jacobs, A. M., & Braun, M. (2001). Identical words are read differently in different languages. Psychological Science, 12, 380–384.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ziegler, J. C., Perry, C., Ma-Wyatt, A., Ladner, D., & Schulte-Körne, G. (2003). Developmental dyslexia in different languages: Language-specific or universal? Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 86, 169–193.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Di Pace, E., Gasperini, F., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2005). Word length effect in early reading and in developmental dyslexia. Brain and Language, 93, 369–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zoccolotti, P., De Luca, M., Judica, A., & Spinelli, D. (2008). Isolating global and specific factors in developmental dyslexia: A study based on the rate and amount model (RAM). Experimental Brain Research, 186, 551–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Jarkko Hautala.

Appendix: List of experimental items

Appendix: List of experimental items

Naming, words

4L2S: lelu, pipo, naru, poro, rapu, sora, mopo, mato, kynä, lasi, sade, kala, juna, vene, kisa, mäki, susi, loma, meri, rivi, raja, veli, koti, valo, kivi, 6L2S: koukku, munkki, nilkka, kurkku, kaivos, lautta, piippu, suihku, lamppu, kuoppa, mainos, patsas, rengas, verkko, kiekko, myrsky, puisto, kerros, leikki, vanhus, taivas, hammas, luonto, kirkko, kauppa, 6L3S: seteli, pusero, pisara, kipinä, vasara, sipuli, peruna, kitara, veturi, meteli, kävely, puhelu, kamera, satama, kanava, ikkuna, mitali, tavara, enkeli, matala, paperi, kaveri, numero, mukava, tarina.

Naming, pseudowords

4L2S: heku, sito, salu, toso, sanu, kopa, lovo, rapo, pylä, jati, tame, jatu, tusa, seke, mila, pähi, luti, tova, sesi, lini, laha, resi, hopi, naro, pini, 6L2S: jousku, kulkki, kiikka, sunkku, haipos, vausta, tiilpu, luisku, talppu, nuuppa, tainos, lattas, pentas, teekko, hiesko, kyysky, vuinto, pertos, reiski, nantus, malvas, jasmas, ruusto, niikko, launpa, 6L3S: keveli, lutero, mikara, liminä, pavara, tiluri, kevuna, pikara, sepuri, hekeli, rätely, muselu, pahera, malama, japava, okkina, nirali, sapara, ankila, rakala, naseri, naveli, tupero, suhava, hapira.

Lexical decision, words

4L2S: hame, havu, latu, maha, tori, mehu, mono, muna, muru, namu, pomo, romu, räme, sumu, papu, vesi, talo, raha, kone, käsi, pora, kylä, levy, lumi, katu, 6L2S: vaippa, veitsi, viitta, viikko, viesti, heikko, sääntö, käärme, miekka, hiekka, laukku, kaappi, toukka, keitto, kioski, kortti, kyltti, liekki, niitty, pensas, piikki, pultti, reitti, teltta, turkki, 6L3S: kolari, lattia, ritari, tivoli, väline, salama, terävä, sokeri, sekava, kamala, ämpäri, kypärä, lokero, pipari, kotelo, hunaja, rypäle, majava, lakana, sopiva, vakava, sanoma, matala, ystävä, tarina,

Lexical decision, pseudowords

4L2S: läte, junu, nalu, hepi, rano, lata, roje, pähi, vola, mykä, jery, ruvi, raku, pale, jatu, hapu, kaja, nosi, seku, romo, vuha, luvu, tasu, soko, kopu, 6L2S: leisto, tooski, soltti, nyrtti, keekki, tiitta, tenpas, huikki, tuntti, leisti, kertta, sulkki, haappa, reetsi, piirta, seikko, kiesto, jeikku, läänti, täisme, vienka, leekka, sausku, taalpi, routka, 6L3S: motero, tovake, voselo, ruhaja, kytäle, patava, sapana, rokiva, lapava, kaloma, rasala, ästivä, sapina, posari, sartia, linari, nitoli, närine, tanama, melävä, hoperi, lemava, parala, amperi, nykärä.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Hautala, J., Aro, M., Eklund, K. et al. The role of letters and syllables in typical and dysfluent reading in a transparent orthography. Read Writ 26, 845–864 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9394-3

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-012-9394-3

Keywords

Navigation