Skip to main content
Log in

Language development and literacy skills in late-talking toddlers with and without familial risk for dyslexia

  • Published:
Annals of Dyslexia Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The relationship between late-talkers’ language development and reading and spelling outcomes was examined in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. The late-talking subgroups were defined using parent- and test-based assessments of receptive and expressive vocabulary and grammar at 2 and 2.5 years as intake criteria. The language skills of late talkers and the remainders of these two groups were assessed at 3.5, 5, and 5.5 years. Reading/spelling outcomes were compared at the end of the second grade. Late-talking toddlers of the at-risk group who had both poor receptive and expressive skills performed less well than all other groups on language measurements at 5.5 years. In contrast, the control group’s late talkers with an expressive delay reached the language level of their age-mates already by 3.5 years, and maintained their age-appropriate position two years later. The most significant differences in the reading skills were found between the at-risk children with receptive and expressive delay and the remainder of the controls. Age-appropriate early language skills did not, however, ensure norm-level fluent reading in the at-risk group. The remainder of the at-risk group performed at a significantly lower level than did the remainder of the controls, both on the oral reading and spelling tasks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Bates, E., Dale, P. S., & Thal, D. (1995). Individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. In P. Fletcher & B. MacWhinney (Eds.), The handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., & Goodman, J. C. (2001). On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition. In M. Tomasello & E. Bates (Eds.), Language development. The essential readings (pp. 134–162). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bayley, N. (1993). The Bayley scales of infant development (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Choudhury, N., & Benasich, A. (2003). A family aggregation study: The influence of family history and other risk factors on language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 261–272.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dale, P. S., Price, T. S., Bishop, D. V. M., & Plomin, R. (2003). Outcomes of early language delay: 1. Predicting persistent and transient language difficulties at 3 and 4 years. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 544–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1981). Peabody picture vocabulary test-revised. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fenson, L., Dale, P. S., Reznick, J. S., Bates, E., Thal, D., & Pethick, S. J. (1994). Variability in early communicative development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 59(5, ser. no. 242).

  • Gallagher, A., Frith, U., & Snowling, M. J. (2000). Precursors of literacy delay among children at genetic risk of dyslexia. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 41, 202–213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Girolametto, L., Wiigs, M., Smyth, R., Weitzman, E., & Pearce, P. S. (2001). Children with a history of expressive vocabulary delay: Outcomes at 5 years of age. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 10, 358–369.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Guttorm, T. K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Poikkeus, A.-M., Eklund, K. M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2005). Brain event-related potentials (ERPs) measured at birth predict later language development in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Cortex, 41, 291–303.

    Google Scholar 

  • Guttorm, T. K., Leppänen, P. H. T., Richardson, U., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Event-related potentials and consonant differentiation in newborns with familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 34, 534–544.

    Google Scholar 

  • Heilmann, J., Ellis Weismer, S., Evans, J., & Hollar, C. (2005). Utility of the MacArthur-Bates communicative development inventory in identifying language abilities of late-talking and typically developing toddlers. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 14, 40–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kaplan, E., Goodglass, H., & Weintraub, S. (1983). The Boston naming test (2nd ed.). Philadelphia: Lea & Febiger.

    Google Scholar 

  • Korkman, M. (1998). NEPSY: A developmental neuropsychological assessment. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leinonen, S., Müller, K., Leppänen, P., Aro, M., Ahonen, T., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Heterogeneity in adult dyslexic readers: Relating processing skills to the speed and accuracy of oral text reading. Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 14, 265–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leppänen, P. H. T., Richardson, U., Pihko, E., Eklund, K. M., Guttorm, T. K., Aro, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Brain responses to changes in speech sound durations differ between infants with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 22, 407–422.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Levenstein, P., Levenstein, S., Shiminski, J. A., & Stolzberg, J. E. (1998). Long-term impact of a verbal interaction program for at-risk toddlers: An exploratory study of high school outcomes in a replication of the mother-child home program. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 19, 267–285.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lindeman, J. (2000). Ala-asteen lukutesti: käyttäjän käsikirja. [The Comprehensive School Reading Test]. Oppimistutkimuskeskus, Turun yliopisto.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Eklund, K., Erskine, J., Guttorm, T. K., Laakso, M.-L., Leppänen, P. H. T., Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Richardson, U., & Torppa, M. (2004). The development of children at familial risk for dyslexia: Birth to early school age. Annals of Dyslexia, 54, 184–220.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Aro, M., Holopainen, L., Leiwo, M., Lyytinen, P., & Tolvanen, A. (2006). Children’s language development and reading acquisition in a highly transparent orthography. In R. M. Joshi & P. G. Aaron (Eds.), Handbook of orthography and literacy (pp. 47–62). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, H., Erskine, J., Tolvanen, A., Torppa, M., Poikkeus, A.-M., & Lyytinen, P. (in press). Developmental trajectories to dyslexia: A follow-up from birth to school age. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly.

  • Lyytinen, P. (1999). Varhaisen kommunikaation ja kielen kehityksen arviointimenetelmä (Finnish manual for communicative development inventories). University of Jyväskylä, Child Research Center and Niilo Mäki Institute. Jyväskylä: Yliopistopaino.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). The play and language behavior of mothers with and without dyslexia and its association to their toddlers’ language development. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 74–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2004). Growth and predictive relations of vocabulary and inflectional morphology in children with and without dyslexia. Applied Psycholinguistics, 25, 397–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lyytinen, P., Poikkeus, A.-M., Laakso, M.-L., Eklund, K., & Lyytinen, H. (2001). Language development and symbolic play in children with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 873–885.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCardle, P., Scarborough, H. S., & Catts, H. W. (2001). Predicting, explaining, and preventing children’s reading difficulties. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 16(4), 230–239.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McCathren, R. B., Warren, S. F., & Yoder, P. (1996). Prelinguistic predictors of later language development. In K. N. Cole, P. S. Dale, & D. J. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (pp. 57–75). Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes.

    Google Scholar 

  • Nation, K. (2005). Connections between language and reading in children with poor reading comprehension. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 41–54). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R. (2000). Predicting outcomes of early expressive language delay: Ethical implications. In D. V. M. Bishop & L. B. Leonard (Eds.), Speech and language impairments in children. Causes, characteristics, intervention and outcome (pp. 195–209). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Paul, R., Murray, C., Clancy, K., & Andrews, D. (1997). Reading and metaphonological outcomes in late talkers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 1037–1047.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pennington, B. F., & Lefly, D. L. (2001). Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Development, 72, 816–833.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1992). Standard progressive matrices. Oxford, UK: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L. (1989). The language development survey: A screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 54, 587–599.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L. (2000). Do late-talking toddlers turn out to have reading difficulties a decade later? Annals of Dyslexia, 50, 87–102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L. (2002). Language and reading outcomes to age 9 in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 360–371.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L. (2005). Age 13 language and reading outcomes in late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48, 459–472.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rescorla, L., Dahlsgaard, K., & Roberts, J. (2000). Late-talking toddlers: MLU and IPSyn outcomes at 3;0 and 4;0. Journal of Child Language, 27, 643–664.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Reynell, J. K., & Huntley, M. (1987). Reynell developmental language scales manual (4th ed.). Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.

    Google Scholar 

  • Richardson, U., Leppänen, P. H. T., Leiwo, M., & Lyytinen, H. (2003). Speech perception of infants with high familial risk for dyslexia differ at the age of six months. Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 387–397.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1990). Very early language deficits in dyslexic children. Child Development, 61, 1728–1743.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (1998). Early identification of children at risk for reading disabilities: Phonological awareness and some other promising predictors. In B. K. Shapiro, P. J. Accardo, & A. J. Capute (Eds.), Specific reading disability: A view of the spectrum (pp. 75–119). Timonium, MD: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (2001). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In S. Neuman & D. Dickinson (Eds.), Handbook for research in early literacy (pp. 97–110). New York: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scarborough, H. S. (2005). Developmental relationships between language and reading: Reconciling a beautiful hypothesis with some ugly facts. In H. W. Catts & A. G. Kamhi (Eds.), The connections between language and reading disabilities (pp. 3–24). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seymour, P. H. K., Aro, M., & Erskine, J. M. (2003) Foundation literacy acquisition in European orthographies. British Journal of Psychology, 94, 143–174.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (2003). Family risk of dyslexia is continuous: Individual differences in the precursors of reading skill. Child Development, 74, 358–373.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Spitz, R. V., Tallal, P., Flax, J., & Benasich, A. A. (1997). Look who’s talking: A prospective study of familial transmission of language impairments. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 40, 990–1001.

    Google Scholar 

  • Turunen, P. (2003). Production of word structures. A constraint-based study of 2, 6-year-old Finnish children at-risk for dyslexia and their controls. Jyväskylä Studies in Language, 54.

  • Viholainen, H., Ahonen, T., Cantell, M., Lyytinen, P., & Lyytinen, H. (2002). Development of early motor skills and language in children at risk for familial dyslexia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 44, 761–769.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1991). Wechsler intelligence scale for children. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation.

    Google Scholar 

  • Whitehurst, G. J., & Fischel, J. E. (1994). Early developmental language delay: What, if anything, should the clinician do about it? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 35, 613–648.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Paula Lyytinen.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Lyytinen, P., Eklund, K. & Lyytinen, H. Language development and literacy skills in late-talking toddlers with and without familial risk for dyslexia. Ann. of Dyslexia 55, 166–192 (2005). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0010-y

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11881-005-0010-y

Key Words

Navigation