Skip to main content
Log in

Lexical decision with left, right and center visual field presentation: a comparison between dyslexic and regular readers by means of electrophysiological and behavioral measures

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This study examined the differences in processing between regular and dyslexic readers in a lexical decision task in different visual field presentations (left, right, and center). The research utilized behavioral measures that provide information on accuracy and reaction time and electro-physiological measures that permit the examination of brain activity during cognitive processing. Two groups of university students, regular and dyslexic readers, were matched on age, gender, intelligence, socioeconomic status, and handedness. A lexical decision task was used in order to examine the processes during word recognition. Subjects were required to decide whether a sequence of letters constituted a real word existing in spoken language or whether the stimulus seen was not an accurate word. For the behavioral measures, it was found that the dyslexics read slower and with more errors than the regular readers. Moreover, the ERP components appeared later in dyslexics as compared to regular readers in this task. The performance of the dyslexics improved and even approached that of the regular readers when the stimuli were presented to the left visual field. Thus, it seems that the dyslexics were relying more on their right hemisphere for linguistic processing, whereas the regular readers were relying more on their language areas in the left hemisphere.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4
Fig. 5
Fig. 6
Fig. 7

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Allan, K., Wilding, E. L., & Rugg, M. D. (1998). Electrophysiological evidence for dissociable processes contributing to recollection. Acta Psychologica, 98, 231–252.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Arzouan, Y., Goldstein, A., & Faust, M. (2007). Brainwaves are stethoscopes: ERP correlates of novel metaphor comprehension. Brain Research, 1160, 69–81.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Badzakova Trajkov, G., Hamm, J. P., & Waldie, K. E. (2005). The effects of redundant stimuli on visuospatial processing in developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia, 43(3), 473–778.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banich, M. T. (1998). The missing link: The role of inter-hemispheric interaction in attentional processing. Brain and Cognition, 36, 128–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Banich, M. T. (2004). Cognitive neuroscience and neuropsychology. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Banich, M. T., Passarotti, A. M., & Janes, D. (2000). Interhemispheric interaction during childhood: 1. Neurologically intact children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 18, 33–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnea, A., & Breznitz, Z. (1998). Phonological and orthographic processing of Hebrew words: Electrophysiological aspects. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 159(4), 492–504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnea, A., Lamm, O., Epstein, R., & Pratt, H. (1994). Brain potentials from dyslexic children recorded during short-term memory tasks. International Journal of Neuroscience, 74, 227–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W. (1991). Proto-literacy, literacy and the acquisition of phonological awareness. Learning and Individual Differences, 3(3), 243–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bell, S. M., McCallum, S. R., & Cox, E. A. (2003). Towards a research-based assessment of dyslexia: Using cognitive measures to identify reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 505–516.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben Dror, I., Polatsek, A., & Scarpati, S. (1991). Word identification in isolation and in context by college students. Brain and Language, 31, 308–327.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandeis, D. L., Vitacco, D., & Steinhausen, H. C. (1994). Mapping brain electric micro-states in dyslexic children during reading. Acta Paedopsychiatrica, 56, 239–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997a). Enhancing the reading of dyslexics by reading acceleration and auditory masking. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 103–113.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (1997b). The effect of accelerated reading rate on memory for text among dyslexic readers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 287–299.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2001). The determinants of reading fluency: A comparison of dyslexic and average readers. In M. Wolf (Ed.), Dyslexia, fluency and the brain (pp. 245–276). Cambridge, MA: York Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2002a). Frequency of reading difficulties and disabilities among fourth graders in Jewish population of Israel. Jerusalem: Ministry of Education Report.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2002b). Asynchrony of visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological word recognition processes: An underlying factor in dyslexia. Reading and Writing, 15(1–2), 15–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2003). Speed of phonological and orthographic processing as factors in dyslexia: Electrophysiological evidence. Genetic, Social and General Psychology Monographs, 129(2), 183–206.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2005). Brain activity during performance of naming tasks: Comparison between dyslexic and regular readers. Scientific Studies of Reading, 9(1), 17–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z. (2006). Fluency in reading: synchronization of processes. Mahwah, NJ, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum and Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Meyler, A. (2003). Speed of lower-level auditory and visual processing as a basic factor in dyslexia: Electrophysiological evidence. Brain and Language, 85(1), 166–184.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breznitz, Z., & Misra, M. (2003). Speed of processing of the visual-orthographic and auditory-phonological systems in adult dyslexics: The contribution of “asynchrony” to word recognition deficits. Brain and Language, 85(3), 486–502.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brickenkamp, R. (1981). Test d2: Aufmerk-sankeits-delastungf-test (7th ed.). Gottingen: Varlag fur Psychology, Dr. C.J. Hogrefe.

    Google Scholar 

  • British Psychological Society (1999). Dyslexia literacy and psychological assessment. Report by a working party of the division of educational and child psychology. Leicester: British Psychological Society.

  • Bruck, M. (1990). Word-recognition skills of adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Developmental Psychology, 26, 439–454.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1992). Persistence of dyslexics’ phonological awareness deficits. Developmental Psychology, 28, 874–886.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bruck, M. (1998). Outcomes of adults with childhood histories of dyslexia. In C. Hulme & J. R. Malatesha (Eds.), Reading and spelling: Development and disorders (pp. 179–200). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brunswick, N., McCrory, E., Price, C. J., Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (1999). Explicit and implicit processing of words and pseudowords by adult developmental dyslexics. Brain, 122(10), 1901–1917.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Compton, D. L., & Carlisle, J. F. (1994). Speed of word recognition as a distinguishing characteristic of reading disabilities. Educational Psychology Review, 6, 115–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Corcos, E., & Willows, D. M. (1993). The processing of orthographic information. In D. N. Willows, R. S. Kruk, & E. Corcos (Eds.), Visual processing in reading and reading disabilities (pp. 163–190). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., Leslie, S. C., & Saron, C. D. (1990). Reaction time measures of interhemispheric transfer time in reading disabled and normal children. Neuropsychologia, 28, 471–485.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Davidson, R. J., & Saron, C. D. (1992). Evoked potential measures of interhemispheric transfer time in reading disabled and normal children. Developmental Neuropsychology, 8, 261–277.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Duncan, C. C., Rumsey, J. M., Wilkniss, S. M., Denckla, M. B., Hamburger, S. D., & Odou-Potkin, M. (1994). Developmental dyslexia and attention dysfunction in adults: Brain potential indices of information processing. Psychophysiology, 31, 386–401.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, B. R., Dunn, D. A., Languis, M., & Andrews, D. (1998). The relation of ERP components to complex memory processing. Brain and Cognition, 36, 355–376.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Elbro, C., Nielsen, I., & Peterson, D. K. (1994). Dyslexia in adults: Evidence for deficits in non-word reading and in the phonological representation of lexical items. Annals of Dyslexia, 44, 205–226.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Erez, A., & Pratt, H. (1992). Auditory event-related potentials among dyslexic and normal-reading children: 3CLT and midline comparisons. International Journal of Neuroscience, 63, 247–264.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fawcett, A. J., Chattopadhyay, A. K., Kandler, R. H., Jarrat, J. A., Nicolson, R. I., & Proctor, M. (1993). Event-related potentials and dyslexia. In P. Tallal, A. M. Galaburda, R. R. Llinas, & C. von Eurler (Eds.), Temporal information processing in the nervous system. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 682, 342–345.

  • Flowers, D. L. (1995). Neuropsychological profiles of persistent reading disability and reading improvement. In C. K. Leong & R. M. Joshi (Eds.), Developmental and acquired dyslexia (pp. 61–77). Dordrecht: Kluwer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Friedes, D. (1974). Human information processing and sensory modality: Cross-modal functions, information complexity, memory and deficit. Psychological Bulletin, 81, 284–310.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Frost, R. (2001). Unpublished word list in Hebrew. Jerusalem, Israel: Hebrew University.

    Google Scholar 

  • Galaburda, A. M., Sherman, G. F., Rosen, G. D., Aboitiz, F., & Geschwind, N. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: Four consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. Annals of Neurology, 18, 222–233.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gladstone, M., & Best, C. T. (1985). Developmental dyslexia: the potential role of interhemispheric collaboration in reading acquisition. Hemispheric Function and Collaboration in the Child. San Francisco, CA: Academic Press.

  • Gladstone, M., Best, C. T., & Davidson, R. J. (1989). Anomalous bimanual coordination among dyslexic boys. Developmental Psychology, 25, 236–246.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gottardo, A., Siegel, L. S., & Stanovich, K. E. (1997). The assessment of adults with reading disabilities: What can we learn from experimental tasks? Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 42–54.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gross-Glen, K., & Rothenberg, S. (1984). Evidence for deficit in interhemispheric transfer of information in dyslexic boys. International Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 23–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hackley, S. A., Woldorff, M., & Hillyard, S. A. (1990). Cross-modal selective attention effects on retinal, myogenic, brainstem, and cerebral evoked potentials. Psychophysiology, 27, 195–208.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Isreal, J. B., Chesney, G. L., Wickens, C. D., & Donchin, E. (1980). P300 and tracking difficulty: Evidence for multiple resources in dual-task performance. Psychophysiology, 17, 259–273.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10, 371–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leong, C. K. (1999). Phonological and morphological processing in adult students with learning/reading disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 32, 224–238.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W. (1992). The visual deficit hypothesis. In N. Singh & I. L. Beale (Eds.), Learning disabilities: Nature, theory and treatment (pp. 246–269). NewYork: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Luck, S. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1994). Electrophysiological correlates of feature analysis during visual search. Psychophysiology, 31, 291–308.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Markee, T., Warren, S. B., Morre, L. H., & Theberge, D. C. (1996). Callosal function in dyslexia: Evoked potential interhemispheric transfer time and bilateral field advantage. Developmental Neuropsychology, 12, 409–428.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Marsolek, C. J., Kosslyn, S. M., & Squire, L. R. (1992). Form-specific visual priming in the right cerebral hemisphere. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 18, 492–508.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Miller-Shaul, S., & Breznitz, Z. (2004). Electrocortical measures during a lexical decision task: A comparison between elementary school-aged children, normal and dyslexic readers and adult, normal and dyslexics readers. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 165, 399–424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Center for Evaluation. (2000). Comprehension test, Israeli psychometric scholastic aptitude test. Tel Aviv, Israel.

  • Neville, H. J., Coffey, S. A., Holcomb, P. J., & Tallal, P. (1993). The neurobiology of sensory and language processing in language-impaired children. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 235–253.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nicolson, R. I., & Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Reaction times and dyslexia. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology - Human Experimental Psychology, 47A, 29–48.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Orgil Medical Equipment. (1997). Analysis software user’s manual for the brain performance measurement system. Ein Ayalla, Israel: Orgil.

    Google Scholar 

  • Orton, S. T. (1937). Reading, writing and speech problems in children. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Osterhout, L., & Hagoort, P. (1999). A superficial resemblance does not necessarily mean you are part of the family: Counterarguments to Coulson, King and Kutas (1998) in the P600/SPS-P300 debate. Language and Cognitive Processes, 14, 1–14.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, E. D., Brown, T. T., Petersen, S. V., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2004). Investigation of the functional neuroanatomy of single word reading and its development. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 203–223.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Palmer, B., Nasman, V. T., & Wilson, G. F. (1994). Task detection difficulty: Effects on ERPs in a same-different letter classification task. Biological Psychology, 38, 199–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1994). Psycholinguistics and reading ability. In M. A. Gernsbacher (Ed.), Handbook of psycholinguistics (pp. 849–885). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., & Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: Computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychological Review, 103, 56–115.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pugh, K. R., Mencl, W. E., Jenner, A. R., Katz, L., Frost, S. J., Lee, J. R., et al. (2001). Neurobiological studies of reading and reading disability. Journal of Communication Disorders, 34, 479–492.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ransby, M. J., & Swanson, L. H. (2003). Reading comprehension skills of young adults with childhood diagnoses of dyslexia. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 36, 538–555.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Raven, J. C. (1965). Advanced progressive matrices sets I and II. London: H. K. Lewis.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rayner, K., Foorman, B. R., Perfetti, C. A., Pesetsky, D., & Seidenberg, M. S. (2001). How psychological science informs the teaching of reading. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 2, 31–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Regan, D. (1989). Human brain electrophysiology: Evoked potentials and evoked magnetic fields in science and medicine. New York: Elsevier.

    Google Scholar 

  • Robeck, M. C., & Wallace, R. R. (1990). The psychology of reading: An interdisciplinary approach (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Russeler, J., Probst, S., Johannes, S., & Munte, T. F. (2003). Recognition memory for high- and low-frequency words in adult normal and dyslexic readers: An event-related brain potential study. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 25, 815–829.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sandak, S., Mencl, W. E., Frost, S. J., & Pugh, K. R. (2004). The neurobiological basis of skilled and impaired reading: Recent findings and new directions. Scientific Studies of Reading, 8, 273–292.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sarkari, S., Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Castillo, E. M., Breier, J. I., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2002). The emergence and treatment of developmental reading disability: Contributions of functional brain imaging. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology, 9, 227–236.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S. (1990). Dyslexia in a computational model of word recognition in reading. In P. B. Gough, L. C. Ehri, & R. Trieman (Eds.), Reading acquisition (pp. 243–273). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seidenberg, M. S., & McClleland, J. L. (1989). A distributed developmental model of word recognition and naming. Psychological Review, 96, 523–568.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Share, D. L. (1994). Deficient phonological processing in disabled readers implicates processing deficits beyond the phonological module. In K. P. van den Bos, L. S. Siegal, D. J. Bakker, & D. L. Share (Eds.), Current directions in dyslexia research. Lisse, Netherlands: Swets & Zeitlinger B. V.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shatil, E. (1997). One-minute test for words and pseudowords. Unpublished test. University of Haifa, Israel.

  • Shaywitz, S. E. (1996). Dyslexia. Scientific American, 275, 98–140.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Fletcher, J. M., Holahan, J. M., Shneider, A. E., Marchione, K. E., Stueberg, K. K., et al. (1999). Persistence of dyslexia: The Connecticut longitudinal study at adolescence. Pediatrics, 104, 1351–1359.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, B. A., Lyon, G. R., & Shaywitz, S. E. (2006). The role of functional magnetic resonance imaging in understanding reading and dyslexia. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30(1), 613–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shaywitz, S. E., Shaywitz, B. A., Pugh, K. R., Fulbright, R. K., Constable, R. T., Mencl, W. E., et al. (1998). Functional disruption in organization of the brain for reading in dyslexia. Neurobiology, 95, 2636–2641.

    Google Scholar 

  • Siegel, L. S., Share, D., & Geva, E. (1995). Evidance for superior orthographic skills in dyslexics. American Psychological Socity, 6, 250–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2000a). Cerebral mechanisms involved in word reading in dyslexic children: A magnetic source imaging approach. Cerebral Cortex, 10, 809–816.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Bergman, E., Fishbeck, K., et al. (2000b). Brain activation profiles in dyslexic children during nonword reading: A magnetic source imaging study. Neuroscience Letters, 290, 61–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Castillo, E. M., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2002a). Brain mechanisms for reading words and pseudowords: An integrated approach. Cerebral Cortex, 12, 297–305.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Fletcher, J. M., Foorman, B. R., Mouzaki, A., & Papanicolaou, A. C. (2001). Age-related changes in regional brain activation during phonological decoding and printed word recognition. Developmental Neuropsychology, 19, 191–210.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Breier, J. I., Wheless, J. W., Maggio, W. W., Fletcher, J. M., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2000c). Brain mechanisms for reading: The role of the superior temporal gyrus in word and pseudoword naming. Neuroreport, 11, 2443–2447.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Bergman, E., Breier, J. I., Foorman, B. R., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2002b). Dyslexia-specific brain activation profile becomes normal following successful remedial training. Neurology, 58, 1203–1213.

    Google Scholar 

  • Simos, P. G., Fletcher, J. M., Sarkari, S., Billingsley, R. L., Francis, D. J., Castillo, E. M., et al. (2005). Early development of neurophysiological processes involved in normal reading and reading disability: A magnetic source imaging study. Neuropsychology, 19, 787–798.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M. J., & Nation, K. A. (1997). Language, phonology, and learning to read. In C. Hulme & M. Snowling (Eds.), Dyslexia: Biology, cognition and intervention (pp. 153–166). London: Whurr.

    Google Scholar 

  • Snowling, M., Nation, K., Moxham, P., Gallagher, A., & Frith, U. (1997). Phonological processing skills of dyslexic students in higher education: A preliminary report. Journal of Research in Reading, 20, 31–41.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sousa, A. D. (2000). How the brain learns. Thousand Oaks, CA: Crowin.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spironelli, C., & Angrilli, A. (2006). Language lateralization in phonological, semantic and orthographic tasks: A slow evoked potential study. Behavioural Brain Research, 175, 296–304.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Steinmetz, H. (1996). Structure, function and cerebral asymmetry: In vivo morphometry of the planum temporale. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 20, 587–591.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Treiman, R. (2000). The foundations of literacy. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 89–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Van Herten, M., Kolk, H. J., & Chwilla, D. J. (2005). An ERP study of P600 effects elicited by semantic anomalies. Cognitive Brain Research, 22, 241–255.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Velay, J. L., Daffaure, V., Giraud, K. M., & Habib, M. (2002). Interhemispheric sensorimotor integration in pointing movements: A study on dyslexic adults. Neuropsychologia, 40, 827–834.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R., Fletcher, J. M., Snowling, M. J., & Scanlon, D. M. (2004). Specific reading disability (dyslexia): what have we learned in the past four decades? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 2–40.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K., & Barker, T. A. (1994). The development of orthographic processing ability. Neuropsychology and Cognition, 8, 243–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wilson, G. F., Swain, C. R., & Ullsperger, P. (1998). ERP components elicited in response to warning stimuli: The influence of task difficulty. Biological Psychology, 47, 137–158.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Shelley Shaul.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Shaul, S. Lexical decision with left, right and center visual field presentation: a comparison between dyslexic and regular readers by means of electrophysiological and behavioral measures. Read Writ 25, 1143–1170 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9311-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11145-011-9311-1

Keywords

Navigation