Skip to main content
Log in

Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia

  • Published:
Reading and Writing Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

In this paper, we review several lines of convergent research to discuss the relationship between developmental dyslexia and slow symbol naming speed. We describe the interactive development of orthographic and phonological codes, and we discuss the methodological problems that may have led to underestimating the importance of individual differences in orthographic processing in our account of reading disabilities. Symbol naming speed is typically subsumed under phonological processing, yet it contributes variance to reading, especially to reading fluency, independently of phonological awareness. We speculate that naming speed may reflect precise timing mechanisms necessary to the development of orthographic codes and to their integration with phonological codes. We argue that an understanding of this precise timing dimension is necessary to incorporate in our models of phonological, orthographic, and semantic processes in reading acquisition and reading failure.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, M. J. (1990).Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about Print. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W. (1986). Word recognition in early reading: A review of the direct and indirect access hypothesis.Cognition 24: 93–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barron, R. W. (1991). Proto-literacy, literacy and the acquisition of phonological awareness.Learning and Individual Differences 31: 243–255.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, I. L. & Carpenter, P. A. (1986). Cognitive approaches to understanding reading: Implications for instructional practice.American Psychologist 41: 1098–1105.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W. (1987). Global, component, and serial processing of printed words in beginning reading.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 43: 387–418.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W. (1990). Multiple orthographic codes: Key to alternative instructional methodologies for developing orthographic-phonological connections underlying word identification.School Psychology Review 19: 518–533.

    Google Scholar 

  • Berninger, V. W., Yates, C. & Lester, K. (1991). Multiple orthographic codes in reading and writing acquisition.Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 3: 115–149.

    Google Scholar 

  • Biemiller, A. (1977–1978). Relationships between oral reading rates for letters, words, and simple text in the development of reading achievement.Reading Research Quarterly 13: 223–253.

    Google Scholar 

  • Blachman, B. A. (1984). Relationship of rapid naming ability and language analysis skills to kindergarten and first-grade reading achievement.Journal of Educational Psychology 76: 610–622.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. (1991). Implications for reading skills of developing abilities to automatize name retrieval under varied conditions. Paper presented at a meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Seattle, Washington.

  • Bowers, P. G. (in press). Text reading and rereading: Determinants of fluency beyond word recognition.Journal of Reading Behavior.

  • Bowers, P. G. (1992b). Tracing the impact on reading disability of several component process deficits. Paper submitted for publication.

  • Bowers, P. G., Steffy, R. A. & Tate, E. (1988). Comparison of the effects of IQ control methods on memory and naming speed predictors of reading disability.Reading Research Quarterly 23: 304–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. & Swanson, L. B. (1991). Naming speed deficits in reading disability: Multiple measures of a singular process.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 51: 195–219.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowers, P. G. & Wolf, M. (in prep.) A double-deficit hypothesis for developmental reading disorders.

  • BBowey, J. A. & Francis, J. (1991). Phonological analysis as a function of age and exposure to reading instruction.Applied Psycholinguistics 12: 91–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, L. & Bryant, P. (1985).Rhyme and reason in reading and spelling. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Breitmeyer, B. G. (1980). Unmasking visual masking: A look at the ‘why’ behind the veil of ‘how’.Psychological Review 87: 52–69.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chall, J. (1967).Learning to read: The great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Cunningham, A. E. & Stanovich, K. E. (1990). Assessing print exposure and orthographic processing skill in children: A quick measure of reading experience.Journal of Educational Psychology 82: 733–740.

    Google Scholar 

  • Denckla, M. & Rudel, R. G. (1976). Rapid ‘automatized’ naming (R.A.N.): Dyslexia differentiated from other learning disabilities.Neuropsychologia 14: 471–479.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1980). The role of orthographic images in learning printed words. In: J. F. Kavanagh & R. L. Venezky (eds),Orthography, reading, and dyslexia (pp. 155–170). Baltimore: University Park Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. (1989). The development of spelling knowledge and its role in reading acquisition and reading disability.Journal of Learning Disabilities 22: 356–365.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. & Wilce, L. S. (1983). Development of word identification speed in skilled and less skilled beginning readers.Journal of Educational Psychology 75: 3–18.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ehri, L. C. & Wilce, L. S. (1987). Does learning to spell help beginners learn to read words?Reading Research Quarterly 22: 47–65.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. (1981). Visual and name coding in dyslexic children.Psychological Research 43: 219–234.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ellis, N. C. & Miles, T. R. (1981). A lexical encoding deficiency, I: Experimental evidence. In: G. Th. Pavlidis & T. R. Mile (eds),Dyslexia research and its applications (pp. 177–215). John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • Felton, R. & Brown, I. S. (1990). Phonological processes as predictors of specific reading skills in children at risk for reading failure.Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal 2: 39–59.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., Naylor, C. E. & Wood, F. B. (1990). Neuropsychological profile of adult dyslexics.Brain and Language 39: 485–497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Felton, R. H., Wood, F. B., Brown, I. S. & Campbell, S. K. (1987). Separate verbal memory and naming deficits in attention deficit disorder and reading disability.Brain and Language 31: 171–184.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goswami, U. (1991). Learning about spelling sequences: The role of onsets and rimes in analogies in reading.Child Development 62: 1110–1123.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gough, P. B. & Tunmer, W. E. (1986). Decoding, reading, and reading disability.Remedial and Special Education 7: 6–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hulme, C. (1988). The implausibility of low-level visual deficits as a cause of children's reading difficulties.Cognitive Neuropsychology 5: 369–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, D. & Myklebust, A. (1964).Learning disabilities. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kennedy, A. & Bowers, P. G. (1991). The effect of practice on word identification speed in children. Poster presented to a meeting of the American Psychological Association, San Francisco.

  • LaBerge, D. (1979). The perception of units in beginning reading. In: L. B. Resnick & P. A. Weaver (eds),Theory and practice of early reading, Vol. 3 (pp. 31–51). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Levy, B. A. & Hinchley, J. (1990). Individual and developmental differences in the acquisition of reading skills. In: T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (eds),Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 81–128). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Livingstone, M., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W. & Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular deficit in developmental dyslexia.Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, USA 88: 7943–7947.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W. J. (1991). Is the question of the role of visual deficits as a cause of reading disabilities a closed one? Comments on Hulme.Cognitive Neuropsychology 8: 435–441.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovegrove, W. (in press). Mechanisms underlying saccadic suppression in specifically disabled and normal readers. In: E. Chakaluk (ed),The role of eye movements in perceptual processes. Amsterdam: North-Holland.

  • Lovegrove, W., Martin, F. & Slaghuis, W. (1986). A theoretical and experimental case for a visual deficit in specific reading disability.Cognitive Neuropsychology 3: 225–267.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lovett, M. W. (1987). A developmental approach to reading disability: Accuracy and speed criteria of normal and deficient reading skill.Child Development 58: 234–260.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R. (1985). Acquisition of word identification skills in normal and disabled readers.Journal of Educational Psychology 77: 78–90.

    Google Scholar 

  • Manis, F. R., Szeszulski, P. A., Holt, L. K. & Graves, L. (1990). Variation in component word recognition and spelling skills among dyslexic children and normal readers. In: T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (eds),Reading and its development: Components skills approaches (pp. 207–259). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mann, V. (1984). Review: Reading skill and language skill.Developmental Review 4: 1–15.

    Google Scholar 

  • Massaro, D. W. & Hestand, J. (1983). Developmental relations between reading ability and knowledge of orthographic structure.Contemporary Educational Psychology 8: 174–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, L. A., Pollatsek, A. & Well, A. D. (1988). Developmental dyslexia and word retrieval deficits.Brain and Language 35: 11–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ojemann, G. A. (1983). Brain organization for language from the perspective of electrical stimulation mapping.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2: 189–230.

    Google Scholar 

  • Olson, R., Wise, B., Conners, F. & Rack, J. (1990). Organization, heritability, and remediation of component word recognition and language skills in disabled readers. In: T. H. Carr & B. A. Levy (eds),Reading and its development: Component skills approaches (pp. 261–322). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A. (1985).Reading Ability. New York: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perfetti, C. A., Beck, I., Bell, L. C. & Hughes, C. (1987). Phonemic knowledge and learning to read are reciprocal: A longitudinal study of first grade children.Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 33: 283–319.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rack, J. P., Snowling, M. J. & Olson, R. K. (1992). The nonword reading deficit in developmental dyslexia.Reading Research Quarterly 27: 28–53.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1983). Printed word learning in beginning readers.Journal of Experimental Child Psychology 36: 321–339.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reitsma, P. (1989). Orthographic memory and learning to read. In: P. G. Aaron & R. M. Joshi (eds),Reading and writing disorders in different orthographic systems (pp. 51–73). Dordrecht/Boston London: Kluwer Academic Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roswell, F. & Natchez, G. (1971).Reading disability. New York: Basic Books.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roth, S. F. & Beck, I. L. (1987). Theoretical and instructional implications of the assessment of two microcomputer word recognition programs.Reading Research Quarterly 22: 197–218.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C. & Capps, C. (1974). Encoding speed, rehearsal, and probed recall of dyslexic boys.Journal of Educational Psychology 66: 780–786.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C. & Davis, J. M. (1988). Relations of digit naming speed with three components of reading.Applied Psycholinguistics 9: 315–334.

    Google Scholar 

  • Spring, C. & Perry, L. (1983). Naming speed and serial recall in poor and adequate readers.Contemporary Educational Psychology 8: 141–145.

    Google Scholar 

  • Stanovich, K. E. (1988). The dyslexic and the garden-variety poor reader: The phonologicalcore variable-difference model.Journal of Learning Disabilities 21: 590–604.

    Google Scholar 

  • Swanson, L. B. (1989). Analyzing naming speed-reading relationships in children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Waterloo, Ontario (Canada): University of Waterloo.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1980). Auditory temporal perception, phonics, and reading disabilities in children.Brain and Language 9: 182–198.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tallal, P. (1983). A precise timing mechanism may underlie a common speech perception and production area in the peri-Sylvian cortex of the dominant hemisphere: Commentary on G. A. Ojemann (1983) target article.The Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6: 219–220.

    Google Scholar 

  • Torgesen, J., Wagner, R., Simmons, K. & Laughon, P. (1990). Identifying phonological coding problems in disabled readers: Naming, counting, or span measures?Learning Disabilities Quarterly 13: 236–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tzeng, O. J. L. & Wang, W. S. (1984). Search for a common neurocognitive mechanism for language and movements.American Journal of Physiology 246: 904–911.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vellutino, F. R. (1979).Dyslexia: Theory and research. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Venezky, R. & Massaro, D. (1979). The role of orthographic regularity in word recognition. In: L. Resnick & P. Weaver (eds),Theory and practice of reading, Vol. 1 (pp. 85–108). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wagner, R. K. & Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills.Psychological Bulletin 101: 192–212.

    Google Scholar 

  • Walsh, D. J., Price, G. & Gillingham, M. G. (1988). The critical but transitory importance of letter naming.Reading Research Quarterly 23: 108–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wimmer, H. (in press). Characteristics of developmental dyslexia in a regular writing system.Applied Psycholinguistics.

  • Wolf, M. (1982). The word-retrieval process and reading in children and aphasics. In: K. Nelson (ed),Children's language, Vol. 3 (pp. 437–493). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. (1991). Naming speed and reading: The contribution of the cognitive neurosciences.Reading Research Quarterly 26: 123–141.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M., Bally, H. & Morris, R. (1986). Automaticity, retrieval processes and reading: A longitudinal study in average and impaired readers.Child Development 57: 988–1005.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. & Goodglass, H. (1986). Dyslexia, dysnomia, and lexical retrieval.Brain and Language 27: 360–379.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, M. & Obregon, M. (1992). Early naming deficits, developmental dyslexia, and a specific deficit hypothesis.Brain and Language 42: 219–247.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F., Ovrut, M. & Drake, C. (1990a). Rate and timing precision of motor coordination in developmental dyslexia.Developmental Psychology 26: 349–359.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. & Ovrut, M. (1990b). Rate variables and automatized naming in developmental dyslexia.Brain and Language 39: 556–575.

    Google Scholar 

  • Wolff, P. H., Michel, G. F. & Ovrut, M. (1990c). The timing of syllable repetitions in developmental dyslexia.Journal of Speech and Hearing Research 33: 281–289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Yap, R. & van der Leij, A. (1991). Word processing in dyslexics: A phonological and a speed deficit? Paper submitted for publication.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bowers, P.G., Wolf, M. Theoretical links among naming speed, precise timing mechanisms and orthographic skill in dyslexia. Read Writ 5, 69–85 (1993). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026919

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF01026919

Key words

Navigation