Skip to main content
Log in

The Time-Course for the Capture and Hold of Visuospatial Attention by Fearful and Happy Faces

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Fearful faces both capture our attention and hold it. However, little is known about the time-course for the capture and hold of spatial attention by fearful, and other emotional, faces. In three dot-probe studies we examined this time-course. Experiments 1 and 2 used fearful and neutral faces. In Experiment 1, dot-probe targets occurred 133, 266, and 532 ms post-face onset and in Experiment 2, dot-probe targets occurred 84, 168, 336, and 672 ms post-face onset. In Experiment 1, for both 133 and 266 ms conditions, reaction times were fastest for congruent trials and slowest for incongruent trials with reaction times for baseline trials falling between the two. The same pattern was found for the 84 and 168 ms conditions in Experiment 2. For the later time-points in both experiments there were no significant differences between conditions. To examine whether this time-course is unique to fearful faces, a third dot-probe experiment using time-points identical to Experiment 2 was conducted with happy and neutral faces. The results from Experiment 3 suggest that happy faces captured and held attention in the 168 and 336 ms conditions. Together, the results indicate that attention is captured and held by fearful faces at times earlier than approximately 300 ms, while happy faces also captured and held attention, but on a slightly different time-course from 168 to 336 ms.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Faces were presented for the same duration (i.e., 133 ms) in all SOA conditions in order to hold the amount of face processing constant across conditions.

  2. Although this may at first glance appear to be a case of circular analysis, it is not circular. A circular analysis is one in which data are reduced based on meeting a particular criterion and then this criterion is tested in the reduced dataset. Here, data are being reduced based on the difference between two conditions (A and B; i.e., congruent and incongruent trials) and are then used in independent comparisons to a third condition (C; baseline trials) and not each other. The difference between A and B (e.g., if A = 350 and B = 400, A < B) has no bearing on whether or not C is different from A or B (e.g., if C = 325, C < A & B; if C = 375, C > A, but < B; or C = 425, C > A & B).

References

  • Adams, R. B., Jr., & Kleck, R. E. (2005). Effects of direct and averted gaze on the perception of facially communicated emotion. Emotion, 5(1), 3–11. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 817–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arndt, J. E., & Fujiwara, E. (2012). Attentional bias towards angry faces in trait-reappraisal. Personality and Individual Differences, 52(1), 61–66. doi:10.1016/J.Paid.2011.08.030.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beaver, J. D., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli and modulation of visuospatial attention. Emotion, 5(1), 67–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beevers, C. G., Gibb, B. E., McGeary, J. E., & Miller, I. W. (2007). Serotonin transporter genetic variation and biased attention for emotional word stimuli among psychiatric inpatients. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 116(1), 208–212. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.116.1.208.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I. (2006). Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: How specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 59(8), 1484–1504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bradley, B., Field, M., Mogg, K., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Attentional and evaluative biases for smoking cues in nicotine dependence: Component processes of biases in visual orienting. Behavioral Pharmacology, 15(1), 29–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bryant, R. A., & Harvey, A. G. (1997). Attentional bias in posttraumatic stress disorder. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 10(4), 635–644.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M. (2016). Facilitated orienting underlies fearful face-enhanced gaze cueing of spatial location. Cogent Psychology, 3(1), 1147120. doi:10.1080/23311908.2016.1147120.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Fee, A. L., & Reinke, K. S. (2009). Backward masked snakes and guns modulate spatial attention. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 527–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2015). Facilitated attentional orienting and delayed disengagement to conscious and nonconscious fearful faces. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39(1), 69–77. doi:10.1007/s10919-014-0185-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Mujica-Parodi, L. R., Harmon-Jones, E., & Hajcak, G. (2012). The orienting of spatial attention to backward masked fearful faces is associated with variation in the serotonin transporter gene. Emotion, 12(2), 203–207.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2008). Masked fearful faces modulate the orienting of covert spatial attention. Emotion, 8(4), 522–529. doi:10.1037/a0012653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2010). Spatial attention-related modulation of the N170 by backward masked fearful faces. Brain Cognition, 73(1), 20–27. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.01.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2014). Attending to the fear in your eyes: Facilitated orienting and delayed disengagement. Cognition and Emotion, 28(8), 1398–1406. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.885410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Reinke, K. S., LaMontagne, P. J., & Habib, R. (2011). Backward masked fearful faces enhance contralateral occipital cortical activity for visual targets within the spotlight of attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 639–645. doi:10.1093/Scan/Nsq076.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Torrence, R. D., & Vander Hyde, M. R. (2016). Beware the eyes behind the mask: The capture and hold of selective attention by backward masked fearful eyes. Motivation and Emotion, 40(3), 498–505. doi:10.1007/s11031-016-9542-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Clarke, P. J., Macleod, C., & Guastella, A. J. (2013). Assessing the role of spatial engagement and disengagement of attention in anxiety-linked attentional bias: A critique of current paradigms and suggestions for future research directions. Anxiety Stress Coping, 26(1), 1–19. doi:10.1080/10615806.2011.638054.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. M., & Langton, S. R. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1321–1329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Elam, K. K., Carlson, J. M., DiLalla, L. F., & Reinke, K. S. (2010). Emotional faces capture spatial attention in 5-year-old children. Evolutionary Psychology, 8(4), 754–767.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fichtenholtz, H. M., Hopfinger, J. B., Graham, R., Detwiler, J. M., & LaBar, K. S. (2007). Happy and fearful emotion in cues and targets modulate event-related potential indices of gaze-directed attentional orienting. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2(4), 323–333. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm026.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Field, M., Mogg, K., Zetteler, J., & Bradley, B. P. (2004). Attentional biases for alcohol cues in heavy and light social drinkers: The roles of initial orienting and maintained attention. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 176(1), 88–93. doi:10.1007/s00213-004-1855-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: the role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, and Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Ridgewell, A., & Ashwin, C. (2009). Looking on the bright side: Biased attention and the human serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Science, 276(1663), 1747–1751. doi:10.1098/rspb.2008.1788.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Russo, R., Bowles, R., & Dutton, K. (2001). Do threatening stimuli draw or hold visual attention in subclinical anxiety? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 681–700.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., et al. (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 115(2), 137–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hakamata, Y., Lissek, S., Bar-Haim, Y., Britton, J. C., Fox, N. A., Leibenluft, E., et al. (2010). Attention bias modification treatment: A meta-analysis toward the establishment of novel treatment for anxiety. Biological Psychiatry, 68(11), 982–990. doi:10.1016/J.Biopsych.2010.07.021.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183–1192.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., Van Damme, S., & Wiersema, J. R. (2006). Components of attentional bias to threat in high trait anxiety: Facilitated engagement, impaired disengagement, and attentional avoidance. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 44(12), 1757–1771.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Verschuere, B., Crombez, G., & Van Damme, S. (2005). Time-course of attention for threatening pictures in high and low trait anxiety. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 43(8), 1087–1098. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.08.004.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kreta, M. E., Jaasmab, L., Biondac, T., & Wijnend, J. G. (2016). Bonobos (Pan paniscus) show an attentional bias toward conspecifics’ emotions. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. doi:10.1073/pnas.1522060113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kuckertz, J. M., & Amir, N. (2015). Attention bias modification for anxiety and phobias: Current status and future directions. Current Psychiatry Reports, 17(2), 9. doi:10.1007/s11920-014-0545-x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, G., & Tipples, J. (2011). Increased gaze following for fearful faces. It depends on what you’re looking for! Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 18(1), 89–95. doi:10.3758/s13423-010-0033-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MacLeod, C., & Mathews, A. (1988). Anxiety and the allocation of attention to threat. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology A, 40(4), 653–670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Macleod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95(1), 15–20.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & Mackintosh, B. (1998). A cognitive model of selective processing in anxiety. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 22(6), 539–560.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mathews, A., & MacLeod, C. (2002). Induced processing biases have causal effects on anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 16(3), 331–354.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Milders, M., Hietanen, J. K., Leppanen, J. M., & Braun, M. (2011). Detection of emotional faces is modulated by the direction of eye gaze. Emotion, 11(6), 1456–1461. doi:10.1037/a0022901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2002). Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 40(12), 1403–1414.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2006). Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1241–1250. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2006.05.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., de Bono, J., & Painter, M. (1997). Time course of attentional bias for threat information in non-clinical anxiety. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 35(4), 297–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., & Williams, R. (1995). Attentional bias in anxiety and depression: The role of awareness. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34(Pt 1), 17–36.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., Holmes, A., Garner, M., & Bradley, B. P. (2008). Effects of threat cues on attentional shifting, disengagement and response slowing in anxious individuals. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 46(5), 656–667. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.011.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Naatanen, R. (1970). The diminishing time-uncertainty with the lapse of time after the warning signal in reaction-time experiments with varying fore-periods. Acta Psychologica (Amst), 34(4), 399–419.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Osinsky, R., Reuter, M., Kupper, Y., Schmitz, A., Kozyra, E., Alexander, N., et al. (2008). Variation in the serotonin transporter gene modulates selective attention to threat. Emotion, 8(4), 584–588. doi:10.1037/a0012826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pecchinenda, A., Pes, M., Ferlazzo, F., & Zoccolotti, P. (2008). The combined effect of gaze direction and facial expression on cueing spatial attention. Emotion, 8(5), 628–634. doi:10.1037/a0013437.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Perez-Edgar, K., Bar-Haim, Y., McDermott, J. M., Gorodetsky, E., Hodgkinson, C. A., Goldman, D., et al. (2010). Variations in the serotonin-transporter gene are associated with attention bias patterns to positive and negative emotion faces. Biological Psychology, 83(3), 269–271. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Inhoff, A. W., Friedrich, F. J., & Cohen, A. (1987). Isolating attentional systems—A cognitive-anatomical analysis. Psychobiology, 15(2), 107–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619–633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Putman, P., Hermans, E., & van Honk, J. (2006). Anxiety meets fear in perception of dynamic expressive gaze. Emotion, 6(1), 94–102. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.6.1.94.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rudaizky, D., Basanovic, J., & MacLeod, C. (2014). Biased attentional engagement with, and disengagement from, negative information: Independent cognitive pathways to anxiety vulnerability? Cognition and Emotion, 28(2), 245–259. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.815154.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Salemink, E., van den Hout, M. A., & Kindt, M. (2007). Selective attention and threat: Quick orienting versus slow disengagement and two versions of the dot probe task. Behavioural Research and Therapy, 45(3), 607–615.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Schmidta, L. J., Belopolskya, A. V., & Theeuwesa, J. (2016). The time course of attentional bias to cues of threat and safety. Cognition and Emotion. doi:10.1080/02699931.2016.1169998.

    Google Scholar 

  • van Honk, J., Tuiten, A., de Haan, E., van den Hout, M., & Stam, H. (2001). Attentional biases for angry faces: Relationships to trait anger and anxiety. Cognition and Emotion, 15(3), 279–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Yiend, J., & Mathews, A. (2001). Anxiety and attention to threatening pictures. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology, 54(3), 665–681.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the students in the Cognitive × Affective Behavior and Integrative Neuroscience (CABIN) lab at Northern Michigan University for assisting in the collection of this data.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joshua M. Carlson.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Torrence, R.D., Wylie, E. & Carlson, J.M. The Time-Course for the Capture and Hold of Visuospatial Attention by Fearful and Happy Faces. J Nonverbal Behav 41, 139–153 (2017). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0247-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0247-7

Keywords

Navigation