Skip to main content
Log in

Beware the eyes behind the mask: The capture and hold of selective attention by backward masked fearful eyes

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Motivation and Emotion Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Fearful facial expressions are important social indicators of environmental threat. Among the various features of a fearful face, the eyes appear to be particularly important for recognizing and responding to these social cues. One way in which fearful faces facilitate observers’ behavior is by automatically capturing attention. This is true for both consciously and nonconsciously processed fearful faces. Recent research suggests that consciously processed fearful eyes alone are sufficient to capture observers’ attention. However, it is unknown as to whether or not nonconsciously processed, backward masked, fearful eyes are sufficient to facilitate spatial attention. To test this possibility, two dot-probe experiments with masked fearful eye stimuli were performed. In Experiment 1, we found that, relative to scrambled eyes, masked fearful eyes facilitate attentional orienting and delay attentional disengagement. In Experiment 2, we replicated this effect when comparing backward masked fearful to neutral eyes. Thus, the data suggest that nonconscious fearful eyes facilitate spatial attention through facilitated orienting and delayed disengagement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adolphs, R., Tranel, D., Hamann, S., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., Phelps, E. A., & Damasio, A. R. (1999). Recognition of facial emotion in nine individuals with bilateral amygdala damage. Neuropsychologia, 37(10), 1111–1117.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Adolphs, R., Gosselin, F., Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., Schyns, P., & Damasio, A. R. (2005). A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage. Nature, 433(7021), 68–72.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Akiyama, T., Kato, M., Muramatsu, T., Umeda, S., Saito, F., & Kashima, H. (2007). Unilateral amygdala lesions hamper attentional orienting triggered by gaze direction. [Research Support, Non-U.S. Gov’t]. Cerebral Cortex, 17(11), 2593–2600. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl166.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson, A. K., & Phelps, E. A. (2001). Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature, 411(6835), 305–309.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armony, J. L., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: An event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 40(7), 817–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Armony, J. L., Corbo, V., Clement, M. H., & Brunet, A. (2005). Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(10), 1961–1963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ashwin, C., Baron-Cohen, S., Wheelwright, S., O’Riordan, M., & Bullmore, E. T. (2007). Differential activation of the amygdala and the ‘social brain’ during fearful face-processing in Asperger Syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 2–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.014.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bach, D. R., Hurlemann, R., & Dolan, R. J. (2014). Impaired threat prioritisation after selective bilateral amygdala lesions. Cortex, 63C, 206–213. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2014.08.017.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beaver, J. D., Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2005). Emotional conditioning to masked stimuli and modulation of visuospatial attention. Emotion, 5(1), 67–79.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Blanchette, I. (2006). Snakes, spiders, guns, and syringes: How specific are evolutionary constraints on the detection of threatening stimuli? The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology (Colchester), 59(8), 1484–1504.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Breiter, H. C., Etcoff, N. L., Whalen, P. J., Kennedy, W. A., Rauch, S. L., Buckner, R. L., & Rosen, B. R. (1996). Response and habituation of the human amygdala during visual processing of facial expression. Neuron, 17(5), 875–887.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2015). Facilitated attentional orienting and delayed disengagement to conscious and nonconscious fearful faces. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 39(1), 69–77. doi:10.1007/s10919-014-0185-1.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2008). Masked fearful faces modulate the orienting of covert spatial attention. Emotion, 8(4), 522–529. doi:10.1037/a0012653.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2010). Spatial attention-related modulation of the N170 by backward masked fearful faces. Brain and Cognition, 73(1), 20–27. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2010.01.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., & Reinke, K. S. (2014). Attending to the fear in your eyes: Facilitated orienting and delayed disengagement. Cognition and Emotion, 28(8), 1398–1406. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.885410.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Fee, A. L., & Reinke, K. S. (2009a). Backward masked snakes and guns modulate spatial attention. Evolutionary Psychology, 7(4), 527–537.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Reinke, K. S., & Habib, R. (2009b). A left amygdala mediated network for rapid orienting to masked fearful faces. Neuropsychologia, 47(5), 1386–1389. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.01.026.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Greenberg, T., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2010). Blind rage? Heightened anger is associated with altered amygdala responses to masked and unmasked fearful faces. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 182, 281–283. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.02.001.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Beacher, F., Reinke, K. S., Habib, R., Harmon-Jones, E., Mujica-Parodi, L. R., & Hajcak, G. (2012). Nonconscious attention bias to threat is correlated with anterior cingulate cortex gray matter volume: A voxel-based morphometry result and replication. Neuroimage, 59(2), 1713–1718. doi:10.1016/J.Neuroimage..09.040.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Reinke, K. S., LaMontagne, P. J., & Habib, R. (2011). Backward masked fearful faces enhance contralateral occipital cortical activity for visual targets within the spotlight of attention. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(5), 639–645. doi:10.1093/Scan/Nsq076.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Cha, J., & Mujica-Parodi, L. R. (2013). Functional and structural amygdala—Anterior cingulate connectivity correlates with attentional bias to masked fearful faces. Cortex, 49(9), 2595–2600. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.07.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Carlson, J. M., Cha, J., Harmon-Jones, E., Mujica-Parodi, L. R., & Hajcak, G. (2014). Influence of the BDNF genotype on amygdalo-prefrontal white matter microstructure is linked to nonconscious attention bias to threat. Cerebral Cortex, 24(9), 2249–2257. doi:10.1093/cercor/bht089.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cooper, R. M., & Langton, S. R. (2006). Attentional bias to angry faces using the dot-probe task? It depends when you look for it. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44(9), 1321–1329.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., & Rhodes, T. (2008). Aggression in young children with concurrent callous-unemotional traits: Can the neurosciences inform progress and innovation in treatment approaches? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 363(1503), 2567–2576. doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0029.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Perry, Y., Hawes, D. J., Merz, S., Riddell, A. C., Haines, D. J., & Abeygunawardane, A. I. (2006). Attention to the eyes and fear-recognition deficits in child psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry, 189, 280–281. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.105.018150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., El Masry, Y., Wimalaweera, S., & Guastella, A. J. (2008). Reduced eye gaze explains “fear blindness” in childhood psychopathic traits. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 47(4), 455–463. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31816407f1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dadds, M. R., Jambrak, J., Pasalich, D., Hawes, D. J., & Brennan, J. (2011). Impaired attention to the eyes of attachment figures and the developmental origins of psychopathy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 52(3), 238–245. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02323.x.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: John Murray.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  • Etkin, A., Klemenhagen, K. C., Dudman, J. T., Rogan, M. T., Hen, R., Kandel, E. R., & Hirsch, J. (2004). Individual differences in trait anxiety predict the response of the basolateral amygdala to unconsciously processed fearful faces. Neuron, 44(6), 1043–1055. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2004.12.006.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E. (2002). Processing emotional facial expressions: The role of anxiety and awareness. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(1), 52–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fox, E., Mathews, A., Calder, A. J., & Yiend, J. (2007). Anxiety and sensitivity to gaze direction in emotionally expressive faces. Emotion, 7(3), 478–486. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.478.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Gamer, M., & Buchel, C. (2009). Amygdala activation predicts gaze toward fearful eyes. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(28), 9123–9126. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1883-09.2009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gur, R. C., Sara, R., Hagendoorn, M., Marom, O., Hughett, P., Macy, L., & Gur, R. E. (2002). A method for obtaining 3-dimensional facial expressions and its standardization for use in neurocognitive studies. Journal of Neuroscience Methods, 115(2), 137–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hardee, J. E., Thompson, J. C., & Puce, A. (2008). The left amygdala knows fear: Laterality in the amygdala response to fearful eyes. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 3(1), 47–54. doi:10.1093/scan/nsn001.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Koster, E. H., Crombez, G., Verschuere, B., & De Houwer, J. (2004). Selective attention to threat in the dot probe paradigm: Differentiating vigilance and difficulty to disengage. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42(10), 1183–1192.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lapate, R. C., Rokers, B., Li, T., & Davidson, R. J. (2014). Nonconscious emotional activation colors first impressions: A regulatory role for conscious awareness. Psychological Science, 25(2), 349–357. doi:10.1177/0956797613503175.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., & Stearns, J. J. (2007). The shape of threat: Simple geometric forms evoke rapid and sustained capture of attention. Emotion, 7(3), 526–534. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.526.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Larson, C. L., Aronoff, J., Sarinopoulos, I. C., & Zhu, D. C. (2009). Recognizing threat: A simple geometric shape activates neural circuitry for threat detection. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21(8), 1523–1535. doi:10.1162/jocn.2009.21111.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lee, D. H., Susskind, J. M., & Anderson, A. K. (2013). Social transmission of the sensory benefits of eye widening in fear expressions. Psychological Science, 24(6), 957–965. doi:10.1177/0956797612464500.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liddell, B. J., Brown, K. J., Kemp, A. H., Barton, M. J., Das, P., Peduto, A., & Williams, L. M. (2005). A direct brainstem-amygdala-cortical ‘alarm’ system for subliminal signals of fear. Neuroimage, 24(1), 235–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Marsh, A. A., & Blair, R. J. (2008). Deficits in facial affect recognition among antisocial populations: A meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32(3), 454–465. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.08.003.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Milders, M., Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., & Braun, M. (2011). Detection of emotional faces is modulated by the direction of eye gaze. Emotion, 11(6), 1456–1461. doi:10.1037/a0022901.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (1999). Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: A replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 37(6), 595–604.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mogg, K., & Bradley, B. P. (2002). Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(12), 1403–1414.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monk, C. S., Telzer, E. H., Mogg, K., Bradley, B. P., Mai, X., Louro, H. M., & Pine, D. S. (2008). Amygdala and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation to masked angry faces in children and adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(5), 568–576. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.5.568.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., Rowland, D., Young, A. W., Calder, A. J., & Dolan, R. J. (1996). A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature, 383(6603), 812–815.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J. S., Ohman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). A subcortical pathway to the right amygdala mediating “unseen” fear. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA, 96(4), 1680–1685.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J. S., DeGelder, B., Weiskrantz, L., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Differential extrageniculostriate and amygdala responses to presentation of emotional faces in a cortically blind field. Brain, 124(Pt 6), 1241–1252.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Morris, J. S., deBonis, M., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Human amygdala responses to fearful eyes. Neuroimage, 17(1), 214–222. doi:10.1006/nimg.2002.1220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cognition, and awareness: Affective priming with optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 723–739.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ohman, A., Flykt, A., & Esteves, F. (2001). Emotion drives attention: Detecting the snake in the grass. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(3), 466–478.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pessoa, L., & Adolphs, R. (2010). Emotion processing and the amygdala: From a ‘low road’ to ‘many roads’ of evaluating biological significance. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(11), 773–783. doi:10.1038/nrn2920.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I. (1980). Orienting of attention. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 32(1), 3–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., Inhoff, A. W., Friedrich, F. J., & Cohen, A. (1987). Isolating attentional systems—A cognitive-anatomical analysis. Psychobiology, 15(2), 107–121.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pourtois, G., Grandjean, D., Sander, D., & Vuilleumier, P. (2004). Electrophysiological correlates of rapid spatial orienting towards fearful faces. Cerebral Cortex, 14(6), 619–633.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Pourtois, G., Schettino, A., & Vuilleumier, P. (2013). Brain mechanisms for emotional influences on perception and attention: What is magic and what is not. Biological Psychology, 92(3), 492–512. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2012.02.007.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Price, R. B., Siegle, G. J., Silk, J. S., Ladouceur, C. D., McFarland, A., Dahl, R. E., & Ryan, N. D. (2014). Looking under the hood of the dot-probe task: An fMRI study in anxious youth. Depression and Anxiety, 31(3), 178–187. doi:10.1002/da.22255.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Rotteveel, M., de Groot, P., Geutskens, A., & Phaf, R. H. (2001). Stronger suboptimal than optimal affective priming? Emotion, 1(4), 348–364.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Spezio, M. L., Adolphs, R., Hurley, R. S., & Piven, J. (2007). Analysis of face gaze in autism using “Bubbles”. Neuropsychologia, 45(1), 144–151. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.027.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tamietto, M., & de Gelder, B. (2010). Neural bases of the non-conscious perception of emotional signals. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(10), 697–709. doi:10.1038/nrn2889.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2003). Distinct spatial frequency sensitivities for processing faces and emotional expressions. Nature Neuroscience, 6(6), 624–631.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Vuilleumier, P., Richardson, M. P., Armony, J. L., Driver, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nature Neuroscience, 7(11), 1271–1278. doi:10.1038/nn1341.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411–418.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Whalen, P. J., Kagan, J., Cook, R. G., Davis, F. C., Kim, H., Polis, S., & Johnstone, T. (2004). Human amygdala responsivity to masked fearful eye whites. Science, 306(5704), 2061.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wolf, R. C., Philippi, C. L., Motzkin, J. C., Baskaya, M. K., & Koenigs, M. (2014). Ventromedial prefrontal cortex mediates visual attention during facial emotion recognition. Brain, 137(Pt 6), 1772–1780. doi:10.1093/brain/awu063.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the students in the Cognitive × Affective Behavior and Integrative Neuroscience (CABIN) lab at Northern Michigan University, and in particular Scott Conger and Maggie Hopp, for assisting in the collection of this data.

Funding

This study was not funded by a grant.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joshua M. Carlson.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of interest

Joshua M. Carlson declares that he has no conflict of interest. Robert D. Torrence declares that he has no conflict of interest. Michelle R. Vander Hyde declares that she has no conflict of interest.

Ethical approval

All procedures performed in our studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional and/or national research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Carlson, J.M., Torrence, R.D. & Vander Hyde, M.R. Beware the eyes behind the mask: The capture and hold of selective attention by backward masked fearful eyes. Motiv Emot 40, 498–505 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9542-1

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11031-016-9542-1

Keywords

Navigation