Skip to main content
Log in

Spatial Contrast Sensitivity in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders

  • Original Paper
  • Published:
Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Adolescents with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) and typically developing (TD) controls underwent a rigorous psychophysical assessment that measured contrast sensitivity to seven spatial frequencies (0.5–20 cycles/degree). A contrast sensitivity function (CSF) was then fitted for each participant, from which four measures were obtained: visual acuity, peak spatial frequency, peak contrast sensitivity, and contrast sensitivity at a low spatial frequency. There were no group differences on any of the four CSF measures, indicating no differential spatial frequency processing in ASD. Although it has been suggested that detail-oriented visual perception in individuals with ASD may be a result of differential sensitivities to low versus high spatial frequencies, the current study finds no evidence to support this hypothesis.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Several addition analyses were conducted to ensure that a group difference, or a lack of group difference, was not due to other uncontrolled factors. (1) Because there were more girls in the TD group (11 out of 25) than the ASD group (0 out of 10), we made sure that there were no group differences between boys and girls in our TD sample. Accordingly, for all visual measures, gender differences were insignificant (all p values >0.31), and for this reason it is justified to use a mixture of boys and girls in the TD sample. (2) Because there were more participants in the TD group (25) than the ASD group (10), we conducted an additional analysis that equated the number of participants (10 in each group, using 10 TD participants well matched to the participants with ASD). The results using 10 TD participants were the same as using all 25. (3) Two ASD and two TD participants had some colour deficiencies, however, because colour vision is not relevant to the current study (which tested only light/dark visual sensitivity), and because analyses with their data included/excluded yielded no differences in results, their data were kept in our analyses. (4) Three ASD and eight TD participants required corrective lenses and wore them during the experimental sessions. The statistical results were also no different if their data were included/excluded, so their data were kept in our analyses.

  2. Note, however, that throughout the paper we use the general term ASD to refer to all participants with an external diagnosis of Autistic Disorder, Asperger’s syndrome or PDD-NOS.

  3. For three TD participants, Performance IQ scores were outlying data points (i.e. more than two standard deviations from the group mean). Data from these participants were removed from the correlational analyses.

  4. For one participant with ASD, the ADOS total score was an outlying data point (i.e. more than two standard deviations from the group mean). Data from this participant were removed from the correlational analyses.

References

  • Abrahams, B. S., & Geschwind, D. H. (2008). Advances in autism genetics: On the threshold of a new neurobiology. Nat Rev Genet, 9(5), 341–355.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • APA. (2004). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ashwin, E., Ashwin, C., Rhydderch, D., Howells, J., & Baron-Cohen, S. (2009). Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 17–21.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bach, M., & Dakin, S. (2009). Commentary on “Eagle-eyed visual acuity: An experimental investigation of enhanced perception in autism”. Biological Psychiatry, 66(10), e19–e20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Badcock, J. C., Whitworth, F. A., Badcock, D. R., & Lovegrove, W. J. (1990). Low-frequency filtering and the processing of local—global stimuli. Perception, 19(5), 617–629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Beglinger, L., & Smith, T. (2001). A review of subtyping in autism and proposed dimensional classification model. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(4), 411–422.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Avidan, G., Leonard, G. L., Kimchi, R., Luna, B., Humphreys, K., et al. (2006a). Configural processing in autism and its relationship to face processing. Neuropsychologia, 44(1), 110–129.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Behrmann, M., Thomas, C., & Humphreys, K. (2006b). Seeing it differently: Visual processing in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(6), 258–264.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Bertone, A., Mottron, L., Jelenic, P., & Faubert, J. (2005). Enhanced and diminished visuo-spatial information processing in autism depends on stimulus complexity. Brain, 128(10), 2430–2441.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boeschoten, M. A., Kemner, C., Kenemans, J. L., & van Engeland, H. (2005). The relationship between local and global processing and the processing of high and low spatial frequencies studied by event-related potentials and source modeling. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 228–236.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boeschoten, M. A., Kenemans, J. L., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2007a). Abnormal spatial frequency processing in high-functioning children with pervasive developmental disorder (PDD). Clinical Neurophysiology, 118(9), 2076–2088.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Boeschoten, M. A., Kenemans, J. L., van Engeland, H., & Kemner, C. (2007b). Face processing in pervasive developmental disorder (PDD): The roles of expertise and spatial frequency. Journal of Neural Transmission, 114(12), 1619–1629.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Brosnan, M. J., Scott, F. J., Fox, S., & Pye, J. (2004). Gestalt processing in autism: Failure to process perceptual relationships and the implications for contextual understanding. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45(3), 459–469.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Constantino, J. N., Przybeck, T., Friesen, D., & Todd, R. D. (2000). Reciprocal social behavior in children with and without pervasive developmental disorders. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 21(1), 2–11.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Davis, R., Bockbrader, M., Murphy, R., Hetrick, W., & O’Donnell, B. (2006). Subjective perceptual distortions and visual dysfunction in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(2), 199–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • de Jonge, M. V., Kemner, C., de Haan, E. H., Coppens, J. E., van den Berg, T., & van Engeland, H. (2007). Visual information processing in high-functioning individuals with autism spectrum disorders and their parents. Neuropsychology, 21(1), 65–73.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Gepner, B., & Tardif, C. (2004). Spatial frequency and face processing in children with autism and asperger syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(2), 199–210.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Deruelle, C., Rondan, C., Salle-Collemiche, X., Bastard-Rosset, D., & Da Fonseca, D. (2008). Attention to low- and high-spatial frequencies in categorizing facial identities, emotions and gender in children with autism. Brain and Cognition, 66(2), 115–123.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dobkins, K. R., Anderson, C. M., & Lia, B. (1999). Infant temporal contrast sensitivity functions (tCSFs) mature earlier for luminance than for chromatic stimuli: Evidence for precocious magnocellular development? Vision Research, 39(19), 3223–3239.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Faul, F., Erdfelder, E., Lang, A. G., & Buchner, A. (2007). G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods, 39(2), 175–191.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gumbel, E. J. (1958). Statistics of extremes. New York: Columbia University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gunther, K. L., & Dobkins, K. R. (2002). Individual differences in chromatic (red/green) contrast sensitivity are constrained by the relative number of L- versus M-cones in the eye. Vision Research, 42(11), 1367–1378.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gwiazda, J., Bauer, J., Thorn, F., & Held, R. (1997). Development of spatial contrast sensitivity from infancy to adulthood: Psychophysical data. Optometry and Vision Science, 74(10), 785–789.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., & Frith, U. (2006). The weak coherence account: Detail-focused cognitive style in autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 5–25.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Happé, F., Ronald, A., & Plomin, R. (2006). Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nature Neuroscience, 9(10), 1218–1220.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, H. C., Nozawa, G., & Kitterle, F. (1996). Global precedence, spatial frequency channels, and the statistics of natural images. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8(3), 197–230.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Jarrold, C., Gilchrist, I. D., & Bender, A. (2005). Embedded figures detection in autism and typical development: Preliminary evidence of a double dissociation in relationships with visual search. Developmental Science, 8(4), 344–351.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jemel, B., Mottron, L., & Dawson, M. (2006). Impaired face processing in autism: Fact or artifact? Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 91–106.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, N. L., Kotz, S., & Balakrisnan, N. (1995). Continuous univariate distributions (Vol. 2). New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Katsyri, J., Saalasti, S., Tiippana, K., von Wendt, L., & Sams, M. (2008). Impaired recognition of facial emotions from low-spatial frequencies in Asperger syndrome. Neuropsychologia, 46(7), 1888–1897.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, D. H. (1977). Visual contrast sensitivity. Journal of Modern Optics, 24(2), 107–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kemner, C., & van Engeland, H. (2006). ERPs and eye movements reflect atypical visual perception in pervasive developmental disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 45–54.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Koh, H. C., Milne, E., & Dobkins, K. R. (in prep). Magnocellular and parvocellular pathway functioning and their contribution to motion processing in adolescents with ASD and their siblings.

  • Lahaie, A., Mottron, L., Arguin, M., Berthiaume, C., Jemel, B., & Saumier, D. (2006). Face perception in high-functioning autistic adults: Evidence for superior processing of face parts, not for a configural face-processing deficit. Neuropsychology, 20(1), 30–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Liu, X. Q., Paterson, A. D., & Szatmari, P. (2008). Genome-wide linkage analyses of quantitative and categorical autism subphenotypes. Biological Psychiatry, 64(7), 561–570.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lord, C., Risi, S., Lambrecht, L., Cook, E. H., Leventhal, B. L., DiLavore, P. C., et al. (2000). The autism diagnostic observation schedule—generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Griffiths, H., Buckley, D., & Scope, A. (2009a). Vision in children and adolescents with autistic spectrum disorder: Evidence for reduced convergence. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 39(7), 965–975.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Scope, A., Pascalis, O., Buckley, D., & Makeig, S. (2009b). Independent component analysis reveals atypical electroencephalographic activity during visual perception in individuals with autism. Biological Psychiatry, 65(1), 22–30.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Milne, E., Swettenham, J., Hansen, P., Campbell, R., Jeffries, H., & Plaisted, K. (2002). High motion coherence thresholds in children with autism. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43(2), 255–263.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Mottron, L., Dawson, M., Soulières, I., Hubert, B., & Burack, J. A. (2006). Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: An update, and eight principles of autistic perception. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(1), 27–43.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Movshon, J. A., & Kiorpes, L. (1988). Analysis of the development of spatial contrast sensitivity in monkey and human infants. Journal of the Optical Society of America Association, 5(12), 2166–2172.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Pellicano, E., Gibson, L., Mayberry, M., Durkin, K., & Badcock, D. R. (2005). Abnormal global processing along the dorsal visual pathway in autism: A possible mechanism for weak visuospatial coherence? Neuropsychologia, 43(7), 1044–1053.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Ridder, W. H. (2004). Methods of visual acuity determination with the spatial frequency sweep visual evoked potential. Documenta Ophthalmologica, 109(3), 239–247.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Risi, S., Lord, C., Gotham, K., Corsello, C., Chrysler, C., Szatmari, P., et al. (2006). Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Amer Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 45(9), 1094–1103.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Robson, J. G. (1966). Spatial and temporal contrast-sensitivity functions of the visual system. Journal of Optical Society America, 56(8), 1141–1142.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rutter, M., Bailey, A., Lord, C., & Berument, S. K. (2003). Social communication questionnaire. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.

    Google Scholar 

  • Scharre, J. E., & Creedon, M. P. (1992). Assessment of visual function in autistic children. Optometry and Vision Science, 69(6), 433–439.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Tadevosyan-Leyfer, O., Dowd, M., Mankoski, R., Winklosky, B., Putnam, S., McGrath, L., et al. (2003). A principal components analysis of the autism diagnostic interview-revised. Journal of American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42(7), 864–872.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tager-Flusberg, H., & Joseph, R. M. (2005). Identifying neurocognitive phenotypes in autism. Philisophical Transactions of the Royal Society London B Biological Science, 358(1430), 303–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Taylor, M. M., & Creelman, C. D. (1967). PEST: Efficient estimates on probability functions. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 41(4A), 782–787.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Virsu, V., & Rovamo, J. (1979). Visual resolution, contrast sensitivity, and the cortical magnification factor. Experimental Brain Research, 37(3), 475–494.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watson, A. B. (1979). Probability summation over time. Vision Research, 19(5), 515–522.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Wechsler, D. (1999). Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment.

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by NIH grant R01 HD052804-01A2 (KD), and a WUN Research mobility award (HCK). We would like to thank all of the families who generously participated in this study, and the schools who helped with participant recruitment. We also acknowledge Ms Beth Hannaman, the Program Manager of Resources for Students with Autism, and the Research Review committee with the Research and Reporting Department, at the San Diego Unified School District for their valuable input and assistance in participant recruitment. We would also like to thank Sarah Song for her assistance with data collection, and Katie Wagner and Hao Ye for technical advice.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Karen Dobkins.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Koh, H.C., Milne, E. & Dobkins, K. Spatial Contrast Sensitivity in Adolescents with Autism Spectrum Disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 40, 978–987 (2010). https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0953-7

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s10803-010-0953-7

Keywords

Navigation