Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental condition characterised by persistent impairments in communication and social interaction, and restricted and repetitive patterns of behaviours, interests, or activities (American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2013). ASD prevalence has been increasing over the past decades (Hansen et al., 2015). AutisticFootnote 1 individuals often display marked difficulties in everyday adaptive functioning, such as in peer relationship and social interactions (Harkins et al., 2022). An early diagnosis is thus critical to determining, implementing, and optimising early intervention programs, considering their positive impact on daily functioning and developmental outcomes (Kodak & Bergmann, 2020).

Autism is more prevalent in males than females, being diagnosed more often in boys than girls, with a male-to-female ratio of approximately 4:1 (Halladay et al., 2015; Lai et al., 2014). In the absence of intellectual disabilities, this ratio is even more pronounced, increasing to 10:1 (Fombonne, 2009; Rivet & Matson, 2011). Prominent gender differences in the prevalence and phenotype of autism may contribute to females being diagnosed later than males—commonly in adolescence or adulthood (Carter et al., 2007)—underdiagnosed, or not even receiving a diagnosis (Loomes et al., 2017). Autistic females often report significant mental health problems and poorer well-being, partly because of vulnerabilities associated with being undiagnosed (Bargiela et al., 2016).

Studies show that the clinical presentation of autism may differ between genders (Head et al., 2014; Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al., 2014). Extensive research has explored possible differential phenotypic profiles between autistic females and males aimed at explaining this gender imbalance in the diagnosis (see Ferri et al., 2018, for a review). Literature points to phenotypic gender differences in multiple areas of functioning, such as in the core symptoms of autism, as well as in cognitive, socioemotional, and behavioural outcomes.

A recent meta-analysis documented that autistic females exhibit greater social interaction and social communication skills than autistic males, when these skills were measured with behavioural methods (e.g. play behaviour) (Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Cortese et al. 2021; Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Mandy et al. 2021). In parallel, when using standard clinical measures, such as the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS), males exhibit more severe presentation of symptoms and greater communication impairments (Rea et al., 2023), as well as more pronounced social interaction difficulties (Mandy et al., 2012) and repetitive and stereotyped behaviours (Van Wijngaarden-Cremers et al., 2014) compared to females. Instead, autistic females are more likely to show more impaired functioning outcomes, such as worse intellectual performance (Frazier et al., 2014; Kreiser & White, 2013), adaptive functioning (Rubenstein et al., 2015), and greater internalising (Oswald et al., 2016) and externalising (Frazier et al., 2014; Guerrera et al., 2019) problems than males. However, it is important to note that findings on gender differences between autistic females and males are complex and may vary by some factors, such as intellectual or behavioural characteristics (Giambattista et al., 2021; Posserud et al., 2021).

It is possible that the current conceptualisation of autism leads to a diagnostic gender bias towards males (Haney, 2016). One explanation for this bias may be related to specifications of the diagnostic criteria of benchmark assessment instruments, such as the ADOS (Lord et al., 2012) and the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R; Rutter, Bailey et al., 2003; Rutter, Le Couteur et al., 2003). These instruments were validated using predominantly male individuals previously diagnosed with autism (Kirkovski et al., 2013; Kopp & Gillberg, 2011; Kreiser & White, 2013; Lai et al., 2015; Mattila et al., 2011), and thus lacking sex-based norms (McPartland et al., 2016). Therefore, conclusions drawn from primarily male samples may narrow the landscape of symptomatology to be assessed by not accurately capturing the female autism phenotype.

The hypothesis of a ‘specific female autism phenotype’ is supported by evidence showing that autistic females without intellectual impairments perform similarly to neurotypical females and higher than autistic males in social cognition tasks (e.g. visual attention to faces) (Harrop et al., 2020) and language abilities (e.g. use more appropriate language in social interactions) (Hiller et al., 2016), which contributes to their under-recognition. Furthermore, this hypothesis is also corroborated by higher levels of motivation for social relationships and fewer social impairments in autistic females, as well as lower levels of restricted and repetitive interests than males (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020). This apparent normal social functioning of autistic females seems associated with their ability to camouflage social behaviours to fit social environmental demands (Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Cortese et al. 2021; Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Mandy et al. 2021).

Camouflaging refers to the process by which autistic individuals, especially females, minimise the visibility of their autism to be considered more suitable and acceptable in social settings/interactions (Hull et al., 2017, 2019; Lai et al., 2017). According to Hull et al. (2017), camouflaging consists of three key coping strategies—compensation (i.e. actively performing behaviours aimed at overcoming social difficulties associated with autistic symptoms); masking (i.e. actively hiding autistic symptoms and/or difficulties); and assimilation (i.e. actively adopting observed behaviours and attitudes to blend in with others in social situations). In this paper, we will use the term ‘camouflaging’ and/or ‘camouflage’ to refer to these strategies.

Research in this area has shown that camouflaging is used mainly by autistic females to adapt their behaviour to different environments, especially as they feel greater pressure to fit into social situations (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020; Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021). However, these behaviours come at a higher cost, as they have been associated with greater symptoms of anxiety and depression (Hull, Levy, et al., 2021), and are likely to cover specific autistic symptoms (Corbett et al., 2021).

Specific cognitive and other phenotypic traits may allow autistic females to camouflage autism-related social difficulties compared to autistic males, such as greater executive functioning abilities (e.g. cognitive flexibility; self-control skills), greater awareness of social norms (e.g. making eye contact), and of other’s cognitive and emotional states (e.g. Theory of Mind) (Hull, Petrides, et al., 2021; Livingston et al., 2019), as well as more social engagement and communication behaviours (Corbett et al., 2021). In addition, it may be that the ability to cover inappropriate social behaviours is shaped by socially constructed expectations directed at females regarding gender roles (Lai et al., 2015; Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021). Autistic females are expected to display more pro-social behaviours and form closer relationships with others compared to autistic males (Hull et al., 2019).

The Current Study

Given the increasing prevalence and phenotypic differences between autistic females and males, as well as camouflaging in females, it is important to formally investigate these differences to possibly inform on their contribution to the imbalance of the male/female ratio in autism diagnosis. We address these questions in two studies. In ‘Study 1 – Phenotypic Differences in Autism’, we first investigated gender differences in the autism diagnosis, by unravelling the different diagnostic symptoms of ASD. To this end, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of phenotypic differences between females and males in the core symptoms of autism (i.e. communication, social interaction and restricted interests and repetitive and stereotyped behaviour), and in cognitive (i.e. intellectual functioning), socioemotional (i.e. internalising problems) and behavioural (i.e. externalising behaviours) phenotypes. With this characterisation and considering that the clinical presentation of these symptoms may be affected by camouflage strategies, we then conducted ‘Study 2 – Camouflaging Differences in Autism’, a systematic review and meta-analysis of studies focusing on camouflaging in autistic females and males.

Study 1 – Phenotypic Differences in Autism

Study 1 addresses gender differences in the core symptoms of autism, as well as in cognitive, socioemotional, and behavioural phenotypes. The protocol for conducting this investigation was registered in PROSPERO (CRD42021282480) and followed the 2020 PRISMA guidelines (Page et al., 2021).

Method

Literature Search

The electronic datasets Pubmed, Scopus, Web of Science, and PsychInfo were searched for empirical studies, published between 2013 (to reflect the latest autism diagnostic criteria as the DSM 5 was published this year) and December 2022. Studies were considered if (i) enrolled males and females with a diagnosis of autism or Asperger’s syndrome according to the DSM-IV-TR and/or DSM 5 (APA, 2013) diagnostic criteria; (ii) focused on sex/gender differences in the core symptoms of autism (i.e. communication, social interaction and restricted interests and repetitive and stereotyped behaviour); and (iii) in cognitive, socioemotional, and behavioural functioning outcomes. The following search terms were used: (‘asd’ OR ‘autis*’ OR ‘asperger’) AND ((‘sex difference’ OR ‘sex differences’) OR (‘sex characteristic’ OR ‘sex characteristics’) OR (‘gender difference’ OR ‘gender differences’)) AND ((‘social’ OR ‘social adaptation’ OR ‘interact*’) OR (‘behav*’ OR ‘stereotyp*’ OR ‘inflexib*’ OR ‘flexib*’ OR ‘ritual*’) OR (‘language’ OR ‘linguistic’ OR ‘communicat*’) OR (‘sensor*’ OR ‘sensory processing’)).

Procedure

The initial database search resulted in 3555 articles, of which 1337 were duplicated. Hence, the title and abstract of 2218 articles were screened for the inclusion criteria by two independent researchers (RA and JM; k = 0.74). A third researcher (SC) acted as a consultant in case of conflict.

Articles were excluded if (i) used non-human samples (n = 270); (ii) were not in article format (e.g. case reports, reviews, or meta-analysis) (n = 117); (iii) were genetic studies (n = 200); (iv) included other pathologies as a comparison group (n = 38); (v) the main pathology described was not autism or had a comorbid diagnosis (e.g. attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] or intellectual disability [ID]) (n = 233); (vi) were gender-oriented but not investigating gender/sex differences in autism (n = 37); and/or (vii) were not investigating gender differences in the core symptoms and functioning outcomes (i.e. cognition, socioemotional, and behaviour) in autism (n = 1102). As studies including autistic children with comorbid ID were excluded, studies including children with an intellectual coefficient (IQ) below 70 (i.e. IQ < 70) were not considered. Of the remaining 221 articles, 14 could not be retrieved. Thus, the screening resulted in 207 potentially relevant articles. The full text of these articles was retrieved and screened for inclusion criteria by two independent researchers (ADC and JM), and, in case of doubt or conflict, a consensus meeting was carried out with other researchers (SC and MFP). After a throughout and comprehensive examination of these articles, 140 were additionally excluded. This occurred because they were duplicated (n = 33), reported qualitative analysis (n = 7), did not provide information about the autism diagnosis (n = 3), did not focus on gender differences in the core symptoms or functioning outcomes (i.e. cognitive, socioemotional, and/or behaviour) (n = 41), were methodological-oriented research (e.g. assessing the discriminative characteristics of a questionnaire) (n = 16), did not present the data correctly (e.g. means and standard deviation values were not presented separately by gender, see Zhang et al., 2022, for an example) (n = 36), and used a different instruments to assess the dimensions under investigation (i.e. could not be included in the analysis due to the inability to compare results) (n = 1). Also, a study (Sturrock, Mardsen et al., 2020) was excluded as it reported the same results as another and earlier study (Sturrock, Yau et al., 2020, which was included in the analysis). Finally, two studies were removed as part of the study participants had comorbid ID (n = 2). Thus, 67 studies were included in the analysis. Figure 1 provides the flowchart of this selection procedure.

Fig. 1
figure 1

PRISMA flowchart depicting study selection procedures

Some studies were included in more than one analysis, as they examined more than one area of interest for this review (e.g. a dimension of the core autistic symptoms, such as social interaction, and a functioning outcome, for example a cognitive functioning; see as an example Frazier et al., 2014). Overall, 45 articles compared females and males in the core symptoms of autism, whereas 44 compared genders regarding functioning outcomes. Of these, 24 addressed only the core symptoms, 23 only functioning outcomes, and 21 core symptoms and functioning outcomes combined (see Table 1 and 2 for detailed information about the included articles).

Table 1 Mean (M) values and standard deviations (SD) of age and measurement scores for the included articles addressing gender differences in the core symptoms of autism (i.e. total symptoms, communication, social interactions, and restricted interests and repetitive and stereotyped behaviour)
Table 2 Mean (M) values and standard deviations (SD) of age and measurement scores for the included articles addressing gender differences in functioning outcomes (i.e. cognitive, socioemotional, and behaviour)

Data Selection and Extraction

The following information was extracted from the articles: (i) sample size, (ii) gender distribution, (iii) autism diagnosis information, and (iv) instruments used and scores for the dimensions investigated. Age was not considered as studies use numerous and different instruments and these are age-specific (see Table 1 and 2 for details on age). In total, the current study included 16,066 autistic individuals, of which 10,917 were males and 5149 females.

Different instruments were used across the studies to measure the core symptoms and functioning outcomes in autistic individuals. The following instruments were used to measure the core symptoms of autism: the ADOS (calibrated severity score—CSS; social affective—SA; and restricted and repetitive behaviours scale—RRB) (n = 21); the ADI-R (communication and restricted and repetitive behaviours scales) (n = 4); the Autism Spectrum Quotient (AQ; total score) (n = 6) (Baron-Cohen et al., 2001); the Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ; total score) (n = 13) (Rutter, Bailey et al., 2003; Rutter, Le Couteur et al., 2003); the Repetitive Behaviour Scale Revised (RBS-R; total score) (n = 6) (Lam & Aman, 2007); the Vineland of Adaptive Behaviour Scales, Second Edition (Vineland-II; communication, socialisation and maladaptive behaviour scales, and the composite score) (n = 12) (Sparrow et al., 2005); and the Social Responsiveness Scale 2 (SRS-2; total raw score and total T score) (n = 10) (Constantino, 2013). Although the Vineland-II does not target autism-specific symptoms, we have included the communication, socialisation, and maladaptive behaviour scales in the core symptoms because they provide valuable clinical information that reflects the core symptoms of autism and on adaptive behaviour that may inform about the diagnosis of developmental disabilities (Dupuis et al., 2021; Milne et al., 2019). All instruments are parent report but the ADOS and the ADI-R are clinician screening reports.

Cognitive functioning was assessed using the full scale intelligence quotient (FSIQ), verbal intelligence quotient (VIQ) and performance intelligence quotient (PIQ) of the Weschler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence (WASI; Wechsler, 1999) (n = 17) or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS; Wechsler, 2008) (n = 2) and of the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC; Wechsler, 2008) (n = 8), the General Conceptual Ability (GCA) of the Differential Ability Scales, Second Edition (DAS-II; Elliot et al., 2018) (n = 5), and the Non-verbal Age Equivalent scale of the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (MSEL; Mullen, 1995) (n = 4). The Child Behavioural Checklist (CBCL; Achenbach & Ruffle, 2000), a parent report questionnaire, was used to measure socioemotional difficulties (Internalising Problems scale) (n = 7) and behavioural problems (Externalising Problems scale) (n = 6). Additionally, the Vineland-II daily living skills scale (n = 5) was used to assess behavioural problems.

The quality and risk of bias were assessed independently using the Joanna Briggs Institute (JBI) critical appraisal checklist for analytic cross-sectional studies. Supplementary Material Table 1 provides information on the quality of the studies.

Data Analysis

Meta-analyses of continuous outcome data were performed with meta R package. Analyses were computed separately for each instrument used to measure the multiple outcomes (e.g. the CSS scale of the ADOS, each subscale of the Vineland-II, the GCA of the DAS-II, or the Internalising and Externalising scales of the CBCL). A meta-analysis of the communication and restricted and repetitive behaviours scales of the ADI-R, the total score of the CBCL, and the Fine Motor, Visual Reception, and Receptive and Expressive Language scales of the MSEL was not computed as only two studies have used these measures and thus did not provide sufficient power to conduct the analysis (Ioannidis et al., 2008).

Standardised mean difference (SMD) was used as a summary measure for pooling studies. The SMD and 95% confident interval (CI) were calculated in R library using the default method (Hedges’ g method). A SMD above zero indicates that males scored higher than females and a SMD below zero indicates that females scored higher than males. Common and random effect estimates were obtained for inverse variance weighting meta-analyses. Heterogeneity was evaluated using the between-study variance (t2) and the I-squared (I2) statistics. For simplicity and considering the applicability of the results beyond the included studies, the random-effect results are discussed; for the dimensions with low heterogeneity (i.e. p > 0.05; I2 < 50%), the random-effects model is also considered.

Results

Figures 2 and 3 depict the forest plots of the gender differences in the autism core symptoms and in the cognitive, socioemotional, and behavioural phenotypes, respectively. The forest plots of the non-significant results are presented in Fig. 1 of the Supplementary Material.

Fig. 2
figure 2

Forest plots of the gender differences in the autism core symptoms—severity of symptoms and social interaction difficulties

Fig. 3
figure 3

Forest plots of the gender differences in the cognitive and behavioural phenotypes

Autism Core Symptoms

Total Symptoms

The meta-analysis revealed significant gender differences in the CSS of the ADOS, SMD = 0.15, 95% CI = (0.02; 0.29), z = 2.18, p = 0.03. Autistic males presented worse severity scores compared to autistic females. No significant results were observed in the AQ, SMD = −0.29, 95% CI = (−0.75; 0.16), z = −1.27, p = 0.20; in the SCQ, SMD = −0.02, 95% CI = (−0.16; 0.12), z = −0.27, p = 0.79, or in the composite score of the Vineland-II, SMD = −0.15, 95% CI = (−0.65; 0.35), z = −0.58, p = 0.56.

Communication

There were no gender differences in the communication scale of the Vineland-II, SMD = −0.00, 95% CI = (−0.19; 0.19), z = −0.02, p = 0.99.

Social Interaction

The meta-analysis revealed significant gender differences in the SA subscale of the ADOS, SMD = 0.26, 95% CI = (0.07; 0.45), z = 2.65, p < 0.01. Autistic males presented more social interactive impairments than females. In addition, significant gender differences were found in the Socialisation scale of the Vineland-II, SMD = 0.15, 95% CI = (0.06; 0.24), z = 3.15, p < 0.01, in which autistic males presented more social interactive abilities than autistic females. The meta-analysis showed significant gender differences in the SRS-2 total raw score, SMD = −0.31, 95% CI = (−0.57; −0.04), z = −2.26, p = 0.02, and in the SRS-2 total-T score, SMD = −0.23, 95% CI = (−0.39; −0.06), z = −2.74, p < 0.01, in which autistic females presented more social interactive impairments than autistic males.

Restricted Interests and Repetitive and Stereotyped Behaviour

There were no significant gender differences in the RRB scale of the ADOS, SMD = 0.10, 95% CI = (−0.05; 0.24), z = 1.31, p = 0.19, or in the RBS-R total score, SMD = 0.02, 95% CI = (−0.08; 0.12), z = 0.37, p = 0.71.

Cognitive, Socioemotional, and Behavioural Functioning Outcomes

The meta-analysis revealed gender differences in the GCA of the DAS-II, SMD = 0.44, 95% CI = (0.18; 0.69), z = 3.39, p < 0.01, in which males presented higher conceptual ability scores than females. No significant gender differences were observed in the WASI/WAIS FSIQ, SMD = −0.00, 95% CI = (−0.13; 0.12), z = −0.06, p = 0.95, VIQ, SMD = 0.01, 95% CI = (−0.15; 0.18), z = 0.17, p = 0.87, and PIQ, SMD = 0.09, 95% CI = (−0.05; 0.22), z = 1.25, p = 0.21, or for the WISC FSIQ, SMD = 0.02, 95% CI = (−0.09; 0.12), z = 0.36, p = 0.72, VIQ, SMD = 0.02, 95% CI = (−0.09; 0.13), z = 0.37, p = 0.71, and PIQ, SMD = 0.04, 95% CI = (−0.14; 0.21), z = 0.39, p = 0.70. Moreover, no gender differences were observed in the Non-Verbal Age Equivalent scale of the MSEL, SMD = −0.06, 95% CI = (−0.27; 0.15), z = −0.53, p = 0.60.

As for socioemotional and behavioural phenotypes, the meta-analysis showed gender differences in the Externalising Problems scale of the CBCL, SMD = −0.09, 95% CI (−0.19; −0.02), z = −2.01, p = 0.04, in which females present higher externalising problems scores than males. No statistically significant gender differences were observed in Internalising Problems scale of the CBCL, SMD = −0.05, 95% CI = (−0.25; 0.15), z = −0.50, p = 0.61, or in the Daily Living Skills of the Vineland, SMD = 0.08, 95% CI = (−0.22; 0.37), z = 0.51, p = 0.61.

Discussion

This study examined phenotypic gender differences in autism core symptoms (i.e. communication, social interaction and restricted interests, and repetitive and stereotyped behaviour), and in cognitive (i.e. intellectual functioning), socioemotional (i.e. internalising problems), and behavioural (i.e. externalising behaviours) phenotypes. The meta-analysis revealed no gender differences in the domains of communication and restricted interests and repetitive and stereotyped behaviours. However, significant differences were observed between autistic females and males in the severity score of the ADOS and in the social interaction domain.

The results indicated that autistic females show a less severe presentation of autism symptoms than males when measured using the ADOS. This is consistent with previous studies and systematic reviews suggesting that males exhibit a more severe presentation of symptoms than females when assessed with clinical instruments (Waizbard-Bartov et al., 2022). Similarly, for the social interaction domain, the meta-analysis revealed that autistic males displayed increased social interaction difficulties compared to females in the ADOS, which is in accordance with other evidence (Mandy et al., 2012). However, when these dimensions were assessed with the SRS-2 or the Vineland-II scales, which are parent/caregiver or teacher (in the case of the SRS-2) reports, autistic females exhibited increased social interaction problems. This is in line with other research suggesting that autistic females are usually more impaired on parent-report measures of social functioning than males, despite the performance on standard diagnostic measures (Ratto et al., 2018). This may also be related to higher social expectations towards females, as they are expected to display more pro-social behaviours and establish closer social relationship with others (Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021). When this is not the case, autistic females are likely to be perceived as having more difficulties in social interactions (Hull et al., 2019).

As for the functioning outcomes, the meta-analysis yielded gender differences in the cognitive and behavioural phenotypes. Particularly, autistic females presented more cognitive difficulties and externalising problems (e.g. defiance or aggressive behaviours) compared to autistic males. This is in line with other evidence suggesting that autistic females exhibit poorer intellectual functioning and more externalising behaviour difficulties, such as more irritability or self-injurious behaviours, than autistic males (Frazier et al., 2014). This seems to indicate that for females to receive a diagnosis of autism, they must present marked difficulties in overall functioning outcomes. For example, a study demonstrated that when females and males were rated similarly on diagnostic measures, females with higher IQs were less likely to meet the criteria for receiving a diagnosis of autism (Ratto et al., 2018). This is also in accordance with research indicating that autistic females need to exhibit more intellectual and behavioural problems to be captured by the current autism diagnostic criteria (Posserud et al., 2021).

In sum, the results seem to indicate that the clinical standard measures to assist an autism diagnosis are biased towards a male manifestation of ASD (Halladay et al., 2015; Loomes et al., 2017). Interestingly, even with comparable levels of symptom severity, females are less likely than males to receive a diagnosis of autism (Geelhand et al., 2019). It is possible that a female presentation of autism, potentially marked by differences in symptoms severity manifestation and social interaction skills compared to males, is not being captured by the current clinical procedures, which may constitute a barrier for females being properly diagnosed (Estrin et al., 2021). Symptoms and difficulties of autistic females may be expressed differently from the traditional, male-biased diagnostic criteria for autism, or may even express characteristics and/or behaviours that are not included in these criteria. In fact, one hypothesis that has gained increasing interest in the literature as underpinning the female autism phenotype is camouflaging, which appears to additionally support gender differences in the manifestation of autistic traits (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020). In addition, given the differences observed between clinician- and parent-reported measures, it is possible that females may be more motivated or able to camouflage during clinical assessments (i.e. more structured interactions), whereas a parent would be aware of difficulties in less structured settings (e.g. home). Although camouflaging had been attributed to autistic females, a comprehensive approach is needed to understand gender differences in the use of camouflage strategies/behaviours. To address this issue, we conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis comparing camouflaging between autistic females and males.

Study 2 – Camouflaging Differences in Autism

Study 2 addresses gender differences in the use of camouflage strategies. This study also extends a previous systematic review conducted by our research team (Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021) on camouflaging in autistic females.

Method

Literature Search

This study expands on a previous systematic review conducted by our research team (Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021) on camouflaging in autistic females. In our previous research, we performed a literature search for empirical articles published between January 2009 and September 2019, which resulted in 13 studies that were included in that systematic review. Here, we conducted an additional search for articles published between October 2019 and October 2022. The same electronic databases described in study 1 and in Tubío-Fungueiriño et al. (2021) were searched for empirical studies in English. Studies were considered if (i) enrolled males and females with a diagnosis of autism or Asperger’s syndrome according to the DSM-IV-TR and/or DSM 5 diagnostic criteria (APA, 2013), and (ii) were focused on camouflaging, masking, compensation, assimilation, copy, or imitation behaviours of autistic symptoms in females. The following search terms were used: (‘autism’ OR ‘asd’ OR autis OR ‘asperger’) AND (‘gender’ OR ‘girls’ OR ‘woman’ OR ‘women’ OR ‘female*’ OR (sex AND difference)) AND (camoufla OR mask OR copy OR compensat OR imitat*).

Procedure

The database search resulted in 1268 articles, of which 400 were duplicated. Thus, the title and abstract of 868 articles were screened for the inclusion criteria by two researchers (SCZ and MTF). Two other researchers (SC and MF) acted as consultants in case of any conflict.

Articles were excluded if (i) used non-human samples (n = 53); (ii) were not in article format (e.g. case reports, reviews, or meta-analysis) (n = 47); (iii) the main pathology described was not autism or Asperger (n = 366); and (iv) were not focused on the study of camouflaging with an autistic population (n = 369). The screening resulted in 33 potentially relevant articles that were retrieved and screened for the inclusion criteria. Of these, one article could not be retrieved. After examining the remaining 32 articles, 10 were further excluded either because they did not include information about the diagnosis (n = 6), consisted of same gender participants (n = 3), or did not examine camouflaging in autistic population (n = 1).

Therefore, 22 studies were selected, and the full text was retrieved and screened for inclusion criteria. At this moment, three studies were excluded because they enrolled the same participants as described in other studies thus reporting the same results. We decided to include Cook et al. (2021) and Jorgenson et al. (2020) because they were conducted first and focused on camouflage behaviours in autistic individuals. These 19 studies were added to the 13 articles previously included in our systematic review (Tubío-Fungueiriño et al., 2021), resulting in 32 studies, whose full text was examined. Figure 4 depicts the flowchart of the selection procedures.

Fig. 4
figure 4

PRISMA flowchart depicting study selection procedures

Data Selection and Extraction

The following information was extracted from the articles: (i) sample size, (ii) gender distribution, (iii) autism diagnosis information, and (iv) instruments used and scores for measuring camouflaging behaviours.

Of the 32 studies, 15 measured camouflaging using The Camouflaging Autistic Traits Questionnaire (CAT-Q), six used the discrepancy method (i.e. capture and compare individuals’ scores on different measures), one used measures of social ability, and 10 conducted qualitative methods, such as observation (n = 3), or author-developed surveys (n = 7). However, due to the optimal statistical power to perform the meta-analysis, only the studies that used the CAT-Q were considered. Of the 15 studies, five were excluded because they did not report camouflaging score separated by gender.

Ten studies investigating camouflaging in autistic individuals were included in the meta-analysis (see Table 3 for detailed information about these studies). The quality and risk of bias of these were assessed using the JBI (Supplementary Material Table 2). Three studies were conducted with adolescents and seven with adults. Five studies reported gender differences considering only the total score of the CAT-Q, while six reported gender differences considering not only the total score, but also the scores of the additional three subscales—compensation, assimilation, and masking. Of the six studies that reported all the CAT-Q scores (the total and the three subscale), two were conducted with adolescents and four with adults. In addition, three studies enrolled non-binary autistic individuals, of which one presented only the CAT-Q total score and two the CAT-Q total and the subscales scores.

Table 3 Mean (M) values and standard deviations (SD) of age and CAT-Q total score and compensation, masking, and assimilation subscales scores for the included articles addressing camouflaging in autism

In total, the studies included in the meta-analysis enrolled 1172 participants, of whom 516 were males, 655 females, and 35 non-binaries. Of these, 998 were adults (592 males and 406 females) and 173 adolescents (110 males and 63 females).

Data Analysis

Data analysis was conducted for the CAT-Q total score and for the subscales—compensation, assimilation, and masking. Gender comparisons were computed for adults and adolescents separately. A SMD above zero indicates that males display more camouflage behaviours, whereas a SMD below zero indicates that females present more camouflage behaviours. As in study 1, the random-effects results are discussed, regardless of the level of heterogeneity. Because no age differences were observed, results for overall effects are presented.

Results

Figure 5 depicts the forest plots of the statistically significant gender differences in the CAT-Q total score and in the compensation, masking, and assimilation subscales.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Forest plots of the gender differences in the CAT-Q total score and compensation, masking, and assimilation subscales

The meta-analysis revealed that females scored higher than males in the total score of the CAT-Q, SMD = −0.30, 95% CI = (−0.48; −0.12), z = −3.26, p < 0.01. No significant gender differences were observed between females or males and non-binary participants. The meta-analysis revealed that females scored higher than males in the compensation, SMD = −0.29, 95% CI = (−0.51; −0.07), z = −2.60, p < 0.01, and masking, SMD = −0.24, 95% CI = (−0.45; −0.02), z = −2.13, p = 0.03, subscales. However, meta-analysis indicated no gender differences in the assimilation subscale, SMD = −0.23, 95% CI = (−0.46; 0.00), z = −1.93, p = 0.05.

Discussion

This study investigated gender differences in camouflaging in autism. The results indicated that when using the CAT-Q, females exhibited higher total camouflaging scores than males. This supports the camouflaging hypothesis in females and is consistent with other literature documenting that autistic females camouflage more than autistic males, both in adolescence and adulthood (Beck et al., 2020; Dean et al., 2017; Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020). We also observed that females utilise more masking and compensation strategies, but not assimilation, compared to males. Evidence is mixed regarding gender differences in the CAT-Q subscales, either reporting gender differences in masking and assimilation strategies, but not in compensation (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020), or in assimilation and compensation strategies, but not on masking (McQuaid et al., 2022). Similarly, evidence shows that non-binary autistic adults exhibit camouflage behaviours (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020; McQuaid et al., 2022), although they are not significantly different from autistic cisgender females or males, which was also observed in our results.

Studies point to the fact that autistic females appear to be more motivated to participate and engage in social interactions as they use different behavioural strategies to adapt to the demands of social environments (Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Cortese et al. 2021; Wood-Downie, Wong, Kovshoff, Mandy et al. 2021). Our meta-analysis supports this evidence and further suggests that this appears to be achieved primarily through the utilisation of two camouflaging strategies—masking and compensation.

Masking is used to cover natural responses and adopt alternative behaviours to be accepted in social situations (Hull et al., 2019). Studies show that autistic females often mimic other people’s facial expressions (Cook et al., 2018), suppress repetitive behaviours (Wiskerke et al., 2018), or maintain appropriate eye contact (Lai et al., 2017) to conform to social group norms. Compensation refers to the use of strategies to overcome specific social difficulties associated with autistic symptoms (Hull et al., 2019). For example, some studies showed that females tend use more nonverbal communication or reciprocal communication on preferred topics (Corbett et al., 2021; Hiller et al., 2014), appear to pay attention to faces (Harrop et al., 2019), or recognise and infer other’s emotional states (Lai et al., 2017). In addition, one study demonstrated that autistic females who scored high on compensation strategies exhibited stronger social engagement and communication behaviours (Corbett et al., 2021).

Females seem to exhibit more socially appropriate behaviours than males, expressed mainly through masking and compensation strategies, which may prevent them from displaying the typical presentation of autism and therefore do not conform to the current diagnostic assessment criteria (McQuaid et al., 2022). This seems to be in line with the results of our first the meta-analysis, which indicated that when assessed with standard clinical measures, autistic females show less severe symptoms and social interactive difficulties. Furthermore, evidence suggests that high cognitive abilities (e.g. executive functions) are required to perform compensation and masking behaviours, as they need to self-monitor and inhibit innate behaviours (Hull, Levy, et al., 2021; Hull, Petrides, et al., 2021). This is equally in line with our first meta-analysis which that females need to present higher cognitive and behavioural problems to be diagnosed. It is possible that by masking and compensating for their autistic symptoms, females are more likely of being uncaptured by the current clinical criteria.

General Discussion

This work has emphasised important gender differences in autism presentation, with females exhibiting lower symptom severity and impairments in social interaction. The underdiagnosis of autism in females may be due to a different expression of autistic symptoms compared to males, therefore not meeting current diagnostic criteria, as they perhaps manifest a specific female autism phenotype (Hull, Lai et al., 2020; Hull, Petrides et al., 2020).

Our results support the argument that current clinical diagnostic tools are biased towards males and that females’ autism presentation may be overlooked in the diagnostic process, especially if they do not present marked cognitive and/or behavioural difficulties. These findings have important implications for how clinicians measure autistic symptoms severity and social interaction difficulties, which certainly determines the assessment of behavioural symptoms in females. If diagnostically relevant behaviours, especially social-related behaviours, are being camouflaged, this may exacerbate the possibility of females being underdiagnosis or misdiagnosis (Cook et al., 2022). It is possible that autism is underdiagnosed in females because they camouflage their social difficulties and therefore conceal symptoms and do not meet current diagnostic criteria. This may reflect a different autistic profile between females and males and contribute to the imbalance in the diagnosis of autism.

Furthermore, research has shown that autistic females experience increased internalising and mental health problems because the try to ‘fit in’ socially by camouflaging their autistic traits (Beck et al., 2020). Other research has also shown that females who engage in camouflaging tend to deliberately inhibit ASD-related behaviours (i.e. externalising problems, such as repetitive behaviours) (Corbett et al., 2021). In sum, evidence suggests that for females to be diagnosed, they must present increased difficulties that, if camouflaged, are unlikely to be captured by current diagnostic procedures.

The lack of a better understanding of a possible female autism phenotype, potentially consisting of camouflaging, may hinder the accurate identification of autistic females (Allely, 2019). In accordance, recent camouflaging-oriented assessment tools, such as the CAT-Q, may be a useful measure to address this issue, capturing different forms of camouflaging strategies and, if integrated into clinical settings, aiding earlier and more accurate diagnoses, especially in females.

Limitations and Future Directions

Although this work aimed to comprehensively understand gender differences in autism core symptoms, functioning outcomes, and camouflaging, synthesising evidence of this sort was challenging because studies use numerous and different assessment instruments. Because of this, and to not lose statistical power in the first study, the analysis was not computed controlling for participants’ age. This is a limitation. In particular, the significant results observed for the DAS (compared to other IQ measures) should be interpreted with caution. Developmental differences also contribute for the results (e.g. the heterogeneity in the trajectory of autistic symptoms across childhood, adolescence, and adulthood) and, thus, future studies should replicate this meta-analysis, controlling for age effects. In addition, the full range of IQ was not considered in this study as the presence of ID was an exclusion criterion. Other research should confirm our findings considering the full range of IQ. In the second study, despite including non-binary people in the analysis, they may not be in sufficient number to capture significant differences. It is important that future research continue to address possible differences in the expression of autistic traits in non-binaries. Importantly, we did not analyse the link between cognitive performance and the use of camouflaging strategies. Future studies should examine this relationship. Besides, the inclusion of only English language articles may not capture possible cultural variations that may be influencing the diagnosis, which should be account for in other investigations. Finally, future research should also include both quantitative and qualitative approaches to add to the comparison of camouflaging experiences across individuals.

Conclusion

Epidemiological and clinical studies in autism have established a male predominance in autism prevalence, possible gender differences in autistic traits and a greater diagnostic difficulty for females. Perhaps females express their autistic traits differently, such as through camouflaging, and are therefore probably being underserved by the current conceptualisation and recognition of autism (Estrin et al., 2021). As a result, females may experience longer delays in clinical assessments and, consequently, are being underdiagnosed and/or misdiagnosed. It is thus important to improve and widespread the understanding and recognition of an autism presentation in females. Otherwise, autistic females may be at greater risk for marginalisation experiences, such as loss of personal, academic, and professional opportunities, loss of social support and understanding, and, consequently, at greater vulnerability to physical and mental health (e.g. anxiety and depression) problems.