Skip to main content
Log in

Cooperation and social classes: evidence from Colombia

  • Published:
Social Choice and Welfare Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

We investigate the relationship between social class belonging and contributions to local public goods. By utilizing the social class classifications in Colombia and an experimental design based on the strategy method, we can both study contributions to public goods and classify subjects into contribution types. We find similar contribution levels between high and medium-low social classes and also similar distributions of contributor types. However, low social class members conditionally contribute a significantly higher level than high social class members. This has implications for policymakers, who may need to consider differential policy schemes for locally provided public goods.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. It should be noted that in a multi-period public goods experiment, a selfish individual might contribute for strategic reasons just to encourage other group members to contribute.

  2. By culture we mean classification of people which is not directly related to genetic inheritance; it can be both material as well as non-material such as beliefs and norms.

  3. To explicitly test for the group effect, Gächter and Thöni (2005) created artificial groups within a culture in a public goods experiment. They conducted a two-stage public goods experiment where they first conducted a one-shot public goods experiment to identify types of contributors and then grouped subjects of similar contribution types together (and informed them about it) in a multi-period public goods experiment. They found that cooperative groups managed to maintain high levels of cooperation throughout the experiment and vice versa. Similar findings are reported in, e.g., Gunnthorsdottir et al. (2007), Ones and Putterman (2007), and Page et al. (2005).

  4. The labeling “low-low” is a direct translation of the Spanish labeling “bajo-bajo.” Although this term would probably be better labeled as “very low” in English, we decided to keep the original Spanish labeling.

  5. For previous studies on common pool resources and public goods in Colombia, see, e.g., Cárdenas et al. (2000) and Cárdenas (2003).

  6. There is substantial evidence that people cooperate also with genetically unrelated people in one-shot events, i.e., “strong reciprocity” (e.g., Gintis 2000; Fehr et al. 2002; Fehr and Fischbacher 2003).

  7. See National Department of Statistics of Colombia (2011a, b).

  8. The exchange rate at the time of the experiment was approximately US$ 1 = COP 2000.

  9. In cases where samples have different opportunity costs, either the absolute amount in the experiment or the opportunity cost can be kept constant. We decided to keep the opportunity cost constant; it should be noted that Kocher et al. (2008) did not find a significant stake effect using a one-shot public goods experiment. In our experiment, a lunch at the MEDIUM-LOW social-class university costs approximately 75 % of a lunch at the HIGH social-class university, and similar differences exist for other goods in the respective neighborhoods.

  10. Both figures include a show-up fee of COP 5000.

  11. When comparing material resources and subjective socio-economic status not using broad categories of social class (university) but instead by socio-economic strata, the results hold. This indicates that socio-economic stratification is a good proxy for social class.

  12. In fact, the conditional contribution of participants from stratum 1 is higher than the average contribution from the other group members, i.e., it is above the perfect conditional contribution schedule.

  13. At the MEDIUM-LOW social class university, 27 % of the participants perfectly match their unconditional contribution given their beliefs and their responses in in the conditional contribution table; the corresponding figure is 20 % for the HIGH social class university. Based on a test of proportions we cannot reject the hypothesis that they are the same (p value = 0.39).

  14. The results hold when using socio-economic strata as an independent variable instead of a dummy variable for HIGH social class.

References

  • Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR (2000) Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: preliminary data in healthy, white women. Health Psychol 19:586–592

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson L, DiTraglia F, Gerlach J (2011) Measuring altruism in a public goods experiment: a comparison of U.S. and Czech subjects. Exp Econ 14:426–437

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Anderson CM, Putterman L (2006) Do non-strategic sanctions obey the law of demand? The demand for punishment in the voluntary contribution mechanism. Games Econ Behav 54:1–24

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Axelrod R (1984) The evolution of cooperation. Basic Books, New York

    Google Scholar 

  • Baland JM, Platteau JP (1996) Halting degradation of natural resources: is there a role for rural communities?. Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations, Rome

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowlby J (1979) The making and breaking of affectional bonds. Tavistock, London

    Google Scholar 

  • Brandts J, Saijo T, Schram AJHC (2004) A four-country comparison of spite and cooperation in public goods games. Public Choice 119:381–424

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Buckley E, Croson R (2006) Income and wealth heterogeneity in the voluntary provision of linear public goods. J Public Econ 90:935–955

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Burlando RM, Hey JD (1997) Do Anglo-Saxons free ride more? J Public Econ 64:41–60

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas J-C (2003) Real wealth and experimental cooperation: experiments in the field lab. J Dev Econ 70:263–289

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas JC (2011) Social norms and behavior in the local commons as seen through the lens of field experiments. Environ Resour Econ 48:451–485

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas J-C, Carpenter J (2008) Behavioural development economics: lessons from field labs in the developing world. J Dev Stud 44:337–364

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cárdenas J-C, Stranlund J, Willis C (2000) Local environmental control and institutional crowding-out. World Dev 28:1719–1733

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Carpenter JP (2007) The demand for punishment. J Econ Behav Organ 62:522–542

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chan KS, Mestelman S, Moir R, Muller A (1996) The voluntary provision of public goods under varying income distributions. Can J Econ 29:54–69

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chaudhuri A (2011) Sustaining cooperation in laboratory public goods experiments: a selective survey of the literature. Exp Econ 14:47–83

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cherry TD, Kroll S, Shogren JF (2005) The impact of endowment heterogeneity and origin on public good contributions: evidence from the lab. J Econ Behav Organ 57:357–365

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Drentea P (2000) Age, debt, and anxiety. J Health Soc Behav 41:437–450

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U (2003) The nature of human altruism. Nature 425:785–791

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Fischbacher U, Gächter S (2002) Strong reciprocity, human cooperation, and the enforcement of social norms. Hum Nature 13:1–25

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gächter S (2000) Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. Am Econ Rev 90:980–994

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fehr E, Gächter S (2002) Altruistic punishment in humans. Nature 415:137–140

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher U, Gächter S (2010) Social preferences, beliefs, and the dynamics of free riding in public good experiments. Am Econ Rev 100:541–556

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Fischbacher U, Gächter S, Fehr E (2001) Are people conditionally cooperative? Evidence from a public goods experiment. Econ Lett 71:397–404

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gardner A, West S (2004) Cooperation and punishment, especially in humans. Am Nat 164:753–764

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Goergantzis N, Proestakis A (2011) Accounting for real wealth in heterogeneous-endowment public good games. ThE Papers 10/20, Department of Economic Theory and Economic History of the University of Granada

  • Gächter S (2007) Conditional cooperation: behavioural regularities from the lab and the field and their policy implications. In: Frey BS, Stuzter A (eds) Psychology and economics: a promising new cross-disciplinary field (Cesinfo seminar Series). MIT Press, Cambridge, pp 19–50

    Google Scholar 

  • Gächter S, Herrmann B (2011) The limits of self-goverence when cooperators get punished: experimental evidence from urban and rural Russia. Eur Econ Rev 55:193–210

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gächter S, Renner E (2010) The effects of (Incentivized) belief elicitation in public goods experiments. Exp Econ 13:364–377

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gächter S, Thöni C (2005) Social learning and voluntary cooperation among like-minded people. J Eur Econ Assoc 3:303–314

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gächter S, Herrmann B, Thöni C (2010) Culture and cooperation. Philos Transit R Soc B Biol Sci 365:2651–2661

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gintis H (2000) Strong reciprocity and human sociality. J Theor Biol 206:169–179

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gunnthorsdottir A, Houser D, McCabe K (2007) Disposition, history and contributions in public goods experiments. J Econ Behav Organ 62:304–315

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hammerstein P (ed) (2003) Genetic and cultural evolution of cooperation. The MIT Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Hardin G (1968) The tragedy of the commons. Science 162:1243–1248

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Henrich J, Henrich N (2007) Why humans cooperate: a cultural and evolutionary explanation. Evolution and cognition series. Oxford University Press, Oxford

    Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann B, Thöni C, Gächter S (2008) Antisocial punishment across societies. Science 319:1362–1367

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Herrmann B, Thöni C (2009) Measuring conditional cooperation: a replication study in Russia. Exp Econ 12:87–92

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hofmeyr A, Burns J, Visser M (2007) Income inequality, reciprocity and public good provision: an experimental analysis. S Afr J Econ 75:508–520

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kocher M, Martinsson P, Visser M (2012) Social background, cooperative behavior, and norm enforcement. J Econ Behav Organ 81:341–354

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kocher M, Cherry T, Kroll S, Netzer R, Sutter M (2008) Conditional cooperation on three continents. Econ Lett 101:175–178

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kosfeld M, Heinrichs M, Zak PJ, Fischbacher U, Fehr E (2005) Oxytocin increases trust in humans. Nature 453:673–676

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus MW, Piff PK, D (2009) Social class, the sense of control, and social explanation. J Personal Soc Psychol 97:992–1004

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus MW, Horberg EJ, Goetz JL, Keltner D (2011) Social class rank, threat vigilance, and hostile reactivity. Personal Soc Psychol Bull 37:1376–1388

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kraus MW, Piff PK, Mendoza-Denton R, Rheinschmidt ML, Keltner D (2012) Social class, solipsism, and contextualism: how the rich are different from the poor. Psychol Rev 119:546–572

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Lamba S, Mace R (2011) Demography and ecology drive variation in cooperation across human populations. Proc Natl Acad Sci 108:14426–14430

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ledyard JO (1995) Public goods: a survey of experimental research. In: Kagel J, Roth A (eds) The handbook of experimental economics. Princeton University Press, Princeton, pp 94–111

    Google Scholar 

  • Maier-Rigaud F, Martinsson P, Staffiero G (2010) Ostracism and the provision of a public good: experimental evidence. J Econ Behav Organ 73:387–395

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Martinsson P, Pham-Khanh N, Villegas-Palacio C (2013) Conditional cooperation and disclosure in developing countries. J Econ Psychol 34:148–155

  • Masclet D, Villeval MC (2008) Punishment, inequality and welfare: a public good experiment. Soc Choice Welf 31:475–502

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • National Department of Statistics of Colombia (2011a) Documento Conpes 3386: Focaliz Subsidious Servicios Publicos. National Department of Statistics of Colombia. http://www.dane.gov.co/files/dig/CONPES_3386_oct2005_Focaliz_subsidios_servicios_publicos.pdf. Accessed 01 Oct 2012

  • National Department of Statistics of Colombia (2011b) Database. http://www.dane.gov.co. Accessed 01 Oct 2012

  • Oakes JM, Rossi RH (2003) The measurement of SES in health research: current practice and steps toward a new approach. Soc Sci Med 56:769–784

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ones U, Putterman L (2007) The ecology of collective action: a public goods and sanctions experiment with controlled group formation. J Econ Behav Organ 62:495–521

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Olson M (1965) The logic of collective action: public goods and the theory of groups. Harvard University Press, Boston

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom E, Gardner R, Walker J (1992) Rules, game and common pool resources. The University of Michigan Press, Michigan

    Google Scholar 

  • Ostrom E (1991) Governing the commons: the evolution of institutions for collective action. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Page T, Putterman L, Unel B (2005) Voluntary association in public goods experiments: reciprocity, mimicry, and efficiency. Econ J 115:1032–1052

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Piff PK, Stancato DM, Côté S, Mendoza-Denton R, Keltner D (2012) Higher social class predicts increased unethical behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci 109:4086–4091

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rico D (2005) Evaluación del costo de las matrículas en la Universidad Nacional de Colombia. Universidad Nacional De Colombia, mimeo, Sede Medellín, Oficina de Planeación

  • Rustagi D, Engel S, Kosfeld M (2010) Conditional cooperation and costly monitoring explain success in forest commons management. Science 330:961–965

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Shogren JF (1990) On increased risk and voluntary provision of public goods. Soc Choice Welf 7:221–229

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Snibbe AC, Markus HR (2005) You can’t always get what you want: educational attainment, agency, and choice. J Personal Soc Psychol 88:703–720

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sober E, Wilson DS (1998) Unto others. The evolution and psychology of unselfish behaviour. Harvard University Press, Cambridge

    Google Scholar 

  • Taylor SE, Klein LC, Lewis BP, Gruenewald TL, Gurung RAR, Updegraff JA (2000) Biobehavioral responses to stress in females: tend-and-befriend, not fight-or-flight. Psychol Rev 107:411–429

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Thöni C, Tyran J-R, Wengström E (2012) Microfoundations of social capital. J Public Econ 96:635–643

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zelmer J (2003) Linear public goods experiments: a meta-analysis. Exp Econ 6:299–310

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

The authors gratefully acknowledge the financial support from the Swedish Research Council (Vetenskapsrådet), Formas through the program Human Cooperation to Manage Natural Resources (COMMONS), and the Swedish International Development Cooperation Agency (Sida) to the Environmental Economics Unit at the University of Gothenburg. We are also grateful to the School of Engineering (Escuela de Ingeniería de Antioquia), Medellín, Colombia, and Universidad Nacional de Colombia, Sede Medellín, for their support of our experiment. Special thanks go to Antonio Villegas-Rivera and Felipe Mejía for excellent research assistance, and to Martin Kocher, Marta Matute, Katarina Nordblom, Patrik Söderholm, Alba Upegui, managing editor Clemens Puppe, and two anonymous referees for their helpful comments.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Conny Wollbrant.

Appendix

Appendix

See Table 6.

Table 6 Distribution of contribution types in selected previous studies

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Martinsson, P., Villegas-Palacio, C. & Wollbrant, C. Cooperation and social classes: evidence from Colombia. Soc Choice Welf 45, 829–848 (2015). https://doi.org/10.1007/s00355-015-0886-3

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s00355-015-0886-3

JEL Classification

Navigation