Skip to main content
Log in

Temporal Organization of Extinction-Induced Responding in Preschool Children

  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Six preschool children were placed individually in an experimental room for 10 to 14 daily 15-min sessions with a mechanical marble-dispensing clown and a toy punching bag. In each of two parts of the study, marbles were dispensed on a schedule consisting of 96-s periods of fixed-time 12-s marble delivery alternating with 96-s nonreinforcement periods (multiple fixed-time/extinction schedule). During nonreinforcement periods, five of the children hit or shook the punching bag (aggression) two to ten times more often than they did during the marble delivery periods. For four of the six children, the amount of bag punching formed a bimodal function with the least amount of responding at the very beginning, middle, and end of the nonreinforcement components. In the second part of the study, the addition of an audible discriminative stimulus during marble delivery periods for two subjects resulted in a decreased rate of responding during the first 12 s of marble delivery as compared to the rate of the children who did not have the tone stimulus. These results provide an expanded basis for the comparison of human and nonhuman extinction-induced behavior by describing the probability of human extinction-induced behavior across time and demonstrating the suppressive effects on extinction-induced responding of a stimulus correlated with reinforcement.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • AZRIN, N. H., HUTCHINSON, R. R., & HAKE, D. F. (1966). Extinction-induced aggression. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 9, 191–204.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • BOSHKA, S. C., WEISMAN, H. M., & THOR, D. H. (1966). A technique for inducing aggression in rats utilizing morphine withdrawal. The Psychological Record, 16, 541–543.

    Google Scholar 

  • CHEREK, D. R. (1982). Schedule-induced cigarette self-administration. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 17, 523–527.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FALK, J. L. (1961). Production of polydipsia in normal rats by an intermittent food schedule. Science, 133, 195–196.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FALK, J. L. (1971). The nature and determinants of adjunctive behavior. Physiology and Behavior, 6, 577–588.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FALLON, J. H., Jr., ALLEN, J. D., & BUTLER, J. A. (1979). Assessment of adjunctive behaviors in humans using a stringent control procedure. Physiology and Behavior, 22, 1089–1092.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FREDERIKSEN, L. W., & PETERSON, G. L. (1974). Schedule-induced aggression in nursery school children. The Psychological Record, 24, 343–351.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • FREDERIKSEN, L. W., & PETERSON, G. L. (1977). Schedule-induced aggression in humans and animals: A comparative parametric review. Aggressive Behavior, 3, 57–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GALLUP, G. G., Jr. (1965). Aggression in rats as a function of frustrative nonreward in a straight alley. Psychonomic Science, 11, 99–100.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HARRELL, W. A. (1972). Effects of extinction on magnitude of aggression in humans. Psychonomic Science, 29, 213–215.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R. (1973). The environmental causes of aggression. In J. K. Cole & D. Jensen (Eds.), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation (pp. 155–181). Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R. (1977). By-products of aversive control. In W. K. Honig & J. E. R. Staddon (Eds.), Handbook of operant behavior (pp. 415–431). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R., AZRIN, N. H., & HAKE, D. F. (1966). An automatic method for the study of aggression in squirrel monkeys. Journal of the Experimental of Behavior, 9, 233–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R., PIERCE, G. E., EMLEY, G. S., PRONI, T. J., & SAUER, R. A. (1977). The laboratory measurement of human anger. Biobehavioral Reviews, 1, 241–259.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • HUTCHINSON, R. R., & RENFREW, J. W. (1978). Functional parallels between the neural and environmental antecedents of aggression. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 2, 33–58.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • KELLY, J. F., & HAKE, D. F. (1970). An extinction-induced increase in an aggressive response within humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 14, 153–164.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • LASITER, P. S. (1979). Influence of contingent responding on schedule-induced activity in human subjects. Physiology and Behavior, 22, 239–243.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • LEVITSKY, D., & COLLIER, G. (1968). Schedule-induced wheel-running. Physiology and Behavior, 3, 571–573.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • LOONEY, T. A., & COHEN, P. S. (1982). Aggression induced by intermittent positive reinforcement. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 6, 15–37.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • MULLER, P. G., CROW, R. E., & CHENEY, C. D. (1979). Schedule-induced locomotor activity in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of behavior, 31, 83–90.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ONO, K. (1987). Superstitious behavior in humans. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 47, 261–271.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • ULRICH, R., DULANEY, S., ARNETT, M., & MUELLER, K. (1973). An experimental analysis of nonhuman and human aggression. In J. F. Knutson (Ed.), The control of aggression: Implications for basic research (pp. 79–111). Chicago: Aldine.

    Google Scholar 

  • WAGNER, G. A., & MORRIS, E. K. (1987). “Superstitious” behavior in children. The Psychological Record, 37, 471–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Additional information

This article is based on research conducted by James T. Todd in partial fulfillment of the degree of Masters of Arts at the University of Kansas. It was supported in part by a University of Kansas Biomedical Science Support Grant (4888-x706-3), by NICHHD training grant HD-02528 and by National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Training Grant HD07173. We thank Lisa M. Johnson, Linda Moreau-Pletch, Susan M. Schneider, and Steven T Higgins for their helpful comments, the teachers and staff of Edna A. Hill Child Development Lab preschools for their cooperation, Janice Pittman and Brenda K. Causey for calculating and reviewing the entire data set, and Jesus Rosales for preparation of figures.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Todd, J.T., Morris, E.K. & Fenza, K.M. Temporal Organization of Extinction-Induced Responding in Preschool Children. Psychol Rec 39, 117–130 (1989). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395056

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395056

Navigation