Skip to main content
Log in

The Status of Rule-Governed Behavior as Pliance, Tracking and Augmenting within Relational Frame Theory: Middle-Level Rather than Technical Terms

  • Theoretical article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

A recent systematic review has highlighted that the terms “pliance,” “tracking,” and “augmenting” have rarely been used as the basis for conducting systematic experimental-analytic research since their conception in 1982, despite their theoretical centrality to the study of rule-governed behavior and their presumed impact on psychological suffering. Given that some time has passed since the review article, it may be useful to reflect again upon their place within the literature on the experimental analysis of human behavior, and relational frame theory in particular. As such, the current article constitutes a “position piece” rather than another formal systematic review. In reviewing (informally) the literature since the systematic review, the recent emergence of psychometric research involving these concepts could be seen as reinforcing the original conclusions, in that researchers are recognizing that pliance, tracking, and augmenting may be of limited value in the experimental analysis of human behavior. Instead, the concept of rule-governed behavior itself, as well as the subcategories of pliance, tracking, and augmenting, should be considered middle-level terms, which lack the relative precision of more technical terms within the literature on relational frame theory.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. We would also note that other articles have been published since our informal review of the literature (in April 2020) that could be seen as broadly relevant (e.g., Ghezzi, Houmanfar, & Crosswell, 2020; Ruíz et al., 2020). However, the general argument we are advancing in the current “position” article, that rule-governed behavior, and thus pliance, tracking, and augmenting, are best considered middle-level terms, are not fundamentally challenged by the publication of these additional articles.

  2. It is also worth noting that some of the leading researchers who have argued for the value of middle-level terms in behavioral psychology have recently questioned the use of traditional psychometry itself in favor of idiographic, process-based research (Hayes et al., 2019). Thus, the use of nomothetic psychometrics in support of the concepts of pliance, tracking and augmenting could be challenged further in the newly emerging idiographic, process-focused era.

References

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Luciano, C., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). From IRAP and REC model to a multi-dimensional multi-level framework for analyzing the dynamics of arbitrarily applicable relational responding. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 6(4), 473–483. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2017.08.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McEnteggart, C. (2020). Updating RFT (more field than frame) and its implications for process-based therapy. The Psychological Record, 70, 605–624. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-019-00372-3.

  • Barnes-Holmes, Y., Hussey, I., McEnteggart, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Foody, M. (2016). Scientific ambition: The relationship between relational frame theory and middle level terms in acceptance and commitment therapy. In R. D. Zettle, S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The Wiley Handbook of Contextual Behavioral Science (pp. 365–382). Wiley-Blackwell.

  • Barnes-Holmes, D., O’Hora, D., Roche, B., Hayes, S. C., Bissett, R. T., & Lyddy, F. (2001). Understanding and verbal regulation. In S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & B. Roche (Eds.), Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition (pp. 103–118). Plenum.

  • Baruch, D. E., Kanter, J. W., Busch, A. M., Richardson, J. V., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2007). The differential effect of instructions on dysphoric and nondysphoric persons. The Psychological Record, 57, 543–554. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395594.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bern, R., Persdotter, T., Harte, C., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2020). Relational coherence and persistent rule-following: The impact of targeting coherence in a “non-critical” component of a relational network. The Psychological Record. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00414-1.

  • Fagerstrøm, A., Stratton, J. P., & Foxall, G. R. (2015). The impact of corporate social responsibility activities on the consumer purchasing situation. Journal of Organizational Behavior Management, 35, 184–205. https://doi.org/10.1080/01608061.2015.1093053.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2018). Exploring the single-trial-type-dominance-effect in the IRAP: Developing a differential arbitrarily applicable responding effects (DAARE) model. The Psychological Record, 68(1), 11–25. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-017-0262-z.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Finn, M., Barnes-Holmes, D., McEnteggart, C., & Kavanagh, D. (2019). Predicting and influencing the single-trial-type-dominance-effect: The first study. The Psychological Record, 69(3), 425–435. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-019-00347-4.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ghezzi, E. L., Houmanfar, R. A., & Crosswell, L. (2020). The motivative augmental effects of verbal stimuli on cooperative and conformity responding under a financially competing contingency in an analog work task. The Psychological Record, 70, 411–431. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-020-00400-7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McEnteggart, C. (2018). The impact of high versus low levels of derivation for mutually and combinatorially entailed relations on persistent rule-following. Behavioural Processes, 157, 36–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2018.08.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). Persistent rule-following in the face of reversed reinforcement contingencies: The differential impact of direct versus derived rules. Behavior Modification, 41(6), 743–763. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517715871.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & Kissi, A. (2020a). The study of rule-governed behavior and derived stimulus relations: Bridging the gap. Perspectives on Behavior Science, 43, 361–385. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40614-020-00256-w.

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., McEnteggart, C., Gys, J., & Hassler, C. (2020b). Exploring the potential impact of relational coherence on persistent rule-following: The first study. Learning & Behavior, 48, 373–391. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-019-00399-0.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Barnes-Holmes, Y., & McEnteggart, C. (2020c). Exploring the impact of coherence (through the presence versus absence of feedback) and levels of derivation on persistent rule-following. Learning and Behavior. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13420-020-00438-1.

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D., Moreira, M., de Almeida, J. H., Aparecida-Passarelli, D., & de Rose, J. C. (2021). Exploring a Training IRAP as a single participant context for analyzing reversed derived relations and persistent rule-following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.671.

  • Hayes, S. C. (2016). Why contextual behavioral science exists: An introduction to part I. In R. D. Zettle, S. C. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, & A. Biglan (Eds.), The Wiley handbook of contextual behavioral science (pp. 9–16) Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Strosahl, K., & Wilson, K. G. (1999). Acceptance and commitment therapy: An experiential approach to behavior change. Guilford Press.

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. Plenum.

  • Hayes, S. C., Hofmann, S. G., Stanton, C. E., Carpenter, J. K., Sanford, B. T., Curtiss, J. E., & Ciarrochi, J. (2019). The role of the individual in the coming era of process-based therapy. Behavior Research & Therapy, 117, 40–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2018.10.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kissi, A., Hughes, S., De Schryver, M., De Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (2018). Examining the moderating impact of plys and tracks on the insensitivity effect: A preliminary investigation. The Psychological Record, 68, 431–440. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0286-z.

  • Kissi, A., Hughes, S., Mertens, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., De Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (2017). A systematic review of pliance, tracking, and augmenting. Behavior Modification, 41(5), 683–707. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517693811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • McAuliffe, D., Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2014). The dark-side of rule governed behavior: An experimental analysis of problematic rule-following in an adolescent population with depressive symptomatology. Behavior Modification, 38(4), 587–613. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445514521630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • O’Connor, M., Byrne, P., Ruiz, F. J., & McHugh, L. (2019). Generalized pliance in relation to contingency sensitivity and mindfulness. Mindfulness, 10, 833–840. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12671-018-1046-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., Roche, B., & Smeets, P. M. (2004). Derived relational networks and control by novel instructions: A possible model of generative verbal responding. The Psychological Record, 54, 437–460. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395484.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2014). Antecedent and consequential control of derived instruction-following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 102(1), 66–85. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Rafacz, S. D., Houmanfar, R. A., Smith, G. S., & Levin, M. E. (2019). Assessing the effects of motivational augmentals, pay-for-performance, and implicit verbal responding on cooperation. The Psychological Record, 69, 49–66. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0324-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rehfeldt, R. A., & Hayes, L. J. (1998). The operant-respondent distinction revisited: Toward and understanding of stimulus equivalence. The Psychological Record, 48, 187–210. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395266.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz, F. J., García-Martín, M. B., Suárez-Falcón, J. C., Bedoya-Valderrama, L., Segura-Vargas, M. A., Peña-Vargas, A., Peña-Vargas, A., Henao, A. M., & Ávila-Campos, J. E. (2020). Development and initial validation of the Generalized Tracking Questionnaire. PLoS ONE, 15(6), e0234393. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0234393.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Ruiz, F. J., Suárez-Falcón, J. C., Barbero-Rubio, A., & Flórez, C. L. (2019). Development and initial validation of the generalized pliance questionnaire. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 12, 189–198. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.03.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ruíz-Martínez, Y., Hernández-Buitrago, P., & Quiroga Baquero, L. A. (2019). Effects of differential training in rule-governed behavior on compliance with traffic rules by pedestrians. Interamerican Journal of Psychology, 53(3), 399–416. https://doi.org/10.30849/rip/ijp.v53i3.883.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Salazar, D. M., Ruiz, F. J., Flórez, C. L., & Suárez-Falcón, J. C. (2018). Psychometric properties of the generalized pliance questionnaire-children. International Journal of Psychology & Psychological Therapy, 18(3), 273–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sidman, M. (1994). Equivalence relations and behavior: A research story. Authors Cooperative.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1953). Science and human behavior. Macmillan.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1957) Verbal behavior. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

  • Skinner, B. F. (1966). An operant analysis of problem solving. In B. Keinmuntz (Ed.), Problem-solving: Research, method, and therapy (pp. 225–257). Wiley.

  • Stapleton, A., & McHugh, L. (2020a). Just ask! Importance of qualitative explorations of children’s rule-following. Children’s Research Digest, 6(2), 1–5.

  • Stapleton, A., & McHugh, L. (2020b). Adolescent correlates of the generalized pliance questionnaire—children. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 15, 131–134. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2019.12.006.

  • Waldeck, D., Pancani, L., & Tyndall, I. (2019). An examination of the construct validity of the generalized pliance questionnaire. Journal of Contextual Behavioral Science, 11, 50–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcbs.2018.12.003.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1982). Rule-governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive-behavior therapy. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (Vol. 1, pp. 73–118). Academic.

Download references

Availability of data an material

Not applicable

Code availability

Not applicable

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Colin Harte.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval

Not applicable

Consent to participate

Not applicable

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, D. The Status of Rule-Governed Behavior as Pliance, Tracking and Augmenting within Relational Frame Theory: Middle-Level Rather than Technical Terms. Psychol Rec 72, 145–158 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00458-x

Download citation

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-021-00458-x

Keywords

Navigation