Skip to main content
Log in

Examining the Moderating Impact of Plys and Tracks on the Insensitivity Effect: a Preliminary Investigation

  • Original Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

This experiment examined whether the tendency for people to adhere to rules or instructions that no longer occasion effective action, depends on the: 1) source (self- versus socially-generated) and/or 2) nature (plys or tracks) of the rule. This was done by providing participants with either instructions (i.e., a ply or track) or no-instructions about the contingencies operating in a Matching To Sample (MTS) task. During the first phase of this task instructed and non-instructed contingencies overlapped. However, halfway through the task, the non-instructed contingencies reversed so that now instructed and non-instructed contingencies conflicted. Overall, the results indicated that after the non-instructed contingency reversal participants adhered more to 1) socially- as opposed to self-generated rules and 2) plys compared to tracks.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1

Similar content being viewed by others

Notes

  1. Note that terms such as (verbal) rules and instructions broadly refer to a set of (verbal) antecedent stimuli and are not technical terms that emerged from an inductive functional analysis (see O’Hora, Barnes-Holmes, & Stewart, 2014, for recent work in that vein). As such, we will use these terms interchangeably as a means to orient the reader towards a particular class of behavior.

  2. In the current experiment we also manipulated the initial accuracy of the rules or instructions (i.e., ply and track). A total of 75 participants were initially recruited (54 women; mean age = 23.68 years, SD = 6.73, age range 18–60). All participants were randomly allocated (i.e., by drawing a piece of paper with participant numbers from a bag) to one of the five experimental groups: the initially accurate ply (N = 15), initially accurate track (N = 17), initially inaccurate ply (N = 15), initially inaccurate track (N = 15) or no-instructions group (N = 13). In this paper, we will only discuss the procedure used and the results obtained from the initially accurate instructions and the no-instructions groups. An outline of the procedure that was used in the initially inaccurate instruction groups as well as the results obtained from those groups can be obtained upon request from the first author (AK).

  3. See Appendix Table 2 for the proportion of MLCS selections for each participant during blocks 1–6.

References

  • Baruch, D. E., Kanter, J. W., Busch, A. M., Richardson, J. V., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2007). The differential effect of instructions on dysphoric and nondysphoric persons. The Psychological Record, 57, 543–554.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Berger-Tal, O., Nathan, J., Meron, E., & Saltz, D. (2014). The exploration-exploitation dilemma: A multidisciplinary framework. PLoS One, 9(4), e95693. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0095693.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Bilalić, M., McLeod, P., & Gobet, F. (2008). Why good thoughts block better ones: The mechanism of the pernicious Einstellung (set) effect. Cognition, 108(3), 652–661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.005.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Donadeli, J. M., & Strapasson, B. A. (2015). Effects of monitoring and social reprimands on instruction-following in undergraduate students. The Psychological Record, 65(1), 177–188.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Harte, C., Barnes-Holmes, Y., Barnes-Holmes, D., & McEnteggart, C. (2017). Persistent rule-following in the face of reversed reinforcement Contingencies: The differential impact of direct versus derived rules. Behavior modification. Advance online publication. doi: https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517715871.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C. (1993). Rule governance: Basic behavioral research and applied applications. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 2, 193–197. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep10769746.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Haas, J. R., & Greenway, D. E. (1986a). Instructions, multiple schedules, and extinction: Distinguishing rule-governed from schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46, 137–147. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1986.46-137.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Brownstein, A. J., Zettle, R. D., Rosenfarb, I., & Korn, Z. (1986b). Rule-governed behavior and sensitivity to changing consequences of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45, 237–256. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1986.45-237.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Roche, B. (2001). Relational frame theory: A post-Skinnerian account of human language and cognition. New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hayes, S. C., Zettle, & Rosenfarb, I. (2004). Rule-following. In S. C. Hayes (Ed.), Rule-governed behavior: Cognition, contingencies & instructional control (pp. 191–218). Oakland: Context Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Henley, A. J., Hirst, J. M., Reed, F. D. D., Becirevic, A., & Reed, D. D. (2017). Function-altering effects of rule phrasing in the modulation of instructional control. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 33, 24–40. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-016-0063-5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hojo, R. (2002). Effects of instructional accuracy on a conditional discrimination task. The Psychological Record, 52(4), 493–506.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2016). Relational frame theory: Implications for the study of human language and cognition. In S. Hayes, D. Barnes-Holmes, R. Zettle, & T. Biglan (Eds.), Handbook of contextual behavioral science (pp. 1–79). New York: Wiley-Blackwell.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joyce, H. J., & Chase, P. N. (1990). Effects of response variability on the sensitivity of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54, 251–262.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kissi, A., Hughes, S., Mertens, G., Barnes-Holmes, D., De Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (2017a). A systematic review of pliance, tracking, and augmenting. Behavior Modification, 41(5), 683–707. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445517693811.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Kissi, A., De Schryver, M., Hughes, S., De Houwer, J., & Crombez, G. (2017b). The dark side of learning via instructions: The insensitivity effect - data sets. Open Science Framework. https://osf.io/mmj2j/.

  • Kroger-Costa, A., & Abreu-Rodrigues, J. (2012). Effects of historical and social variables on instruction following. The Psychological Record, 62(4), 691–705.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kudadjie-Gyamfi, E., & Rachlin, H. (2001). Rule-governed versus contingency-governed behavior in a self-control task: Effects of changes in contingencies. Behavioural Processes, 57, 29–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kühberger, A., Fritz, A., & Scherndl, T. (2014). Publication bias in psychology: A diagnosis based on the correlation between effect size and sample size. PLoS One, 9(9), e105825. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0105825.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Martinez-Sanchez, H., & Ribes-Iñesta, E. (1996). Interactions of contingencies and instructional history on conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 46(2), 301–318.

  • Matthews, B. A., Shimoff, E., Catania, C., & Sagvolden, T. (1977). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity to ratio and interval contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 27, 453–467. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1977.27-453.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • McAuliffe, D., Hughes, S., & Barnes-Holmes, D. (2014). The dark-side of rule governed behavior: An experimental analysis of problematic rule-following in an adolescent population with depressive symptomatology. Behavior Modification, 38, 587–613. https://doi.org/10.1177/0145445514521630.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Miller, J. R., Hirst, J. M., Kaplan, B. A., DiGennaro Reed, F. D., & Reed, D. D. (2014). Effects of mands on instructional control: A laboratory simulation. The Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 30(6), 100–112. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40616-014-0015-x.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mitchell, C. J., De Houwer, J., & Lovibond, P. F. (2009). The propositional nature of human associative learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 32, 183–246. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X09000855.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Monestès, J. L., Villatte, M., Stewart, I., & Loas, G. (2014). Rule-based insensitivity and delusion maintenance in schizophrenia. The Psychological Record, 64(2), 329–338.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Nevin, J. A., & Grace, R. C. (2000). Behavioral momentum: Empirical, theoretical, and metaphorical issues. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(1), 117–125.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ninness, H. A. C., & Ninness, S. A. (1998). Superstitious math performance: Interactions between rules and scheduled contingencies. The Psychological Record, 48, 45–62.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Hora, D., Barnes-Holmes, D., & Stewart, I. (2014). Antecedent and consequential control of derived instruction-following. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 102, 66–85. https://doi.org/10.1002/jeab.95.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Orne, M. T. (1962). On the social psychology of the psychological experiment: With particular reference to demand characteristics and their implications. American Psychologist, 17(11), 776–783. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0043424.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Otto, T. L., Torgrud, L. J., & Holborn, S. W. (1999). An operant blocking interpretation of instructed insensitivity to schedule contingencies. The Psychological Record, 49, 663–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Podlesnik C., DeLeon I. (2015). Behavioral Momentum Theory: Understanding Persistence and Improving Treatment. In: DiGennaro Reed F., Reed D. (Eds.), Autism Service Delivery (pp 327–351). Autism and Child Psychopathology Series. New York: Springer. doi:https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4939-2656-5_12.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

  • Rosenfarb, I. S., Newland, M. C., Brannon, S. E., & Howey, D. S. (1992). Effects of self-generated rules on the development of schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58(1), 107–121. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1992.58-107.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Catania, A. C., & Matthews, B. A. (1981). Uninstructed human responding: Sensitivity of low-rate performance to schedule contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 36, 207–220. https://doi.org/10.1901/jeab.1981.36-207.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • Shimoff, E., Matthews, B. A., & Catania, A. C. (1986). Human operant performance: Sensitivity and pseudosensitivity to contingencies. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 46(2), 149–157.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Törneke, N. (2010). Learning RFT: An introduction to relational frame theory and its clinical applications. Oakland: New Harbinger Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Törneke, N., Luciano, C., & Valdivia-Salas, S. (2008). Rule-governed behavior and psychological problems. International Journal of Psychology and Psychological Therapy, 8, 141–156.

    Google Scholar 

  • Villas-Bôas, A., Meyer, S. B., Kanter, J. W., & Callaghan, G. M. (2015). The use of analytic interventions in functional analytic psychotherapy. Behavior Analysis: Research and Practice, 15(1), 1–19.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zettle, R. D., & Hayes, S. C. (1982). Rule governed behavior: A potential theoretical framework for cognitive therapy. In P. C. Kendall (Ed.), Advances in cognitive-behavioral research and therapy (pp. 73–118). New York: Academic.

    Chapter  Google Scholar 

Download references

Funding

The preparation of this paper was made possible with the support of the Ghent University Grant BOF16/MET_V/002 and the Interuniversity Attraction Poles Program initiated by the Belgian Science Policy Office (IUAPVII/33).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Ama Kissi.

Ethics declarations

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Ethical Approval

All procedures performed with the (human) participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of the institutional research committee and with the 1964 Helsinki declaration and its later amendments or comparable ethical standards.

Informed Consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Appendex

Appendex

Table 2 Mean proportion of most-like comparison stimulus selections for each participant during blocks 1–6

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kissi, A., Hughes, S., De Schryver, M. et al. Examining the Moderating Impact of Plys and Tracks on the Insensitivity Effect: a Preliminary Investigation. Psychol Rec 68, 431–440 (2018). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0286-z

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40732-018-0286-z

Keywords

Navigation