Skip to main content
Log in

Effects of Historical and Social Variables on Instruction Following

  • Article
  • Published:
The Psychological Record Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study investigated the effect of the presence of the experimenter on behavioral sensitivity to contingency change. In history training, college students were exposed to differential reinforcement of low rate (DRL) and fixed-ratio (FR) schedules, and in testing, to a fixed-interval (FI) schedule. For the control group, instructions were never provided, whereas for the without-observer and with-observer groups, inaccurate instructions were given (variable ratio [VR], fixed time [FT], and differential reinforcement of high rate [DRH], respectively). The experimenter was present during testing for the with-observer group. In history training, DRL response rates were lower than FR rates with or without instructions. In testing, the control and without-observer groups showed lower response rates and a more efficient behavioral patterning than did the with-observer group. These results indicate that instruction following was affected by the presence of the experimenter despite a history of no reinforcement for such behavior, and they suggest that instructional control may be strengthened by social contingencies.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • BARRETT, D. H., DEITZ, S. M., GAYDOS, G. R., & QUINN, P. (1987). The effects of programmed contingencies and social conditions on response stereotypy with human subjects. The Psychological Record, 37(4), 489–505.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • BAUMANN, A. A., ABREU-RODRIGUES, J., & SOUZA, A. S. (2009). Rules and self-rules: Effects of variation upon behavioral sensitivity to change. The Psychological Record, 59(4), 641–670. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/tpr/vol59/iss4/8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CERUTTI, D. T. (1991). Discriminative versus reinforcing properties of schedules as determinants of schedule insensitivity in humans. The Psychological Record, 41(1), 51–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • CERUTTI, D. T. (1994). Compliance with instructions: Effects of randomness in scheduling and monitoring. The Psychological Record, 44(2), 259–269.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • COTTRELL, N. B., WACK, D. L., SEKERAK, G. J., & RITTLE, R. H. (1968). Social facilitation of dominant responses by the presence of an audience and the mere presence of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 9(3), 245–250. doi:10.1037/h0025902

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • FREEMAN, T. J., & LATTAL, K. A. (1992). Stimulus control of behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 57(1), 5–15. doi:10.1901/jeab.1992.57-5

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GALIZIO, M. (1979). Contingency-shaped and rule-governed behavior: Instructional control of human loss avoidance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 31(1), 53–70. doi:10.1901/jeab.19779.31-53

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • GALIZIO, M., JACKSON, L. A., & STEELE, F. O. (1979). Enforcement symbols and driving speed: The overreaction effect. Journal of Applied Psychology, 64(3), 311–315.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • GRACE, R. C., SCHWENDIMAN, J. W., & NEVIN, J. A. (1998). Effects of unsignaled delay of reinforcement on preference and resistance to change. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 69(3), 247–261. doi:10.1901/jeab.1998.69-247

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HACKENBERG, T. D., & JOKER, V. R. (1994). Instructional versus schedule control of humans’ choices in situations of diminishing returns. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 62(3), 367–383. doi:10.1901/jeab.1994.62-367

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HANNA, E. S., BLACKMAN, D. E., & TODOROV, J. C. (1992). Stimulus effects on concurrent performance in transition. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58(2), 335–347. doi:10.1901/jeab.1992.58-335

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • HAYES, S. C., BROWNSTEIN, A. J., ZETTLE, R. D., ROSENFARB, I., & KORN, Z. (1986). Rule-governed behavior and sensitivity to changing consequences of responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 45(3), 237–256. doi:10.1901/jeab.1986.45-237

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • JOYCE, J. H., & CHASE, P. N. (1990). Effects of response variability on the sensitivity of rule-governed behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 54(3), 251–262. doi:10.1901/jeab.1990.54-251

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • KAUFMAN, A., BARON, A., & KOPP, R. E. (1966). Some effects of instructions on human operant behavior. Psychonomic Monograph Suplements, 1(11), 243–250.

    Google Scholar 

  • LEFRANÇOIS, J. R., CHASE, P. N., & JOYCE, J. H. (1988). The effects of a variety of instructions on human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 49(3), 383–393. doi:10.1901/jeab.1988.49-383

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MADDEN, G. J., CHASE, P. N., & JOYCE, J. H. (1998). Making sense of sensitivity in the human operant literature. The Behavior Analyst, 21(1), 1–12.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • MARKHAM, M. R., DOUGHER, M. J., & WULFERT, E. (1993). Social contingencies and the effects of punishment in alcoholics and nonalcoholics. Behavior Therapy, 24(2), 277–284. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80296-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MARTINEZ, H., & TAMAYO, R. (2005). Interactions of contingencies, instructional accuracy, and instructional history in conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 55(4), 633–646. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/trp/vol55/iss4/8

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • MARTINEZ-SANCHEZ, H., & RIBES-IÑESTA, E. (1996). Interactions of contingencies and instructional history on conditional discrimination. The Psychological Record, 46, 301–318.

    Google Scholar 

  • MATTHEWS, B. A., CATANIA, A. C., & SHIMOFF, E. (1985). Effects of uninstructed verbal behavior on nonverbal responding: Contingency descriptions versus performance descriptions. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 43(2), 155–164. doi:10.1901/jeab.1985.43-155

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • NEVIN, J. A., SMITH, L. D., & ROBERTS, J. (1987). Does contingent reinforcement strengthen operant behavior? Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 48(1), 17–33. doi:10.1901/jeab.1987.48-17

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • OKOUCHI, H. (1999). Instructions as discriminative stimuli. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 72(2), 205–214. doi:10.1901/jeab.1999.72-205

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • OKOUCHI, H. (2003a). Stimulus generalization of behavioral history. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 80(2), 173–186. doi:10.1901/jeab.2003.80-173

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • OKOUCHI, H. (2003b). Effects of differences in interreinforcer intervals between past and current schedules on fixed-interval responding. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 79(1), 49–64. doi:10.1901/jeab.2003.79-49

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • OKOUCHI, H., & LATTAL, K. A. (2006). An analysis of reinforcement history effects. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 86(1), 31–42. doi:10.1901/jeab.2006.75-05

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • OTTO, T. L., TORGRUD, L. J., & HOLBORN, S. W. (1999). An operant blocking interpretation of instructed insensitivity to schedule contingencies. The Psychological Record, 49, 663–684.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ROSENFARB, I., & HAYES, S. C. (1984). Social standard setting: The Achilles heel of informational accounts of therapeutic change. Behavior Therapy, 15(5), 515–528. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(84)80053-2

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • ROSENFARB, I. S., NEWLAND, M. C., BRANNON, S. E., & HOWLEY, D. S. (1992). Effects of self-generated rules on the development of schedule-controlled behavior. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 58(1), 107–121. doi:10.1901/jeab.1992.58-107

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • SELIGMAN, M. E. P., ROSELLINI, R. A., & KOZAK, M. J. (1975). Learned helplessness in the rat: Time course, immunization and reversibility. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 88, 542–547.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • SOUZA, A. S., ABREU-RODRIGUES, J., & BAUMANN, A. A. (2010). History effects on induced and operant variability. Learning and Behavior, 38(4), 426–437. doi:10.3758/LB.38.4.426

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • TORGRUD, L. J., HOLBORN, S. W., & ZAK, R. D. (2006). Determinants of human fixed interval performance following varied exposure to reinforcement schedules. The Psychological Record, 56(1), 105–133. Retrieved from http://opensiuc.lib.siu.edu/trp/vol56/iss1/7

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1964). Conditioning history and human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 7(5), 383–385. doi:10.1901/jeab.1964.7-383

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WEINER, H. (1969). Controlling human fixed-interval performance. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 12(3), 349–373. doi:10.1901/jeab.1969.12-349

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  • WILLIAMS, K., HARKINS, S., & LATANÉ, B. (1981). Identifiability as a deterrent to social loafing: Two cheering experiments. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(2), 303–311. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.40.2.303

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • YANO, Y., & HUNZIKER, M. H. L. (2000). Desamparo aprendido e imunizagao com diferentes respostas de fuga [Learned helplessness and immunization with different escape responses]. Acta Comportamentalia, 8, 143–166. Retrieved from http://132.247.146.34/index.php/acom/article/view/18195

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Andréia Kroger-Costa.

Additional information

This article is part of a master’s thesis submitted by the first author to the Universidade de Brasilia under the guidance of the second author. It was partially supported by a scholarship provided to the first author by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES).

The authors thank Fernando Rocha for developing the computer software.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Kroger-Costa, A., Abreu-Rodrigues, J. Effects of Historical and Social Variables on Instruction Following. Psychol Rec 62, 691–706 (2012). https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395829

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03395829

Keyword

Navigation