Skip to main content
Log in

Categories of Illustrated Problems for Training Children in Inductive Reasoning

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Klauer and Phye’s Cognitive Training for Children (Cognitive training for children: a developmental program of inductive reasoning and problem solving. Hogrefe & Hogrefe Publisher, Kirkland, 1994) provides instruction in inductive reasoning through a sequence of 120 illustrations following a prescribed two-way categorization (a) attributes of objects versus relations between objects, and (b) similarities or differences versus both similarities and differences in attributes or relations. While the program’s effectivity has been established, its prescribed categorization of problems has yet to be validated. If training performance is in accordance with the prescribed categorization, then performance patterns should be more similar for problems in the same than in different categories. In the current research, correlations of performance between problem categories were used as similarity measures in multidimensional scaling. The resulting solution yielded the attribute–relation and similarity–difference dimensions thus showing that performance reflects problem complexity. Visual salience, however, may override problem complexity, as suggested by the finding that the matrix arrangement of objects facilitated training in the algorithmically complex similarity-and-difference problems. The use of everyday-life objects as opposed to abstract objects also was shown to facilitate inductive reasoning.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2
Fig. 3
Fig. 4

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Barkl, S., Porter, A., & Ginns, P. (2012). Cognitive training for children: Effects on inductive reasoning, deductive reasoning, and mathematics achievement in an Australian school setting. Psychology in the Schools, 49, 828–843. doi:10.1002/pits.21638.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Ben-Hur, M., & Feuerstein, R. (2011). Feuerstein’s new program for the facilitation of cognitive development in young children. Journal of Cognitive Education and Psychology, 10, 224–237.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: A critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Cattell, R. B., & Horn, J. L. (1978). A check on the theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence with description of new subtest designs. Journal of Educational Measurement, 15(3), 139–164.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Christou, C., & Papageorgiou, E. (2007). A framework of mathematics inductive reasoning. Learning and Instruction, 17, 55–66. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.11.009.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Csapó, B. (1997). The development of inductive reasoning: Cross-sectional assessments in an educational context. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 20, 609–626.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • de Koning, E., Hamers, J. H. M., Sijtsma, K., & Vermeer, A. (2002). Teaching inductive reasoning in primary education. Developmental Review, 22, 211–241. doi:10.1006/drev.2002.0548.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • DeLoache, J. S., & Sharon, T. (2005). Symbols and similarity: You can get too much of a good thing. Journal of Cognition and Development, 6, 33–49.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Demetriou, A., & Kyriakides, L. (2006). The functional and developmental organization of cognitive developmental sequences. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(2), 209–242. doi:10.1348/000709905X43256.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Diekhoff, G. (1992). Statistics for the social and behavioral sciences: univariate, bivariate, multivariate. Dubuque, IA: Wm. C. Brown Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fisher, A. V. (2005). Inductive generalization: Underlying mechanisms and developmental course (Doctoral dissertation). https://etd.ohiolink.edu/rws_etd/document/get/osu1117039741/inlinet

  • Frostig, M., Lefever, D. W., & Whittlesey, J. R. (1961). A developmental test of visual perception for evaluating normal and neurologically handicapped children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 12, 383–394.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gelman, S. A., & Markman, E. M. (1986). Categories and induction in young children. Cognition, 23, 183–209.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Gentner, D., & Medina, J. (1998). Similarity and the development of rules. Cognition, 65, 263–297.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hager, W., & Hasselhorn, M. (1998). The effectiveness of the cognitive training for children from a differential perspective: A meta-evaluation. Learning and Instruction, 8, 411–438.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Hamers, J. H. M., de Koning, E., & Sijtsma, K. (1998). Inductive reasoning in third grade: Intervention promises and constraints. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 23, 132–148.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Handley, S. J., Capon, A., Beveridge, M., Dennis, I., & Evans, J. S. B. (2004). Working memory, inhibitory control and the development of children’s reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 10, 175–195.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heit, E., & Hahn, U. (2001). Diversity-based reasoning in children. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 243–273. doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0757.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heit, E., & Rubinstein, J. (1994). Similarity and property effects in inductive reasoning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 411–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Jaworska, N., & Chupetlovska-Anastasova, A. (2009). A review of multidimensional scaling (MDS) and its utility in various psychological domains. Tutorials in Quantitative Methods for Psychology, 5, 1–10.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, R. A., & Wichern, D. W. (1992). Applied multivariate statistical analysis. Englewood, NJ: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. J. (1985). Framework for a theory of teaching. Teaching and Teacher Education, 1, 5–17.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. J., & Phye, G. D. (1994). Cognitive training for children: A developmental program of inductive reasoning and problem solving. Kirkland, WA: Hogrefe & Hogrefe Publisher.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. J., & Phye, G. D. (2008). Inductive reasoning: A training approach. Review of Educational Research, 78, 85–123. doi:10.3102/0034654307313402.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klauer, K. J., Willmes, K., & Phye, G. D. (2002). Inducing inductive reasoning: Does it transfer to fluid intelligence? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 1–25. doi:10.1006/ceps.2001.1079.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kruskal, J. B. (1964). Multidimensional scaling by optimizing goodness of fit to a non-metric hypothesis. Psychometrika, 29(1), 115–129.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kuhn, D., & Franklin, S. (2008). The second decade: What develops (and how)? In W. Damon & R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Child and adolescent development: An advanced course (pp. 517–550). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leevers, H. J., & Harris, P. L. (1999). Persisting effects of instruction on young children’s syllogistic reasoning with incongruent and abstract premises. Thinking and Reasoning, 5, 145–173.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Maslow, P., Frostig, M., Lefever, D. W., & Whittlesey, J. R. (1964). The Marianne Frostig developmental test of visual perception. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 19, 463–499.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Mayer, D., Sodian, B., Koerber, S., & Schwippert, K. (2014). Scientific reasoning in elementary school children: Assessment and relations with cognitive abilities. Learning and Instruction, 29, 43–55. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2013.07.005.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, K. S. (2005). The Cattell–Horn–Carroll theory of cognitive abilities: Past, present, and future. In D. P. Flanagan & P. L. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (pp. 136–181). NY: Guilford Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGrew, K. S., & Wendling, B. J. (2010). Cattell–Horn–Carroll cognitive-achievement relations: What we have learned from the past 20 years of research. Psychology in the Schools, 47(7), 651–675. doi:10.1002/pits.20497.

    Google Scholar 

  • Molnár, G. (2011). Playful fostering of 6- to 8-year-old students’ inductive reasoning. Thinking Skills and Creativity, 6, 91–99. doi:10.1016/j.tsc.2011.05.002.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Moseley, D., Elliott, J., Gregson, M., & Higgins, S. (2005). Thinking skills framework for use in education in training. British Educational Research Journal, 31, 367–390. doi:10.1080/01411920500082219.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Perkins, D. N., & Salomon, G. (1992). Transfer of learning. In International encyclopedia of education (Vol. 2). Oxford: Pergamon Press.

  • Raven, J. C., Court, J. H., & Raven, J. (1995). Manual for Raven’s progressive matrices and vocabulary scales. Oxford: Oxford Psychologists Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Roberts, M. J., Welfare, H., Livermore, D. P, I. V., & Theadom, A. M. (2000). Context, visual salience, and inductive reasoning. Thinking and Reasoning, 6, 349–374.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Roth-van der Werf, T., Resing, W., & Slenders, A. P. (2002). Task similarity and transfer of an inductive reasoning training. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 296–325. doi:10.1006/ceps.2001.1096.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sternberg, R. J. (1986). Toward a unified theory of human reasoning. Intelligence, 10, 281–314.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tomic, W., & Klauer, K. J. (1996). On the effects of training inductive reasoning: How far does it transfer and how long do the effects persist? European Journal of Psychology of Education, 11, 283–299.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunteler, E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2007). Effects of prior assistance in using analogies on young children’s unprompted analogical problem solving over time: A microgenetic study. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 43–67.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tunteler, E., Pronk, C. M. E., & Resing, W. C. M. (2008). Inter- and intra-individual variability in the process of change in the use of analogical strategies to solve geometric tasks in children: A microgenetic analysis. Learning and Individual Differences, 18, 44–60.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Watters, J. J., & English, L. D. (1995). Children’s application of simultaneous and successive processing in inductive and deductive reasoning problems: Implications for developing scientific reasoning skills. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 32, 699–714.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Woodman, G. F., Vecera, S. P., & Luck, S. J. (2003). Perceptual organization influences visual working memory. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 10, 80–87.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Colleges of Education and Liberal Arts of De La Salle University and by the Exaltacion C. Ramos Professorial Chair in Tests and Measurement. We acknowledge the contribution of Denise Mitzi Roman, Hazel Joy Marie Ordenes, and Maila Q. Castro in training the participants and of Maria Ruteza A. Bautista in the data management. We thank Allan B. I. Bernardo and Encarnita D. Balayon for the administrative support.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Melissa Lopez Reyes.

Appendices

Appendix 1

Table 5 Mean percentages of problems solved without help and of problems not solved despite help for boys and girls across problem categories and illustration types (with standard deviation)

Appendix 2

Table 6 Mean percentages of problems solved without help and of problems not solved despite help for the different ages across problem categories and illustration types (with standard deviation)

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Reyes, M.L., Amarnani, R.K. Categories of Illustrated Problems for Training Children in Inductive Reasoning. Asia-Pacific Edu Res 25, 239–250 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0257-y

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0257-y

Keywords

Navigation