Skip to main content
Log in

The Effects of Learning from Correct and Erroneous Examples in Individual and Collaborative Settings

  • Regular Article
  • Published:
The Asia-Pacific Education Researcher Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Research indicates that learning from erroneous examples (EE) is superior to correct examples because errors may provide students with a stimulus to spontaneously produce more self-explanations, leading to better learning outcomes. However, because most studies were conducted in individual settings, it remains an open question whether the advantage of EE could be reproduced in collaborative settings. Therefore, this study used a 2 × 2 factorial design varying in example types (correct vs. erroneous examples) and learning settings (individuals vs. collaboration). An experimental study of a 2-week course on subtraction for third grade students was conducted (N = 109). Results indicated that collaborative learning may enhance the transfer knowledge of subtraction. The improvements were still maintained over 2 weeks. In addition, collaboration also facilitated students’ confidence in handling errors. However, there was no significant difference in promoting isomorphic and transfer knowledge between learning from correct and erroneous examples. Additionally, this study also explored the absence of the intended benefit of erroneous examples from a cognitive load perspective.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Fig. 1
Fig. 2

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Adams, D. M., McLaren, B. M., Durkin, K., Mayer, R. E., Rittle-Johnson, B., Isotani, S., et al. (2014). Using erroneous examples to improve mathematics learning with a web-based tutoring system. Computers in Human Behavior, 36, 401–411.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Beishuizen, M. (1993). Mental strategies and materials or models for addition and subtraction up to 100 in Dutch second grades. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 24(4), 294–323.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Booth, J. L., Lange, K. E., Koedinger, K. R., & Newton, K. J. (2013). Using example problems to improve student learning in algebra: Differentiating between correct and incorrect examples. Learning and Instruction, 25, 24–34.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Bridger, E., & Mecklinger, A. (2014). Errorful and errorless learning: The impact of cue–target constraint in learning from errors. Memory & Cognition, 42(6), 898–911.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Brown, J. S., & VanLehn, K. (1980). Repair Theory: A generative theory of bugs in procedural skills. Cognitive Science, 4(4), 379–426.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Catrambone, R., & Holyoak, K. J. (1989). Overcoming contextual limitations on problem-solving transfer. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 15(6), 1147.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chen, M.-P., Wong, Y.-T., & Wang, L.-C. (2014). Effects of type of exploratory strategy and prior knowledge on middle school students’ learning of chemical formulas from a 3D role-playing game. Educational Technology Research and Development, 62(2), 163–185.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Chi, M. T. H., Bassok, M., Lewis, M. W., Reimann, P., & Glaser, R. (1989). Self-explanations: How students study and use examples in learning to solve problems. Cognitive Science, 13(2), 145–182.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Curry, L. A. (2004). The effects of self-explanations of correct and incorrect solutions on algebra problem-solving performance. Paper presented at the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Chicago.

  • Durkin, K., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2012). The effectiveness of using incorrect examples to support learning about decimal magnitude. Learning and Instruction, 22(3), 206–214.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. S. (1996). Implicit theories as organizers of goals and behavior. In P. Gollwitzer, & J. A. Bargh (Eds.), The Psychology of Action: Linking Cognition and Motivation to Behavior (pp. 69–90): Guilford.

  • Fiori, C., & Zuccheri, L. (2005). An experimental research on error patterns in written subtraction. Educational Studies in Mathematics, 60(3), 323–331.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning and Instruction, 17(6), 612–634.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heemsoth, T., & Heinze, A. (2014). The impact of incorrect examples on learning fractions: A field experiment with 6th grade students. Instructional Science, 42(4), 639–657.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Heitzmann, N. (2014). Fostering diagnostic competence in different domains. Dissertation, LMU Munich,

  • Isotani, S., Adams, D., Mayer, R. E., Durkin, K., Rittle-Johnson, B., & McLaren, B. M. (2011). Can erroneous examples help middle-school students learn decimals? Paper presented at the 6th European Conference On Technology Enhanced Learning, Palermo.

  • Kerr, N. L., & Bruun, S. E. (1981). Ringelmann revisited: Alternative explanations for the social loafing effect. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 7(2), 224–231.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kirschner, F., Paas, F., & Kirschner, P. A. (2009). A cognitive load approach to collaborative learning: United brains for complex tasks. Educational Psychology Review, 21(1), 31–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Klopp, E., Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (2013). Psychological factors affecting medical students’ learning with erroneous worked examples. Journal of Education and Learning, 2(1), 158–170.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Kollar, I., Ufer, S., Reichersdorfer, E., Vogel, F., Fischer, F., & Reiss, K. (2014). Effects of collaboration scripts and heuristic worked examples on the acquisition of mathematical argumentation skills of teacher students with different levels of prior achievement. Learning and Instruction, 32, 22–36.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Leppink, J., Paas, F., van Gog, T., van der Vleuten, C. P. M., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2014). Effects of pairs of problems and examples on task performance and different types of cognitive load. Learning and Instruction, 30, 32–42.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Litzler, E., Samuelson, C., & Lorah, J. (2014). Breaking it down: Engineering student STEM confidence at the intersection of race/ethnicity and gender. Research in Higher Education, 55(8), 810–832.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McEldoon, K. L., Durkin, K. L., & Rittle-Johnson, B. (2013). Is self-explanation worth the time? A comparison to additional practice. British Journal of Educational Psychology, 83(4), 615–632.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • McLaren, B., Adams, D., Durkin, K., Goguadze, G., Mayer, R., Rittle-Johnson, B., et al. (2012). To err is human, to explain and correct is divine: A study of interactive erroneous examples with middle school math students. Paper presented at the 7th European Conference on Technology Enhanced Learning, Saarbrücken.

  • Melis, E., Sander, A., & Tsovaltzi, D. (2010). How to support meta-cognitive skills for finding and correcting errors? Paper presented at the AAAI Fall Symposium: Cognitive and Metacognitive Educational Systems, Arlington

  • Moreno, R., Reisslein, M., & Ozogul, G. (2009). Optimizing worked-example instruction in electrical engineering: The role of fading and feedback during problem-solving practice. Journal of Engineering Education, 98(1), 83–92.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • O’Donnell, A. M. (2006). The role of peers and group learning. In P. A. Alexander & P. H. Winne (Eds.), Handbook of Educational Psychology (pp. 781–802). Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peltenburg, M., van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., & Doig, B. (2009). Mathematical power of special-needs pupils: An ICT-based dynamic assessment format to reveal weak pupils’ learning potential. British Journal of Educational Technology, 40(2), 273–284.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Quilici, J. L., & Mayer, R. (1996). Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88(1), 144–161.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (1999). Learning mathematics from worked-out examples: Analyzing and fostering self-explanations. European Journal of Psychology of Education, 14(4), 477–488.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2002). Worked-out examples: Instructional explanations support learning by self-explanations. Learning and Instruction, 12(5), 529–556.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Renkl, A. (2014). Toward an instructionally oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38(1), 1–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Retnowati, E., Ayres, P., & Sweller, J. (2010). Worked example effects in individual and group work settings. Educational Psychology, 30(3), 349–367.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Rybowiak, V., Garst, H., Frese, M., & Batinic, B. (1999). Error orientation questionnaire (EOQ): Reliability, validity, and different language equivalence. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 20(4), 527–547.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Safadi, R., & Yerushalmi, E. (2014). Problem solving vs. troubleshooting tasks: The case of sixth-grade students studying simple electric circuits. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 12(6), 1341–1366.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Siegler, R. S., & Chen, Z. (2008). Differentiation and integration: Guiding principles for analyzing cognitive change. Developmental Science, 11(4), 433–448.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Stark, R., Kopp, V., & Fischer, M. R. (2011). Case-based learning with worked examples in complex domains: Two experimental studies in undergraduate medical education. Learning and Instruction, 21(1), 22–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J. (2010). Element interactivity and intrinsic, extraneous, and germane cognitive load. Educational Psychology Review, 22(2), 123–138.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition and Instruction, 2(1), 59–89.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Sweller, J., van Merrienboer, J. G., & Paas, F. W. C. (1998). Cognitive architecture and instructional design. Educational Psychology Review, 10(3), 251–296.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  • Tsovaltzi, D., Melis, E., McLaren, B. M., Dietrich, M., Goguadze, G., & Meyer, A.-K. (2009). Erroneous examples: A preliminary investigation into learning benefits. Paper presented at the 4th European Conference On Technology Enhanced Learning, Nice.

  • Webb, N. M. (1991). Task-related verbal interaction and mathematics learning in small groups. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 22(5), 366–389.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgments

This study was supported by the National Social Science Fund of China (14BGL131) and the National Key Technology R&D Program of China (Grant No. 2013BAH72B01).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Meng Wang.

Appendices

Appendix 1

See Table 2.

Table 2 The error types and the examples of corresponding erroneous examples

Appendix 2

See Table 3.

Table 3 The questionnaire of confidence and attitude toward errors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Yang, Zk., Wang, M., Cheng, H.N.H. et al. The Effects of Learning from Correct and Erroneous Examples in Individual and Collaborative Settings. Asia-Pacific Edu Res 25, 219–227 (2016). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0253-2

Download citation

  • Published:

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s40299-015-0253-2

Keywords

Navigation