Introduction

More than 30 years ago, Orton (1987) already claimed that “it might be that problems of vocabulary are considered to be fairly superficial within the whole issue of language and mathematics learning, but it is nevertheless critical that such problems are not ignored in the hope they will go away” (p. 127). This is even more important due to the cognitive and epistemic function of language in general and of the language of schooling in particular, which can be challenging for many students (Schleppegrell 2004). These challenges are relevant with respect to all school subjects, including mathematics (OECD 2007), as well as in multilingual contexts (Planas and Civil 2013).

However, the systematic specification of topic-specific language demands still remains a major goal that has yet to be realized. On the lexical level, this relates to the required key vocabulary for the different mathematical topics in order to better understand and define the topic-specific school academic-language demands. This study makes a contribution towards this goal in the mathematical topic of equivalent fractions. The learning processes from an overarching content- and language-integrated intervention study are investigated in order to understand the role and use of vocabulary in students’ learning processes towards understanding equivalent fractions. More specifically, the vocabulary offered in the teaching material and the activated lexical means of the students are inventoried and analyzed qualitatively in relation to the individual conceptual learning pathways towards equivalent fractions. Thus, this paper contributes to the important overall research question (to be refined in the “Equivalent fractions: mental models, representations, and registers” section): Which lexical demands appear and how are they interrelated with processes of developing conceptual understanding of equivalent fractions?

To approach this overall research question, the theoretical and empirical background on language in mathematics is introduced in the “Theoretical background: school academic language” section with a focus on vocabulary teaching and learning. The theoretical background for the field of fractions and related aspects of language are sketched in the “Equivalent fractions: mental models, representations, and registers” section. The “Research context and design” section outlines the research context and the methodology of the study. The “Students’ use of vocabulary along conceptual learning pathways” section presents selected results of the qualitative (the “Specifying students’ language demands on the basis of lexical trace analyses” section) and quantitative (the “Quantitative comparisons of self-initiated and triggered vocabulary” section) analyses, while the “Discussion” section relates and discusses the empirical findings with respect to their implications and methodological restrictions.

Theoretical background: school academic language

In this section, the theoretical background with respect to language and its challenges in mathematical learning processes is presented. A focus is put on the teaching and learning of vocabulary in mathematics and the research aims in this field.

School academic language in mathematics teaching and learning

In some countries, researchers have investigated the mathematics achievement gap between students with diverse language backgrounds for more than 25 years (Secada 1992), and the attention to language gaps is currently increasing (OECD 2007; Haag et al. 2013). In the German context, a variety of large-scale studies have replicated the language gap for mathematics achievement (Heinze et al. 2009; Prediger et al. 2015b). More precisely, research has shown that many students with low language proficiency encounter other serious obstacles beyond reading challenges, especially when high conceptual demands are required (Ufer et al. 2013). That is why, in addition to the communicative role of language, its epistemic role in processes of knowledge construction as a medium of thinking (Heller and Morek 2015; Vygotsky 1978) also has to be taken into account. As a consequence, mathematics education researchers’ and mathematics teachers’ awareness that “the chief cause of the achievement gap between socio-economic groups is a language gap” (Hirsch 2003, p. 22)—more precisely a school academic-language gap—constantly increases.

Bailey (2007) summarizes academic-language proficiency as the knowing and being able to:

use general and content-specific vocabulary, specialized or complex grammatical structures, and multifarious language functions and discourse structures—all for the purpose of acquiring new knowledge and skills, interacting about a topic, or imparting information to others.

(Bailey 2007, p. 10). Here, a differentiation is made not only between the communicative and epistemic role of (academic) language but also (1) between a rather general academic-language proficiency needed across different subjects and the specific academic language needed for a particular subject, and (2) between the different features of academic language on the lexical, syntactical, and discursive levels. These levels are also used by other linguists to characterize the general differences between everyday language and school academic language (e.g., on the lexical level, by specialized vocabulary, composite or unfamiliar words, and specific connectors; on the syntactical level, by long and syntactically complex sentences, passive voice constructions, and long noun phrases and prepositional phrases; and on the discursive level, by specific discursive practices such as arguing or explaining why; Bailey 2007; Heller and Morek 2015).

So far, many studies have compared the mathematics performance of (a) students who speak the language of instruction as a second language (in English studies often referred to as English language learners (ELL)) with the performance of proficient speakers of the language of instruction or (b) monolingual with multilingual students (for an overview, see Haag et al. 2013). Though their results are not fully consistent, the studies suggest that “descriptive, lexical, and grammatical features of test items may influence the performance of second language learners” (ibid., p. 26). Of course, monolingual students and students with the language of instruction as their first language are also academic-language learners. Thus, in this study, we speak of language learners, which refer not only to second-language learners but to all students with low academic-language proficiency in the language of instruction (Planas and Civil 2013, for politically mediated tensions in multilingual settings of mathematics teaching and learning).

School academic language on the lexical level and its relation to discourse

Lexical learning aims at extending the students’ individual mental lexicons. A mental lexicon functions as a mental web of vocabulary, which constantly changes when new lexical means, their meanings, and information on spelling, pronunciation, or usage in phrases are added (Aitchison 2003). To develop understanding of a mathematical concept, of course, in addition to acquiring words and phrases, students need to construct the meaning of the concept (semantic area) and learn for which problems and purposes it is applied (pragmatic area; Schleppegrell 2004). Together, these are the facets that make up conceptual understanding of a mathematical concept, which is operationalized as mental models (Prediger 2008) and involves the construction of new mental objects and relationships (Steinbring 2005) in contrast to technical, procedural skills that can be carried out without deeper understanding.

In research contributions on academic language, the relevance of lexical means is often discussed in relation to the discursive level of academic language, while the latter is regarded to be more important and more challenging than the former. Relying on the seminal work by Halliday (1978), O’Halloran (2015) subsumes that “the difficulty of scientific language is the larger theoretical framework within which technical terms are defined and the metaphorical nature of the discourse of argumentation in which these terms are used, rather than individual terms themselves” (p. 65). With respect to designing classroom activities, Moschkovich also concludes that we need to think of “how instruction can best support students to learn vocabulary as they actively engage in mathematical reasoning about important mathematical topics” (Moschkovich 2013, p. 46f).

On the other hand, there is empirical evidence indicating that for meeting discursive demands, students need particular lexical means. Quasthoff (2012) distinguishes more demanding discourse practices such as “explaining” or “arguing” from less demanding ones such as “reporting” or “describing.” She characterizes the first kind as lexically more demanding because more elaborated lexical means are needed in order to make connections and conclusions explicit. There is also empirical evidence that the development of discourse competence is related to a differentiated mental lexicon (see Grundler 2009). Analyses of small-group discussions indicate that oral and written argumentative units of discourse are strongly related to the vocabulary of students. For her analyses, the author classifies (ibid., p. 85) students’ self-initiated lexical means as autonomous vocabulary; in contrast, the collective vocabulary is established by teaching material or previous lexical means in the local discourse under investigation. By inventorying the autonomous and collective vocabulary on the basis of transcripts, the author observed that a high level of autonomous vocabulary goes along with complex lines of argumentation. In argumentative settings, students with a high level of autonomous vocabulary have been identified as those speakers who stand out, since they put forward the argument (ibid., p. 91).

One instructional approach for implementing vocabulary teaching in rich discursive settings is the Sheltered Instruction Observation Protocol (SIOP) (Short 2017). The SIOP model suggests teaching strategies such as the use of concept definition maps, word banks, sentence starters, and sentence frames (for an overview of strategies, see Fisher and Frey 2008; Donelly and Roe 2010; Riccomini et al. 2015). However, there has been limited research that focuses specifically on interventions such as these vocabulary teaching strategies and their effects on developing vocabulary in mathematics (Riccomini et al. 2015, p. 247) or, more importantly for mathematics educators, on developing an understanding of mathematical concepts.

To implement lexical teaching strategies, it is crucial to know what exactly constitutes the key vocabulary for a specific mathematical topic. Potential rather general pitfalls of the mathematical register have been well described theoretically for both English (e.g., overview in O’Halloran 2015; Thompson and Rubenstein 2000; Pimm 1987) and German (see Niederhaus et al. 2016). For the German language, connectors such as prepositional phrases or conjunctions have been identified as crucial for mathematical learning, especially for developing a conceptual understanding (Prediger and Zindel 2017). One explanation for this phenomenon draws upon the specifics of mathematics: “The grammatical patterning of mathematics often presents processes as if they were things by construing them as nouns and noun phrases” (Schleppegrell 2007, p.146). To unfold the processes and ideas encapsulated in noun phrases, prepositions, and more complex sentence structures are often needed (see Prediger 2011a, for the case of the preposition “out of” (German “von”)). It has to be acknowledged though that this research has been carried out for English and German, each with a specific grammar relying on the use of prepositions. Different results are expected for other languages which, for example, rely on case marking. In any case, topic-specific empirical research that specifies key vocabulary needed for developing conceptual understanding is still required for many topics (exceptions Pöhler and Prediger 2015, for percentages; Prediger and Zindel 2017, for functions).

Equivalent fractions: mental models, representations, and registers

In the following section, equivalent fractions as the mathematical topic under investigation is outlined (the “Mental models of equivalent fractions” section). An additional focus is put on relating registers and representations as theoretical background and core design principle for the design of the learning opportunities (the “Representations and registers for understanding mathematical concepts” section), which are presented in the “Learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions and typical student challenges” section.

Mental models of equivalent fractions

The development of conceptual understanding is a central goal of mathematics instruction (for the different strands of mathematical competence, see Kilpatrick et al. 2001). Fractions were chosen as the specific mathematical topic for the larger intervention since fractions are one of the most difficult topics in the middle school curriculum, especially if the aim is developing a conceptual understanding (Aksu 1997). The latter is encapsulated in the construct of rational number sense:

Students who have developed rational number sense have an intuitive feel for the relative sizes of rational numbers and the ability to estimate, to think qualitatively and multiplicatively, to solve proportions and to solve problems, to move flexibly between interpretations and representations, to make sense, and to make sound decisions and reasonable judgments.

(Lamon 2007, p. 636). To understand and reason the equivalence of fractions, the structural understanding of relating two fractions is crucial (Prediger 2013). In addition, Lamon (2007) reveals critical components that underlie multiplicative thinking, especially relative thinking, unitizing, reasoning up and down, sharing, and comparing, as well as norming. These aspects are important when specifying the relevant mental models of equivalent fractions in different representations and interpretations of fractions (for the construct of mental models see Prediger 2008):

  • In part-whole interpretations, equivalent fractions are found by thinking of the parts and wholes in terms of larger or smaller chunks. To develop conceptual understanding, context situations involving sharing are often applied, e.g., “A cake is cut in different ways, 3 out of 4 equal pieces is as much cake as __ out of 8 (12, 16, ...) equal pieces?” Here, the comparison of two fractions relies on the mental model “as much as,” e.g., “3 out of 4 equal pieces is as much as 6 out of 8 equal pieces.” This mental model draws upon the critical components unitizing and reasoning up and down.

  • Beyond the part-whole interpretation, the concept of ratio (a relationship between a pair of quantities) is fundamental to fractions (Lamon 2007). Comparisons of ratio and rates, for example in contexts of sports competitions, serve to develop conceptual understanding of equivalent fractions (Prediger 2013). Here, a comparison relies on the mental model “as good as,” e.g., a rate of 3 goals out of 4 attempts is as good as a rate of 6 goals out of 8 attempts. This mental model draws upon the critical components relative thinking and reasoning up and down.

  • To interpret equivalent fractions in a graphical representation, a structural understanding and the critical components of unitizing and norming are important. In fraction bars of equal length, equivalent fractions are found by vertical comparison (see Fig. 1). Fractions for which the colored part in the bar is equally long are equivalent. An explanation for this equivalence relies on the mental models of refining and coarseningFootnote 1 which can be developed when the structure of both fraction bars is related to each other. From 6/8 to 3/4, the fraction is simplified, but the fraction bar structure is coarsened: Every two pieces in the bar of eighths become one piece in the bar of fourths while the colored part stays the same. From 6/8 to 12/16 the fraction bar structure is refined: Every piece in the bar of eighths is divided by two and thus becomes two pieces in the bar of sixteenths while the colored part stays the same (“as long as”).

Fig. 1
figure 1

Equivalent fractions in graphical representation

Representations and registers for understanding mathematical concepts

Due to the specific ontological nature of mathematical objects as abstract and mostly relational entities, a mathematical concept such as fraction cannot be understood just by referring to real objects. This is why mathematics educators (Duval 2006; Lesh 1979; Clarkson 2009) and psychologists (Bruner 1967) have emphasized the importance of multiple representations. By transitioning between verbal, symbolic, graphical, and concrete representations, students can construct the mental objects and relations to which a mathematical concept refers (Lesh 1979; Cramer et al. 1997).

For the theoretical underpinning of the construct of registers, this study draws on Halliday’s definition of the register as “set of meanings, the configuration of semantic patterns, that are typically drawn upon under the specific conditions, along with the words and structures” (Halliday 1978, p. 23). He emphasizes the social embeddedness: “A register can be defined as the configuration of semantic resources that a member of a culture typically associates with the situation type … in a given social context” (ibid., p. 111). Hence, for Halliday, registers are characterized by the types of communication situations, their field of language use, the discourse styles, and modes of discourse. Drawing on Halliday’s theory, O’Halloran (2015) develops a multimodal approach to the mathematics register (as a multimodal register) in which language operates in conjunction with symbolism and visual representations to create meanings. By doing so, she extends the understanding of language by also taking into account functions and grammatical features of graphical representations (in her wording “images”) and mathematical symbolism. Duval (2006) also gives different mathematical representations a status of different “semiotic registers” and emphasizes that the meaning of a mathematical object can change with a shift in representation. In a social semiotic perspective, both can be subsumed as registers and the relating registers and representations approach was proven as a fruitful design strategy for integrating language and mathematics learning (Clarkson 2009; Prediger and Wessel 2013).

Coming back to the different registers of language, namely everyday, academic, and technical registers (see the “Theoretical background: school academic language” section), empirical research implies that the language registers themselves need to be refined, because the registers do not grasp the mathematics-specific subtleties sufficiently: Based upon their theoretical and textbook analyses, Prediger and Zindel (2017, p. 4163), in their study for dealing with word problems in the field of functions, specify “explaining the meaning of concepts” as the most important discursive language demands in the development of conceptual understanding. To meet this discursive demand, very specific lexical means, in the following subsumed in the basic meaning-related vocabulary, is needed. These are words and phrases that grasp mathematical relations and meanings (e.g., “take away” that is often used in situations of subtraction) which mostly belong to the academic school register (Pöhler and Prediger 2015; Wessel 2015). Examples are given in Table 1 for the part-whole interpretation of and operations with fractions.

Table 1 Refined model of representations and registers with sample representations and vocabulary.

In contrast, formal vocabulary is specified in mathematics curricula and textbooks. The formal vocabulary, as part of the more technical register, is often specified as an explicit learning goal. From an empirical research perspective, it is not yet clear how students acquire relevant vocabulary and how this relates to their mathematical learning. A case study by Zahner et al. (2012) relates mathematical learning gains of students to the vocabulary introduced by three teachers in a mathematics lesson on functions. For these analyses, the vocabulary was analyzed in-depth and students’ learning gains were measured with a curriculum-aligned assessment. The lesson in view was conceptually focused, and all three teachers used a similar set of terminologies. Two of the three teachers, whose students had relatively high gains, “both used informal language as a resource to introduce an important concept and then used the formal vocabulary term after the idea had been introduced into the discussion” (ibid., p. 438). In contrast, the third teacher, who was less successful in promoting students’ learning, “supplied the formal terminology in talk that appeared less carefully structured to bridge informal and formal ways of reasoning” (ibid., p. 438). The authors conclude “that learning conceptually demanding mathematics requires more than being exposed to technical vocabulary” (ibid., p. 438). Due to the small sample of three teachers and classrooms, these findings of course have to be interpreted carefully. Still, the findings indicate the need for topic-specific empirical research on lexical demands in learning processes towards understanding a certain mathematical concept.

Learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions and typical student challenges

The overarching intervention is based on the design principles of providing rich opportunities for language production, scaffolding, and the principle of relating registers and representations (for an overview see Schüler-Meyer et al. 2019). The intended conceptual learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions (see Wessel 2017) have been adapted from previous design research (Prediger 2013; Prediger and Wessel 2013) and start with students’ individual approaches and everyday experiences in comparing scored goals for an unequal number of attempts in a competitive situation.

In Task 1 (see Fig. 2), three fictitious groups with different numbers of attempts and scored goals in the context of a soccer competition are introduced. It then proceeds to constructing a meaning of the given data by introducing the fraction bar board (see Fig. 2, Task 2) which the students already know from the previous intervention lesson to represent fractions, so that they are already familiar with it at this point. The graphical representation is used to support the development of mental models of equivalence (see the “Mental models of equivalent fractions” section). Task 2 aims at elaborating students’ informal comparison strategies by focusing on the need for normed referent wholes (here fraction bars of normed length) and the necessity of including every group’s number of attempts (not only the number of scored goals).

Fig. 2
figure 2

Learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions translated from German to English (except for original German labels of fraction bars) and shortened (full fraction bar board includes fraction bars up to bars of twentieths)

According to previous research on fraction comparison strategies initiated by a similar task (Prediger 2011b), students’ first informal comparison strategy is often an absolute comparison with a focus only on the absolute number of scored goals. Accounting for the total number of attempts is a necessary step towards multiplicative comparison, for which “comparing with the fraction bar board” is a sustainable sample strategy (other sustainable multiplicative strategies are for example “comparing with one half” or “reasoning up and down,” cf. ibid.). Of course, there is an ambiguity in the task’s question “who shot best” about what “best” means in this context. This is one reason why an absolute comparison is also an interpretation of the question that is reasonable in a real-world sense.

Intended to initiate elaboration of the fraction bar comparison strategy, Task 2 aims at developing a structural understanding of the given rates and corresponding fractions bars. Structural understanding can become visible in processes of structural reasoning which is differentiated from empirical reasoning. Empirical reasoning denotes coming to a conclusion on the basis of empirical observations of representations or a small number of examples without attending to an underlying, explaining structure (see Schwarzkopf 2003, p. 231; Steinbring 1994; see also the taxonomy of proof by Balacheff 1988). Structural reasoning can also rest upon representations, concrete numbers, and examples, but they function to explain a more general finding in a more generic reasoning. Schwarzkopf (2003) describes this as a structural view behind the empirically found phenomenon (also see Moschkovich 2015 for the practice "attending to structure"). Prediger’s case studies (2011c; 2013) revealed that the conceptually relevant step towards structural understanding and reasoning can be challenging for students: “especially weaker students tend not to construct the mathematically intended structural relations automatically” (Prediger 2013, p. 343), so that these processes need more explicit support. This is one reason why, after the collaborative work and discussions in Task 2, students in Task 3 are individually asked to fulfill the discursive demand of giving reasons when writing down their answers to the question of “which group wins.”

Research questions

On the basis of the theoretical background and the research aims listed above, the empirical analyses of the learning processes initiated by the learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions are guided by the following research questions:

  1. Q1.

    Which task- or concept-specific lexical means do students use along the learning pathway and how do they relate to the development of conceptual understanding of equivalent fractions?

  2. Q2.

    To what extent do students self-initiate or take up lexical means from teaching materials, the teacher, or peers in local processes initiated by the presented learning opportunities?

Research context and design

In this section, methods for data collection and the sampling procedure for data analysis are presented. The data analysis is based on the approach of lexical trace analysis by Pöhler and Prediger (2015). Its modification for this study’s research focus is presented in the “Qualitative and quantitative methods for data analysis” section.

Design experiments as a method for data collection and sampling decisions

The research was conducted using a mixed-methods design in which topic-specific didactical design research and a quantitative intervention study are combined. Design experiments are considered the methodological core of design research studies as they allow in-depth investigations of learning processes rather than only learning outcomes (Cobb et al. 2003; Gravemeijer and Cobb 2006). They serve as a means of data gathering for the qualitative in-depth analyses of conceptual and lexical learning processes (Prediger et al. 2015a).

The design experiments were conducted and videotaped within the larger research project MesutFootnote 2 with N = 186 mathematically low-achieving students, aged 13 to 15, from comprehensive schools in North Rhine-Westphalia, Germany. The setting was small-group teaching (with 3–6 students per group, in total 38 groups taught by 16 teachers, who were either research team members or students in their final year of studies in a master program of mathematics education). All teachers took part in a 2-day training on the basic design principles, lesson plans, and task activities. The training consisted of video vignette analyses and collection of teaching experience with the teaching material in role plays.

For the qualitative in-depth analysis, six student groups (taught by four different teachers) have been selected according to students’ German academic-language proficiency (measured with a German C-test; Prediger and Wessel 2013) and language background (mono- or multilingual, operationalized by “speaks at least one language other than German with a parent or grandparent”) as well as richness of the initiated learning processes. The sampling aimed at a linguistically heterogeneous sub-sample to conduct case analyses and contrast different language backgrounds. The quantitative analysis draws upon the qualitative in-depth analysis (see “Qualitative and quantitative methods for data analysis” section).

Qualitative and quantitative methods for data analysis

To explore research questions Q1 and Q2, the six teacher-moderated small groups’ learning processes have been transcribed for the following learning opportunities and video data:

  • The learning opportunities towards understanding equivalent fractions (from the second lesson of the overarching intervention, tasks presented in the “Equivalent fractions: mental models, representations, and registers” section): 113 min of video data displaying work of n = 16 students. The qualitative in-depth case studies discussed in the “Students’ use of vocabulary along conceptual learning pathways” section draw upon this data.

  • Selected learning opportunities towards the part-whole interpretation of fraction (from the first lesson of the overarching intervention, see Prediger and Wessel (2013) for detailed description of task): 176 min of video data displaying work of n = 17 students (one student did not pursue taking part in the intervention after the first lesson).

In total, 298 min of video data have been transcribed for carrying out the following steps of analysis and which relate to the research questions as follows:

Step 1: Conceptual analysis

To analyze students’ conceptual development, task-specific categories have been applied that relate to Task 1 and Task 2 of the learning opportunities (see Fig. 2). Previous research by Prediger (2011b) provided comparison strategies (absolute and multiplicative) and mental models for fraction equivalence (as much as, as good as, as long as) which both serve as theoretically based categories for deductive conceptual analysis.

Step 2: Trace analysis

The methodological basis for identifying and contrasting lexical learning pathways builds a modification of the trace analysis developed by Prediger and Pöhler (2015, p. 1187). This analytical tool allows inventorying and tracing the students’ lexical means in three steps.

Preparation for Steps 2a and 2b

Based on all transcripts and conceptual analyses, two consolidated corpora of relevant lexical means (i.e., words and phrases; see appendix Table 8 for the equivalent fractions corpus) have been defined for the actual trace analyses for each of the two analyzed learning opportunities. The lexical means have been assigned to be included in the consolidated corpora by two researchers, who came to their decision after discussing the importance of a specific lexical mean for modeling and elaborating on the given situation in the task, and expressing important individual ideas related to conceptual understanding.

Step 2a

In line with Prediger and Pöhler (2015), those lexical means (words and phrases) of the consolidated corpus are inventoried along the tasks and transcripts. For this purpose, trace analysis tables (see Fig. 3) are used. Lexical means from the consolidated corpus are highlighted in the transcript and coded in the corresponding trace analysis tables according to Step 2b with one column for teacher and teaching material and one column for each student involved in the transcribed learning process. The completed trace analysis tables allow for keeping track of the micro-processes of students, teacher, and teaching material introducing and taking up lexical means (see the next step and Fig. 3).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Example of trace analysis table for a sample transcript (see the “Specifying students’ language demands on the basis of lexical trace analyses” section for transcript). The turn numbers in the far-left column refer to the transcript lines.

Step 2b

The inventoried lexical means are coded in the trace analysis table for the mode in which they are used (black print for oral, gray print for written use). The inventoried lexical means in Fig. 3 are all in black print, indicating that the inventoried vocabulary is used orally. Moreover, inventoried lexical means are coded for their trigger which indicates by whom and in which mode (oral or written (o/w)) a lexical mean was used just before the moment of usage in view:

  • Students self-initiate lexical means, indicated with (si) (which is in line with Prediger and Pöhler (2015) who used “teacher-initiated” as complement category), or

  • Students take up lexical means from either the teaching material (indicated with TM), the teacher (T), or other peers in the group (indicated by the first letter of students’ name).

The set of information “when,” “by whom,” and “mode” can be found in the trace analysis table in brackets for each inventoried lexical mean (see Fig. 3 and examples for each column at the top).

Step 2c

The results of Steps 2a and 2b form the database for an overall quantitative analysis for exploring research question Q2. For this purpose, numbers of inventoried lexical means are compared with expected numbers of lexical means in a chi-square test. This allows for interpreting and comparing the rates of the four coded categories (self-initiated vocabulary and vocabulary triggered by teaching material, teacher, or peers) in written and oral use. The database for this quantitative analysis comprises all transcribed video data (lexical trace analysis for 298 min of video data).

Accounting the self-initiated vocabulary as autonomous vocabulary and the vocabulary triggered by teaching material, teacher, or peers as part of the collective vocabulary (see the “School academic language on the lexical level and its relation to discourse” section) enables an analysis of the relation of autonomous and collective vocabulary. Of course, such an approach has its limitations: autonomous vocabulary relates to vocabulary as self-initiated in these specific local processes which are confined by the selected tasks. This implies that a student might have used a specific lexical mean in other situations beforehand (e.g., for data from the second lesson, in the first lesson). With this method of analysis, it is zoomed into specific excerpts of the interaction. Only for these specific excerpts, vocabulary is coded as self-initiated or triggered. Conversely, we blend out hypothetical previous processes of self-initiation which could have taken place before the analyzed learning process in view starts. Being aware of this limitation and interpretation, this method still provides insights into how students’ vocabulary is triggered in this very local perspective.

Students’ use of vocabulary along conceptual learning pathways

In this section, the research questions Q1 and Q2 are explored. While qualitative analyses are carried out to approach Q1 in the “Specifying students’ language demands on the basis of lexical trace analyses” section for equivalent fractions (see the “Qualitative and quantitative methods for data analysis” section for database), the “Quantitative comparisons of self-initiated and triggered vocabulary” section draws upon the lexical trace analyses of data from two lessons and presents a quantitative view at the data.

Specifying students’ language demands on the basis of lexical trace analyses

In this section, the focus is put on research question Q1 Which task- or concept-specific lexical means do students use along the learning pathway and how do they relate to the development of conceptual understanding towards equivalent fractions? By presenting selected moments and cases from the in-depth qualitative analyses, conclusions are drawn for an empirically informed specification of lexical means that have been evaluated as a key vocabulary for

  • developing conceptual understanding of equivalent fractions in teaching designs that draw upon activities of relating registers, and

  • meeting the discursive demand of structural reasoning why two rates (consequently two fractions) are equivalent.

For this purpose, the presented case studies already combine results of conceptual and trace analyses. In an intertwined way, key steps on the conceptual learning pathway are related to the identified lexical demands.

The case of Kiran introducing the “thereof” preposition for making part-whole relations explicit

In the analyzed data set, absolute and multiplicative comparison strategies have been identified. For overcoming absolute comparison strategies, the importance of the meaning-related prepositional phrases “__ out of __” (part stated first, whole stated second) and “__ thereof __” (whole stated first, part stated second) as relevant lexical means for relating parts and wholes had already become visible in previous case studies (Wessel 2017).

While the teaching material introduces the prepositional “__ out of __” construction in Task 1, in all six analyzed groups, the “__ thereof __” construction was also introduced by the students. For the presented case of Group X, it is Kiran who introduces this expression. The relevant excerpt of the learning process has been chosen for discussion because for this group, the trace analysis reveals that the other students in the group adopt the expression.

Kiran (multilingual, high language proficiency) works together with Makbule and Vehbiya. The excerpt in Table 2 shows how—when working on Task 2 of the learning opportunities—an argumentative unit of discourse is initiated by reflecting on why the chosen fraction bar of fifths fits the results of the boys (initiated by the teacher in Turn 56).

Table 2 Episode with Makbule (M), Kiran (K), Vehbiya (V), and Teacher (T): Kiran (K) introduces "thereof" construction.

In Turn 67, Kiran self-initiates the prepositional phrase from his autonomous vocabulary. The use of this phrase is part of Kirans’s answer to the teacher repeatedly asking “why does the fraction bar fit?” (Turns 56 and 59). Again, in Turn 63 the teacher asks the students to agree on one justification. So far, several explanations came up: “Because they [the boys] took five shots and have only scored four times” (Makbule in Turn 56 and again interrupted in Turn 64), “Because it’s four fifths” (Makbule, Turn 61), “In the fraction, it would be four fifths” (Kiran, Turn 60/62), “Because they [the boys] scored four goals” (Vehbiya, Turn 64).

Though the previous utterances of Makbule (Turn 64) and Vehbiya (Turn 66) refer to the context situation, namely the number of attempts (“they had five”) and scored goals (“they scored four goals”), their structural relation is not yet made explicit. Kiran (maybe assuming that the teacher is not yet satisfied with their arguments) lexically introduces a way for relating the number of attempts and number of scored goals more explicitly and precisely. To do so, he introduces the prepositional phrase “five shots and thereof only scored four” into the group’s collective vocabulary (in addition to the prepositional phrase introduced by the material). This phrase is identified as key vocabulary on the conceptual learning pathway because it puts emphasis on the total number of attempts. Linguistically, this emphasis is obtained by putting the whole (here: number of attempts) at the beginning of the phrase.

How Kiran’s peers take up the prepositional phrase “__ thereof __” in the following learning process (in written as well as oral mode) is shown in Table 3 for the case of Makbule (for details on conceptual development and a comparison of Makbule’s and Kiran’s inventories of vocabulary, see Wessel 2017).

Table 3 Episode with Makbule (M), Kiran (K), Vehbiya (V), and Teacher (T): Makbule (M) adopts "thereof" construction.

Table 3 gives a condensed insight into Makbules reasoning initiated by Task 2. The transcript is shortened to show how she flexibly applies both prepositional phrases (“__ out of __”, and “__ thereof __”). Interestingly, she uses “out of” for the part-whole interpretation of the fraction (Turn 76) and “thereof” for relating the whole and the part in the soccer context.

Looking at both episodes holistically, we see how, in this group, the students are repeatedly reasoning about the relations between the three teams’ rates (verbal register) and fraction bars as graphical representation (graphical register). In these processes of reasoning, the prepositional “__ thereof __” phrase seems to function as a lexical mean in the sense of language as a resource, because it stresses the conceptually relevant step of accounting for the number of attempts in order to fairly compare the three teams.

Table 4 Episode with Kibariye (Ki), Yagis (Y), and Teacher (T) initiated by teacher moves to relate registers 

Lexical demands for making structural relations within and between different registers explicit

The next case of two students, Yagis (multilingual, low language proficiency) and Kibariye (multilingual, low language proficiency) from Group P (see episode in Table 4), is chosen for showing how students can be challenged lexically and conceptually when relating the three registers (graphical, symbolic, verbal) as well as when talking about structural relations within the graphical register. The discourse is initiated by the teacher aiming at clarification whether one of the teams scored better than the others (Turn 95).

Kibariye shows approval in Turn 96 so that the teacher draws attention to the fraction bar board (Turn 97). It is again Kibariye who states that the fraction bar was used for comparison (Turn 98). Yagis extends this thought by explaining that “It’s the same” (Turn 100). “It” might refer to the colored part in the three fraction bars, and for each fraction bar, “it” is the same (see Fig. 4 for the picture of the fraction bar board). The teacher asks for clarification so that Yagis uses a gesture bound to the fraction bar board and states “this is all the same” (Turn 102). Still, Yagis’ explanation remains implicit (“this”), and only in Turn 106 he extends his idea and becomes more explicit: He identifies a structural specificity of the fraction bar of twentieths (probably in comparison to the bar of fifths, and tenths): “Well, in the bar of twentieths, it is more – well smaller (…).” Although “it” is still not explicated, it becomes clear that Yagis expresses a comparison: something being “more” and/or something being “smaller.” He is more explicit in the next part of the sentence, when he says that “the pieces are smaller.” Here, he refers to the pieces in the bar of twentieths compared with pieces in fraction bars “at the top” (Turn 106) of the fraction bar board, such as pieces in the bar of fifths.

Fig. 4
figure 4

Makbule colored 8/10 and 16/20 in the bar board

Yagis uses this insight for reasoning why “all three” (Turn 110) won: “They are all equal – only the pieces are just bigger for the boys and the girls.” (Turn 116; between 110 and 116 one student starts a private discussion that the teacher solves in these turns). In Turn 106, we saw Yagis struggling between the notions of number (more) and size (smaller), a typical challenge on the conceptual and lexical learning pathway: Talking about the number of pieces in a fraction bar demands different vocabulary from talking about the sizes of pieces. This reveals a lexical demand of differentiating the number of pieces from the size of pieces (when equivalent fractions are investigated in the fraction bar). Yagis discovered an important general structural relation in the fraction bar board (within the graphical register): the pieces are getting smaller when you move down (see Fig. 4). Moreover, he also explains a structural relation between two registers (graphical and verbal): The equal length of the colored part is important for coming to the conclusion that none of the teams wins the competition.

So far, lexical demands appeared when students were challenged by making structural relations explicit. The case of Makbule now shows a successful learning process of overcoming this challenge by self-initiating structure-related lexical means. In contrast to this successful case, after that we also look at the case of Noel, Sarah, and their teacher who misses a chance to make structural relations explicit.

The case of Makbule introducing structure-related lexical means for understanding equivalent fractions

The learning processes in all analyzed groups have an important step on the conceptual learning pathway in common: they all accomplish empirical reasoning on the basis of observations drawn from the fraction bar board and come to the conclusion that no group scored better than another. The case of Makbule (multilingual, low language proficiency) is exceptional, because she self-initiates vocabulary from her individual everyday register (autonomous vocabulary) for making her structural reasoning explicit. Doing so, she also structurally relates four fifths, eight tenths, and sixteen twentieths in the graphical register (Table 5).

Table 5 Makbule (M) generates language for structural reasoning (with Teacher (T))

Makbule identifies the structural relations in the fraction bar board and self-initiates the phrase “to separate in the middle” to talk about refining the division in the bar of fifths and tenths from her autonomous vocabulary (Turn 151): “And for the bar of tenths it is, ehm, one has to separate it in the middle one time.” Here, she refers to the bar of fifths and describes how one has to divide each fifth into halves in order to generate tenths (see Fig. 4).

When answering Task 3 (Turn 180), she uses the phrase “separating in the middle” for generating twentieths. Instead of using the verb “to quarter,” she verbalizes the process of halving twice (dividing fifths into halves gives tenths, dividing tenths into halves gives twentieths). Makbule’s self-initiated vocabulary is rather informal (“normal big,” “to separate in the middle,” “divide small”) but captures the structural relations that constitute the process of refining.

While some students do not overcome empirical reasoning on the basis of the fraction bars’ equal length, Makbule observes the differences in the fraction bars’ structures and takes on a structural view behind the empirically found phenomenon of equal length: “They have all shot similarly, only the bars look different” (Turn 153). Moreover, she is also able to make explicit how the bar of twentieths structurally relates to the bar of fifths and tenths.

The case of Noel, Sarah, and their teacher leaving structural relations implicit

Contrary to the previous case, in the group with Noel and Sarah (both monolingual, high language proficiency), talking about structural relations within and between the fraction bars plays a less prominent role. The case has been chosen to exemplify how a teacher misses a chance to talk about structural relations explicitly.

When discussing Task 1 (see Table 6), Turns 41–44 indicate moments of structural reasoning. Sarah, when reflecting on the unequal number of attempts for the different groups, carries out a thought experiment: What if the teachers also only had five attempts like the boys? (Turn 43, “the teachers, if they had had five”). Noel concludes that in this case the teachers “would have also only missed one” (Turn 44).

Table 6 Noel (N) and Sarah (S) carrying out a thought experiment

When working on Task 2, Noel comes back to this more structural view (Turn 108). He simplifies from twentieths and tenths to fifths and draws the conclusion that in this case “they all would have another score as well.” This notion is taken up by the teacher in a rather vague way by rephrasing: “Into this bar of tenths one can also see the bar of fifths, right?” (Turn 109). At this moment, the teacher seems to aim at the structural relation between the fraction bars of fifths and tenths . However, the students are not asked to further explicate the relations between the different registers (results of the girls, results of the boys, and graphical representations in the respective fraction bars). Additionally, they do not get further lexical support from the teacher for talking about the changes in the graphical register when simplifying from tenths to fifths (e.g., vocabulary offers such as “two pieces in the bar of tenths become one piece in the bar of fifths”). In summary, in this group’s learning process, the structural relations between the graphical and verbal registers stay rather implicit.

A concluding discussion of the insights gained from comparing and contrasting the presented cases is carried out in the “Discussion” section. Before that, research question Q2 will be explored in the next section.

Quantitative comparisons of self-initiated and triggered vocabulary

In this section, research question Q2 is in focus (To what extent do students self-initiate or take up lexical means from teaching material, the teacher, or peers in local processes initiated by the presented learning opportunities?). A quantitative summary of the results of the trace analyses is given. For this summary, the transcribed small-group learning processes initiated in two different lessons of the overarching intervention build the database (n = 16 students for lesson 2, and n = 17 for lesson 1; in total 298 min of analyzed video data).

Table 7 presents the absolute numbers and average rates for the four categories “self-initiation” (1), “material as trigger” (2), “teacher as trigger” (3), and “peers as trigger” (4) across all students in the sample and for the modes “oral (o)” and “written (w)” language use (see Steps 2b and 2c of trace analysis).

Table 7 Quantitative results of trace analysis

With a chi-square test, the expected numbers of lexical means in the four categories have been calculated and compared with the inventoried (observed) numbers of lexical means (see Table 7). Testing the N0 hypothesis of “mode” and “category” not being associated provides rejection of independence (chi-squared = 18.304, df = 3, p = 0.0003807 < 0.05). The test results provide a significant relationship between the variables “mode” and “category”: the use of a lexical mean as self-initiated or being triggered by others seems to relate to the mode of use (oral or written). In consequence, the inventoried numbers of lexical means and average rates in percent can be interpreted more closely.

In both modes, the deviation of observed lexical means and expected lexical means is rather high for the categories “self-initiation” and “teacher as trigger” (see absolute values in italics in Table 7). Comparing the rates for oral and written mode, a slight decrease in self-initiation (from 55% (o) to 49% (w)) happens together with an increase of lexical means triggered by the teacher (from 11% (o) to 19% (w)). An explanation for the latter might be the structure of the tasks of the learning opportunities: Writing down one’s own reasoning in both analyzed lessons happens at the end of the analyzed learning opportunities. There might be a tendency of the students to rely more on the teacher as a lexical role model over the course of a lesson. This could explain why the number of inventoried lexical means in this category (“teacher as trigger”) and in written mode lies above the expected number of lexical means (while in oral mode the number of inventoried lexical means is below the expected number of lexical means).

For further interpretation of the category “self-initiation,” and thus for an interpretation of the students’ autonomous vocabulary, the coding procedure has to be taken into account (see the “Qualitative and quantitative methods for data analysis” section): “self-initiation” refers to lexical means that have been identified as self-initiated within the analyzed excerpts of two lessons of the overarching intervention. For both, oral and written mode, “self-initiation” is the most frequently coded category. Of course, these numbers have to be interpreted in light of the methodological restrictions. With these methodological limitations in mind, the quantitative summary shows that task- and concept-specific lexical means are retrieved from students’ autonomous vocabularies (self-initiated lexical means) and the groups’ collective vocabularies (lexical means triggered by teacher, material, peers) at almost comparable rates (roughly 50% each).

Discussion

The quantitative summary of the lexical trace analyses of students’ lexical means across two lessons shows the following patterns: while we see high rates of self-initiation for the inventoried lexical means (55% in oral, 48% in written mode), another roughly 40% (36% in oral, 41% in written mode) of the inventoried vocabulary is triggered by teachers or peers. This finding underlines the importance of micro-scaffolding on the lexical level (for the notion of micro-scaffolding, see Gibbons 2002) not only with respect to teachers but also in peer-to-peer situations. The case of Kiran in the “Specifying students’ language demands on the basis of lexical trace analyses” section exemplified how one of the students introduces vocabulary into a group’s collective vocabulary and how another student in the group adopts the vocabulary for stressing a conceptually relevant relation. Moreover, the quantitative results show that students take up lexical means from the teaching material at rather low rates in these specific learning processes being analyzed.

For exploring research question Q1, qualitative in-depth analyses have been carried out. It turned out that the students use a variety of lexical means and introduce their own meaning-related vocabulary into the group’s collective vocabulary for making structural relations explicit. This might be one of the reasons why few lexical means have been taken up from the teaching material. The cases of Yagis and Makbule showed how students with low language proficiency rely on their rather informal lexical resources from their everyday language resources (Planas and Civil 2013). Drawing further on the concept of language as a resource for the inspected learning opportunities, it is concluded that the lexical means “refine” and “coarsen” (offered in the teaching material) did not yet function as a resource and needed to be unfolded. Additionally, some of the lexical means introduced by teachers (e.g., “see a finer division in the bar” in the case of Noel’s and Sarah’s teacher) did not meet the identified conceptual and lexical demands of students. Extending the perspective from students’ learning processes to teachers’ lexical (scaffolding) moves, the empirical insights point to the topic-specific knowledge demands imposed on teachers when teaching with multiple representations. Employing appropriate language when using representations is distilled as one task in the framework by Mitchell et al. (2014) for teaching with representations. An understanding of appropriate language, however, needs topic-specific research that combines methodological approaches that allow for conceptual and linguistic insights like the previous ones and those summarized in the following.

Both, Makbule and Yagis, introduced more informal vocabulary (such as “to separate in the middle,” and “this one piece becomes two” by Makbule; “only the pieces are just bigger” by Yagis) for unfolding the meaning behind the process of refining or coarsening the inner structure in a fraction bar. Relying on the notion of structure-related lexical means from the everyday language and school academic language, we see how this vocabulary serves as a resource for developing structural understanding. The empirical analyses thus contribute to topic-specifically identifying the lexical demands of structural reasoning why two rates are equally good (consequently why two fractions are equivalent) initiated by a teaching design that draws upon activities of relating multiple registers and representations.

The in-depth analyses of the full data set further show that the intended structural reasoning for equivalence was not initiated similarly deeply in all groups. At a continuum between empirical reasoning (based on observations at the fraction bar board, e.g., no team wins because the fraction bars are all equally long) and formal reasoning (independent from the context and the graphical representation), two kinds of structural reasoning have been activated (not to be seen as hierarchical order):

  1. (a)

    Structural reasoning on the basis of the given context situation as a mental model: When you have x as many attempts you also have to score x times as many goals to be as good. From a lexical perspective, this kind of reasoning demands context-related lexical means.

  2. (b)

    Structural reasoning on the basis of the inner structure of the fraction bars as a mental model: The colored parts of the fraction bars are equal, but the division is either refined (more parts in a whole, each piece gets smaller) or coarsened (less parts in a whole, each piece gets bigger). From a lexical perspective, this kind of reasoning demands structure-related lexical means.

Different kinds of reasoning go along with certain lexical demands (rather context-related in (a), rather structure-related in (b)). The specification and structuring of the relevant lexical means revealed that in the analyzed learning opportunities (see Fig. 2), the lexical offers rather relate to empirical reasoning and structural reasoning on the basis of the given context. Lexical means for structural reasoning on the basis of the inner structure of the fraction bars as a mental model (e.g., the verbs “refine” and “coarsen” to talk about the structural relations) stay rather implicit. As a consequence, in learning processes in which structural relations in the fraction bar board have been made explicit, the relevant lexical means are student- or teacher-generated.

As a contribution for local theory building as one important aim of design research, the trace analyses and lexical inventories structured along the conceptual learning pathways lead to further refining the model of representations and registers (Fig. 5). On the verbal academic school register level, the basic meaning-related vocabulary needs differentiation into the two finer sub-categories: meaning-related vocabulary referring to contexts and meaning-related vocabulary referring to structure (in Fig. 5 as context-related and structure-related vocabulary). In contrast, the formal vocabulary is independent of context and graphical representations.

Fig. 5
figure 5

Context- and structure-related vocabulary as part of the basic meaning-related vocabulary

With respect to design principles for initiating lexical learning processes, this study shows that in the design of activities, both kinds of vocabulary (context- and structure-related) seem important for developing conceptual understanding. This is why planning content- and language-integrated lesson designs requires a careful analysis of the relevant lexical means with respect to both kinds of meaning-related vocabulary.

Limitations of the study arise due to the fact that the learning processes of only a small sample of students working on very particular learning opportunities have been analyzed. Additionally, research on language and language learning like it is carried out in this study highly depends on the language of teaching and instruction, which in this case was German. Although issues will always remain on the extent to which the findings are transferrable to other language contexts, the study provides relevant theoretical and empirical contributions to the field of language-responsive mathematics teaching and learning.