Abstract
Foraging animals must contend with fluctuating environmental variables that affect foraging success, including conditions like wind noise, which could diminish the usefulness of particular sensory modes. Although the documented impact of anthropogenic noise on animal behavior has become clear, there is limited research on natural noise and its potential influence on mammalian behavior. We investigated foraging behavior in the myrmecophagous bat-eared fox (Otocyon megalotis), a species known to rely predominantly on hearing for prey detection. For a year, we monitored the foraging behavior of 18 bat-eared foxes from a habituated population in South Africa, amidst varying wind speeds (0–15.5 km/h). In contrast to expectations, foraging rates did not generally decline with increasing wind speed, except for foraging rate outside termite patches in fall. Furthermore, wind speed had little correlation with time spent in patches. In winter, however, we observed an increase in foraging rate with increasing wind speed both within and outside patches. At the observed wind speeds, these acoustically driven insectivores continue to forage effectively despite potentially distracting or masking noises. With anthropogenic noise producing sound across a broader frequency range, it is important to examine the responses of these canids to artificial sources of acoustic disturbance as well.
Introduction
Predators rely on various sensory cues from their prey to successfully detect food items. The salience of different cues varies both with the ecology of the predator (Klinka and Reimchen 2009), and with ambient environmental conditions. Changes in factors such as light intensity, vegetation density, or noise levels affect the utility of sensory cues (Barber et al. 2010; Goerlitz et al. 2008; Klinka and Reimchen 2009). In visual hunters such as pikes (Esox lucius), for example, increasing water turbidity results in reduced reaction distance when attacking prey (Ranåker et al. 2012), while black-backed jackals (Canis mesomelas) increase hunting behavior and exploration of risky anthropogenic habitats during brighter, full moon nights (Botha et al. 2022).
Most of the current research on acoustic disturbance has focused on the effect of anthropogenic noise (Shannon et al. 2016), particularly as it affects communication, mating, or vigilance patterns (Barber et al. 2010; Quinn et al. 2006; Reijnen et al. 1997). Much less is known about the impact of natural sounds—geophonies like water and wind—on foraging behavior in acoustic hunters (Gomes et al. 2021). For many nocturnal insectivores, acoustic cues are of paramount importance in prey detection (Microcebus murinus, Goerlitz and Siemers 2007; Myotis myotis, Schaub et al. 2008, Otocyon megalotis, Renda and le Roux 2017). The auditory sense enables such hunters to locate prey not only from a distance but also in visually cluttered environments (Goerlitz and Siemers 2007). Visually inconspicuous prey may therefore still be detected through pin-pointing their location aurally, often with the aid of over-sized pinnae (Ramsier and Dominy 2012). Foragers reliant on this sense are also subject to ambient interferences: noise, both of anthropogenic and natural origin, plays a role in obstructing prey-generated sounds (Schaub et al. 2008) and has been shown to reduce the prey-detection efficiency of predators such as greater mouse-eared bats (Myotis myotis) and Daubenton’s bats (Myotis daubentonii) (Siemers and Schaub 2011; Luo, Siemers, and Koselj 2015). Schaub et al. (2008) revealed that both anthropogenic and natural ambient noise resulted in lower foraging success in greater mouse-eared bats. Interestingly, a greater deleterious effect was noted from simulated wind-generated noise than from anthropogenic noise. The disruptions brought on by noise are not necessarily due to acoustic masking (overlapping with sensory cues used to detect prey), but can sometimes be ascribed to acoustic distraction, which interferes with hunting success (Allen et al. 2021). Natural environmental noise may disrupt foraging behavior even in non-specialist foragers like California ground squirrels, Otospermophilus beecheyi (Le et al. 2019). Given the potential of geophonies to shape behavior, this is a rich area for further behavioral research and one that has received little attention in comparison to anthropogenic noise.
Bat-eared foxes (Otocyon megalotis, “foxes” hereafter) are nocturnal insectivores that feed primarily on termites, predominantly harvester termites, Hodotermes mossambicus, although a variety of other prey items are consumed as and when they become available (Jumbam et al. 2019; Kuntzsch and Nel 1992; Malcolm 1986). Whereas foxes’ habitat selection patterns do not closely mirror those of the harvester termites (Périquet and le Roux 2018), their primary prey items are invertebrates that do not produce conspicuous warning or advertising sounds (Grant and Samways 2015). These canids have been observed to use predominantly auditory stimuli when seeking prey under natural (Grant and Samways 2015; Malcolm 1986) and experimental conditions (Renda and le Roux 2017). The disproportionately small olfactory turbinals in fox skulls further underscore that they rely less on olfactory cues compared to other terrestrial canids (Green et al. 2012). Given their reliance on audition in finding acoustically inconspicuous prey (Goerlitz et al. 2008), we would therefore expect wind to have a detrimental effect on their foraging success. In this study, we recorded foraging behavior of known individual foxes along with wind speed used as a proxy for ambient noise levels (cf. Hayes and Huntly 2005). We anticipated that higher wind speeds, inducing higher levels of ambient noise, would hinder foxes’ prey detection and more specifically that:
-
The foraging rate (number of successful foraging events per unit of time) outside termite patches should decrease with increasing wind, aligned with an increase in ambient noise.
-
The foraging rate within termite patches should decrease with wind as foxes would continue feeding in patches for longer, as these are predictable sources of food, once the patch is discovered.
-
Foxes should spend more time feeding in termite patches under windy conditions due to the difficulty of detecting single prey items outside these relatively rich areas, and the effect should be more prominent in winter when overall arthropod availability drops.
Materials and methods
Study site and population
We observed habituated foxes from a wild population in the Kuruman River Reserve (28°580 S, 21°490 E), Northern Cape province, South Africa. Vegetation in the reserve consists of scattered camel thorn trees (Acacia erioloba) grading out into dry scrubland and sparsely vegetated dunes. The reserve is characterized by four seasons, based on temperature and rainfall. Winters (June to August) are usually dry and cold, with temperatures often below 0°C during the night, while summers (December to February), when most of the precipitation occurs (~250mm/year), can be extremely hot (40°C during daytime). Foxes were fully habituated to the presence of observers following them on foot at night (foxes became very wary of human observers during the day) from a few meters away (Renda and le Roux 2017). Observers could identify foxes individually thanks to natural markings, and VHF radio-collars on a few individuals (n=8). We followed known individuals only once a week, for a 2-h session between dusk and dawn, to reduce disturbance. In these “follow” sessions, we noted a wide range of prey items being eaten, including termites (Isoptera: Hodotermitidae), ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), beetles (Coleoptera: Carabidae; Tenebrionidae; Scarabaeidae), antlions (Neuroptera: Myrmeleontidae), Lepidoptera (various families), Arachnida (various families), and some mammalian vertebrates (Rodentia: Muridae), as well as occasional amphibians or reptiles. Despite their relatively flexible diets, this study population was confirmed termite specialists (Jumbam et al. 2019).
Behavioral observations and foraging parameters
We conducted our observations of 18 adult foxes (8 females and 10 males) between July 2014 and April 2016. While observer presence could have had a potential effect on prey behavior (e.g., they might fly away or cease movement), this effect is unlikely to cause any patterns in our results since it would have been present during all our observations. The same remark is true concerning the potential effect of observer on fox behavior and noise resulting from observer movement (though noise from observers was minimized insofar as possible). Ten minutes after finding a fox, we would start data recording, using the program Cybertracker (www.cybertracker.org) loaded on an Android tablet (Samsung Galaxy). We noted the date, time, and GPS coordinates of each successful foraging instance, including a description of the item(s) eaten, when clearly visible to the observer.
When foxes foraged on termites, it was not possible to count the exact number of termites eaten. In this case, we characterized successful foraging events using “productive steps.” We defined a “productive step” as a step (displacement of either front limb) during which at least one termite was eaten. We further defined termite “foraging bouts” as at least 15 consecutive productive steps (typically with multiple termites eaten per step), grouping bouts within 20 meters and 5 minutes of one another into larger, contiguous “patches.” This approach allowed us to conservatively quantify termite-feeding bouts, which were periods of focused movement within a relatively small space as termites close to a nest entrance were snapped up. These bouts were different from pouncing, scratching, or coursing movement when foxes hunted other prey or moved between patches. For analysis, we treated individual termite patches as discrete foraging events, as we presumed foxes to be less reliant on audition when foraging on termites in this manner—termites were typically active in dense concentrations on the surface. Across all observation sessions (hereafter referred to as “follows”), we identified 413 termite patches. We computed foraging rate inside each termite patch using the number of productive steps within a given patch divided by the time spent in this patch, and calculated total time spent within termite patches for each follow.
Whereas we measured foraging rate for every single patch used, foraging rate outside patches was calculated per hour of observation time. Outside termite patches, each foraging event corresponded to the consumption of a discrete prey item and we thus calculated the daily foraging rate outside termite patches as the total number of foraging events (i.e. number of items eaten) outside patches, divided by total time spent outside patches. For this measure, we pooled all prey items together. The datasets analyzed during this study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.
Meteorological data
We collected meteorological data from an on-site weather station at the Kuruman River Reserve field station, roughly at the center of our study area. The station recorded hourly wind speeds at a height of 2 m above-ground, and temperature throughout the year. The maximum recorded wind speeds were 15.5 km/h (classified as a Beaufort value of 3 or “gentle breeze”) for all observation sessions. According to the Beaufort scale, wind speeds of 6 km/h upwards would start generating noise through light leaf rustling, up to more pronounced noise produced by moving branches and grasses (World Meteorological Organization 2016). Average wind speed across all behavioral observations was 3.8 km/h (SD= 3.8). Considering the relative homogeneity of the landscape at the study site, we assumed that wind speed would be an effective proxy for noise level, similar to the strong correlation between wind speed and noise spectrum (from 500 Hz-8,000Hz) in shallow water (Murugan, Natarajan, and Kumar 2011). As temperature is a factor known to affect arthropod activity (Heatwole 2012), and we did not want to add temperature as a possible confounding variable, we cut all data corresponding to temperatures below 10 °C from the dataset, based on the thermal limits of foraging Hodotermes workers (Mitchell et al. 1993).
Statistical analyses
We performed all data analyses in the R statistical environment (version 3.5.1, R Core Team 2016). We assessed the effect of the interaction between wind and season on foraging rate outside and inside termite patches using Linear Mixed Effects Models (LMMs), with each follow’s identity (i.e., date and time stamp) nested in individual as a random intercept. To meet model assumptions, we log-transformed foraging rates outside and inside patches. We ran LMMs using the “nlme” package (version 3.1-137, Pinheiro et al., 2007).
We used Generalized Linear Mixed Effects Models (GLMMs) to study the effect of the interaction between wind and season on the time spent in patches (per hour of observation/follow), using follow duration (log-transformed) as an offset and follow identity nested within individual as a random intercept. As models using a Poisson error distribution showed high overdispersion, we used a negative binomial error distribution for the final models. We fit GLMMs with the package “glmmADMB” (Fournier et al. 2012; Skaug et al. 2016). Following Zuur and Ieno (2016), we verified model assumptions by plotting residuals versus fitted values, versus each covariate in the model and versus each covariate not in the model. We compared models to a null model using likelihood ratio tests (LRTs) to assess model fit.
Results
Foraging rate outside termite patches
We extracted foraging rates outside termite patches for a total of 804 focal hours. A model including the wind-season interaction performed better than the null model (LRT: χ2= 39.1, P <0.001). The interaction wind-season was significant (χ2 = 23.4, P <0.001) with wind having a significant positive effect on foraging rate in winter (P <0.001, Table 1) and negative in all other seasons (Table 1, Fig. 1).
Foraging rate inside termite patches
We extracted foraging rates inside termite patches for a total of 813 focal hours. The model including the wind-season interaction was significantly better than the null one (LRT: χ2 = 14.7, P =0.04). Only season had a significant effect (χ2 = 9.6, P = 0.02, Fig. 2) on foraging rate. Foraging rate in termite patches in fall and spring was significantly higher than in winter (P = 0.03, Table 1, Fig. 2).
Time spent in termite patches
We recorded a total of 204 bouts of foraging in termite patches. The model including the wind-season interaction was not significantly better than the null model (LRT: χ2 = 7.0, P = 0.43), suggesting that wind had no effect on the time foxes spent foraging on termites in patches.
Discussion
We used wind speed as a proxy for ambient noise and expected that stronger winds would negatively affect bat-eared foxes’ foraging behavior due to their reliance on sound to detect prey. Contrary to our predictions, wind did not have a deleterious effect on foraging rates, and in fact correlated with increased foraging rates in winter. Although wind did have a significant negative effect on foraging outside of termite patches in all seasons compare to winter, this effect was very small (see Fig. 1) and barely negative, and thus may not be biologically relevant in terms of the actual intake of a foraging fox. Interestingly, in winter, foraging rate increased significantly with wind speed, both within and outside patches. This effect may have been mediated by a shift in fox ecology peculiar to the winter season, as foxes’ activity patterns closely mirror that of their termite prey, similar to other myrmecophagous mammals (e.g., Abba and Cassini 2010). In southern Africa, foxes often exhibit a more diurnal foraging pattern in winter (Lourens and Nel 1990; Nel 1990), linked to a diurnal shift in harvester termite activities (Nel 1990). Additionally, desert day-time wind speeds are typically higher in winter, and foxes have been noted to decrease diurnal foraging as winds increase, with activity often ceasing altogether at high speeds (Lourens and Nel 1990). Windy days during the current study followed this trend, with winter wind speeds typically higher earlier in the day and tapering off by evening (see Supplementary Information). It is likely, therefore, that foxes in the present study missed diurnal foraging opportunities more frequently during windy winter days. With winter being a relatively food-constrained season (Jumbam et al. 2019), foxes presumably experienced much higher motivation to forage more effectively during nights following windy days.
In our third prediction, foxes were expected to spend more time in termite patches under windy conditions as wind noise would increase the difficulty of finding alternate patches or food items. Again, the amount of time foxes spent in termite patches did not increase, and there appeared to be no greater value to remaining in patches as wind speeds rose. Although information is limited, sensory information is known to influence patch use in both invertebrates and vertebrates. For wolf spiders (Schizocosa ocreata), patches containing clear sensory signals indicating the presence of prey elicited longer residence times than control patches (Persons and Uetz 1996). Similarly, common brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula) spent more time foraging in patches where olfactory cues indicate richer food sources being available (Mella et al. 2018). However, up to the moderate wind speeds recorded in this study, foxes did not alter their patch residence times. This may align with Mankin and Benshemesh’s (2006) findings that subterranean acoustic cues from disturbed ant and termite nests remain detectible (by geophone) at close range even amidst wind gusts. As foxes are sensitive to ambient light conditions (Welch et al. 2017), and substrate-borne vibrational cues may also be detectible through paws rather than pinnae (Mason and Wenger 2019), we also cannot eliminate the possibility that foxes increase reliance on other sensory modalities during windy nights.
It could be argued that wind alters prey behavior and that the concomitant decline in patch profitability would result in foxes abandoning patches earlier despite out-of-patch sensory constraints. However, though the impact of wind on Hodotermes worker activity specifically has not been quantified, studies on harvester termites with similar foraging ecology have revealed little effect of wind on activity levels. Workers of Trinervitermes, for example, were found to continue foraging at wind speeds in excess of 21.6 km/h (Adam et al. 2008). Similarly, wind was found to have little effect on the foraging behavior of Baucaliotermes workers (Geerts et al. 2016). Comparable results have been found for other common arthropod prey items of foxes including ants (Hymenoptera:Formicidae) and beetles (Coleoptera:Tenebrionidae, Briese and Macauley 1980; Curtis, 1985; Heatwole 2012). Wind may even have a stimulatory effect on some scorpions (Scorpiones: Buthidae), beetles (Coleoptera: Tenebrionidae), and antlions (Neuroptera: Myrmeleontidae), particularly at lower speeds i.e., < 9 km/h for antlions (Heatwole 2012; Skutelsky 1996; Szentkirályi et al. 2005). Where inhibition of arthropod activity does occur, this is often at higher levels (upwards of 14.4 km/h for ants; Heatwole 2012), very close to the maximum of 15.5 km/h recorded in the current study. We are aware that the range of wind speeds under consideration here is not very wide, but as foraging observations were conducted on a daily basis throughout the year, we believe these wind speeds are representative of the overall conditions in which foxes forage in the study area.
It may appear counterintuitive that the naturally occurring geophony of wind did not affect the foraging behavior of an acoustic hunter like the bat-eared fox. This contrasts with the known and significant impacts of white noise and anthropogenic sounds (technophonies) on other mammals’ foraging behavior (Gomes et al.,2021; Shannon et al. 2016). It may be that we did not measure the biologically salient responses of foxes to wind noise. Similar to foraging bats (Schaub et al. 2008), foxes may have moved to microhabitats with less pronounced acoustic disturbance. We did not measure actual noise levels in different habitat and wind conditions so we cannot discuss this possibility. Foxes may also simply stop foraging once noise levels increase beyond a certain threshold: on a few occasions of very strong wind, we noticed that foxes stopped foraging and rested, or took flight in the presence of observers, suggesting some compromise of their auditory awareness. A final consideration, however, is that bat-eared foxes are well-adapted to the natural disturbance of even moderate winds. As anthropogenic noise often disrupts natural soundscapes across a range of frequencies that differs from environmental and biological sources of sound (Farina 2019; Gomes et al. 2021), this acoustic adaptation of bat-eared foxes—and those of other acoustic hunters—may be severely tested with continuing global change.
Change history
20 May 2023
A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13364-023-00695-1
References
Abba AM, Cassini MH (2010) Ecological differences between two sympatric species of armadillos (Xenarthra, Mammalia) in a temperate region of Argentina. Acta Theriol 55(1):35–44. https://doi.org/10.4098/J.AT.0001-7051.083.2008
Adam RA, Mitchell JD, van der Westhuizen MC (2008) Aspects of foraging in the harvester termite, Trinervitermes trinervoides (Sjöstedt) (Termitidae: Nasutitermitinae). Afr Entomol 16(2):153–161. https://doi.org/10.10520/EJC32787
Allen LC, Hristov NI, Rubin JJ, Lightsey JT, Barber JR (2021) Noise distracts foraging bats. Proc Royal Soc B 288:20202689. https://doi.org/10.1098/RSPB.2020.2689
Barber JR, Crooks KR, Fristrup KM (2010) The costs of chronic noise exposure for terrestrial organisms. Trends Ecol Evol 25(3):180–189. https://doi.org/10.1016/J.TREE.2009.08.002
Botha AE, Bruns AC, le Roux A (2022) The spatial ecology of black-backed jackals (Canis mesomelas) in a protected mountainous grassland area. Afr Zool 57(1):43–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2022.2057818
Briese DT, Macauley BJ (1980) Temporal structure of an ant community in semi-arid Australia. Aust J Ecol 5(2):121–134. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1442-9993.1980.TB01236.X
Curtis BA (1985) Activity of the Namib Desert dune ant. Camponotus detritus. Afr Zool 20(2):41–48 https://www.ajol.info/index.php/az/article/view/152892
Farina A (2019) Ecoacoustics: a quantitative approach to investigate the ecological role of environmental sounds. Mathematics 7(1):21. https://doi.org/10.3390/MATH7010021
Fournier DA, Skaug HJ, Ancheta J, Ianelli J, Magnusson A, Maunder MN, Nielsen A, Sibert J (2012) AD Model Builder: using automatic differentiation for statistical inference of highly parameterized complex nonlinear models. Optim Methods Softw 27(2):233–249. https://doi.org/10.1080/10556788.2011.597854
Geerts S, van der Linden J, van der Linden E (2016) The ecology and foraging behaviour of the harvester termite, Baucaliotermes hainesi in semi-arid grasslands in the northwestern interior of South Africa. Insectes Soc 63(3):457–465. https://doi.org/10.1007/S00040-016-0488-9
Goerlitz HR, Siemers BM (2007) Sensory ecology of prey rustling sounds: acoustical features and their classification by wild grey mouse lemurs. Func. Ecol 21(1):143–153. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2435.2006.01212.x
Goerlitz HR, Greif S, Siemers BM (2008) Cues for acoustic detection of prey: insect rustling sounds and the influence of walking substrate. J Exp Biol 211(17):2799–2806. https://doi.org/10.1242/JEB.019596
Gomes DGE, Francis CD, Barber JR (2021) Using the past to understand the present: coping with natural and anthropogenic noise. BioScience 71(3):223–234. https://doi.org/10.1093/BIOSCI/BIAA161
Grant PBC, Samways MJ (2015) Acoustic prey and a listening predator: interaction between calling katydids and the bat-eared fox. Bioacoustics 24(1):49–61. https://doi.org/10.1080/09524622.2014.927789
Green PA, van Valkenburgh B, Pang B, Bird D, Rowe T, Curtis A (2012) Respiratory and olfactory turbinal size in canid and arctoid carnivorans. J Anat 221(6):609–621. https://doi.org/10.1111/J.1469-7580.2012.01570.X
Hayes AR, Huntly NJ (2005) Effects of wind on the behavior and call transmission of pikas (Ochotona princeps). J Mammal 86(5):974–981. https://doi.org/10.1644/1545-1542(2005)86[974:EOWOTB]2.0.CO;2
Heatwole H (2012) Energetics of desert invertebrates. Springer Science & Business Media
Jumbam KR, Périquet S, Dalerum F, le Roux A (2019) Spatial and temporal variation in the use of supplementary food in an obligate termite specialist, the bat-eared fox. Afr Zool 54(1):63–71. https://doi.org/10.1080/15627020.2019.1596754
Klinka DR, Reimchen TE (2009) Darkness, twilight, and daylight foraging success of bears (Ursus americanus) on salmon in coastal British Columbia. J Mammal 90(1):144–149. https://doi.org/10.1644/07-MAMM-A-200.1
Kuntzsch V, Nel JAJ (1992) Diet of bat-eared foxes Otocyon megalotis in the Karoo. Koedoe 35(2):a403. https://doi.org/10.4102/koedoe.v35i2.403
Le MLT, Garvin CM, Barber JR, Francis CD (2019) Natural sounds alter California ground squirrel, Otospermophilus beecheyi, foraging, vigilance and movement behaviours. Anim Behav 157:51–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2019.08.014
Lourens S, Nel JAJ (1990) Winter activity of bat-eared foxes Otocyon megalotis on the Cape West coast. Afr Zool 25(2):124–132. https://doi.org/10.1080/02541858.1990.11448200
Luo J, Siemers BM, Koselj K (2015) How anthropogenic noise affects foraging. Glob Chang Biol 21(9):3278–3289. https://doi.org/10.1111/GCB.12997
Malcolm JR (1986) Socio-ecology of bat-eared foxes (Otocyon megalotis). J Zool 208(3):457–469. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7998.1986.tb01907.x
Mankin RW, Benshemesh J (2006) Geophone detection of subterranean termite and ant activity. J Econ Entomol 99(1):244–250. https://doi.org/10.1093/JEE/99.1.244
Mason MJ, Wenger LMD (2019) Mechanisms of vibration detection in mammals. In: Hill PSM, Lakes-Harlan R, Mazzoni V, Narins PM, Virant-Doberlet M, Wessel A (eds) Biotremology: Studying vibrational behavior. Springer, Switzerland, pp 177–208. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-030-22293-2_10
Mella VSA, Possell M, Troxell-Smith SM, McArthur C (2018) Visit, consume and quit: Patch quality affects the three stages of foraging. J Anim Ecol 87(6):1615–1626. https://doi.org/10.1111/1365-2656.12882
Mitchell JD, Hewitt PH, Van Der Linde TD (1993) Critical thermal limits and temperature tolerance in the harvester termite Hodotermes mossambicus (Hagen). J Insect Physiol 39(6):523–528. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1910(93)90085-6
Murugan SS, Natarajan V, Kumar RR (2011) Noise model analysis and estimation of effect due to wind driven ambient noise in shallow water. Int J Oceanogr 2011:1–4. https://doi.org/10.1155/2011/950838
Nel JAJ (1990) Foraging and feeding by bat-eared foxes Otocyon megalotis in the southwestern Kalahari. Koedoe 33(2):a436. https://doi.org/10.4102/koedoe.v33i2.436
Périquet S, le Roux A (2018) Seasonal patterns of habitat selection in the insectivorous bat-eared fox. Afr J Ecol 56(3):548–554. https://doi.org/10.1111/aje.12492
Persons MH, Uetz GW (1996) The influence of sensory information on patch residence time in wolf spiders (Araneae: Lycosidae). Anim Behav 51(6): 1285–1293. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0133
Pinheiro J, Debroy S, Sarkar D, Bates D, R Core Team (2007) nlme: linear and nonlinear mixed effects models. R Package Version 3(57):1–89
Quinn JL, Whittingham MJ, Butler SJ, Cresswell W (2006) Noise, predation risk compensation and vigilance in the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs. J Avian Biol 37(6):601–608. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2006.0908-8857.03781.x
R Core Team (2016) R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing
Ramsier MA, Dominy NJ (2012) Receiver bias and the acoustic ecology of aye-ayes (Daubentonia madagascariensis). Commun Integr Biol 5(6):637–640. https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.21509
Ranåker L, Jönsson M, Nilsson PA, Brönmark C (2012) Effects of brown and turbid water on piscivore-prey fish interactions along a visibility gradient. Freshw Biol 57(9):1761–1768. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2427.2012.02836.x
Reijnen R, Foppen R, Veenbaas G (1997) Disturbance by traffic of breeding birds: evaluation of the effect and considerations in planning and managing road corridors. Biodivers Conserv 6(4):567–581. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1018385312751
Renda S, le Roux A (2017) The sensory ecology of prey detection in the bat-eared fox (Otocyon megalotis). Behaviour 154:227–240
Schaub A, Ostwald J, Siemers BM (2008) Foraging bats avoid noise. J Exper Biol 211(19):3174–3180. https://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.022863
Shannon G, McKenna MF, Angeloni LM, Crooks KR, Fristrup KM, Brown E, Warner KA, Nelson MD, White C, Briggs J, McFarland S, Wittemyer G (2016) A synthesis of two decades of research documenting the effects of noise on wildlife. Biol Rev 91(4):982–1005. https://doi.org/10.1111/BRV.12207
Siemers BM, Schaub A (2011) Hunting at the highway: traffic noise reduces foraging efficiency in acoustic predators. Proc Royal Soc B 278(1712):1646–1652. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2262
Skaug H, Fournier D, Bolker B, Magnusson A, Nielsen A (2016) Generalized linear mixed models using AD model builder. R Package Version 0.8 3(3):1–21
Skutelsky O (1996) Predation risk and state-dependent foraging in scorpions: effects of moonlight on foraging in the scorpion Buthus occitanus. Anim Behav 52(1):49–57. https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1996.0151
Szentkirályi F, Kazinczy L, Kádár F, Bernáth B, Barabás S (2005) Monitoring of antlions (Neuroptera Myrmeleontidae) by light trapping: influence of weather elements on daily and seasonal flight patterns. Ann Mus Civ St Nat Ferrar 8:167–172
Welch RJ, Périquet S, Petelle MB, le Roux A (2017) Hunter or hunted? Perceptions of risk and reward in a small mesopredator. J Mammal 98(6):1531–1537. https://doi.org/10.1093/jmammal/gyx100
World Meteorological Organization (2016) WMO Manual on Codes No. 306 part A, Alphanumeric codes, Section E. Online: http://www.wmo.int/pages/prog/www/WMOCodes/WMO306_vI1/Publications/2016update/Sel10.pdf Accessed: 31/01/2017
Zuur AF, Ieno EN (2016) A protocol for conducting and presenting results of regression-type analyses. Methods Ecol Evol 7(6):636–645. https://doi.org/10.1111/2041-210X.12577
Acknowledgements
We thank the Kalahari Research Trust, Kuruman River Reserve and Kalahari Meerkat Project (supported by ERC Grant No. 294494 to TH Clutton-Brock since 1 July 2012) for logistical support and access to the reserve.
Funding
Open access funding provided by University of the Free State. This research was funded by South African National Research Foundation’s Thuthuka Grant (TTK1206041007).
Author information
Authors and Affiliations
Corresponding author
Ethics declarations
Ethical approval
We received ethical clearance from the University of the Free State’s Animal Ethics Committee (protocol #1/2013) for this research project and complied with national regulations by obtaining a fieldwork permit from the Northern Cape Province’s Department of Nature Conservation, under permit # 476/2/2013.
Additional information
Communicated by: Marietjie Landman
Publisher’s note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
The original online version of this article was revised due to corrected figure 2.
Supplementary information
Rights and permissions
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
About this article
Cite this article
Renda, S., Périquet, S. & le Roux, A. Blown away? Wind speed and foraging success in an acoustic predator. Mamm Res 68, 215–221 (2023). https://doi.org/10.1007/s13364-023-00673-7
Received:
Accepted:
Published:
Issue Date:
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1007/s13364-023-00673-7
Keywords
- Acoustic
- Bat-eared fox
- Foraging
- Hodotermes mossambicus
- Otocyon megalotis
- Prey detection
- Sensory ecology